首页 > 最新文献

Psychophysiology最新文献

英文 中文
Optimal filters for ERP research II: Recommended settings for seven common ERP components. 企业资源规划研究的最佳过滤器 II:企业资源规划七个常见组成部分的建议设置。
Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-28 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14530
Guanghui Zhang, David R Garrett, Steven J Luck

In research with event-related potentials (ERPs), aggressive filters can substantially improve the signal-to-noise ratio and maximize statistical power, but they can also produce significant waveform distortion. Although this tradeoff has been well documented, the field lacks recommendations for filter cutoffs that quantitatively address both of these competing considerations. To fill this gap, we quantified the effects of a broad range of low-pass filter and high-pass filter cutoffs for seven common ERP components (P3b, N400, N170, N2pc, mismatch negativity, error-related negativity, and lateralized readiness potential) recorded from a set of neurotypical young adults. We also examined four common scoring methods (mean amplitude, peak amplitude, peak latency, and 50% area latency). For each combination of component and scoring methods, we quantified the effects of filtering on data quality (noise level and signal-to-noise ratio) and waveform distortion. This led to recommendations for optimal low-pass and high-pass filter cutoffs. We repeated the analyses after adding artificial noise to provide recommendations for data sets with moderately greater noise levels. For researchers who are analyzing data with similar ERP components, noise levels, and participant populations, using the recommended filter settings should lead to improved data quality and statistical power without creating problematic waveform distortion.

在事件相关电位(ERPs)研究中,激进的滤波器可以大幅提高信噪比并最大限度地提高统计功率,但同时也会产生明显的波形失真。尽管这种权衡已被充分记录下来,但该领域仍缺乏对滤波器截止值的建议,以定量解决这两个相互竞争的考虑因素。为了填补这一空白,我们对一组神经畸形青壮年记录的七个常见 ERP 成分(P3b、N400、N170、N2pc、错配负性、错误相关负性和侧化准备电位)进行了定量分析,结果显示了多种低通滤波器和高通滤波器截止点的影响。我们还研究了四种常见的评分方法(平均振幅、峰值振幅、峰值潜伏期和 50% 区域潜伏期)。对于每种成分和评分方法的组合,我们都量化了过滤对数据质量(噪声水平和信噪比)和波形失真的影响。由此,我们提出了最佳低通和高通滤波器截止点的建议。在加入人工噪音后,我们重复进行了分析,为噪音水平中等偏上的数据集提供了建议。对于正在分析具有类似 ERP 成分、噪声水平和参与者群体的数据的研究人员来说,使用推荐的滤波器设置应能提高数据质量和统计能力,而不会造成波形失真问题。
{"title":"Optimal filters for ERP research II: Recommended settings for seven common ERP components.","authors":"Guanghui Zhang, David R Garrett, Steven J Luck","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14530","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14530","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In research with event-related potentials (ERPs), aggressive filters can substantially improve the signal-to-noise ratio and maximize statistical power, but they can also produce significant waveform distortion. Although this tradeoff has been well documented, the field lacks recommendations for filter cutoffs that quantitatively address both of these competing considerations. To fill this gap, we quantified the effects of a broad range of low-pass filter and high-pass filter cutoffs for seven common ERP components (P3b, N400, N170, N2pc, mismatch negativity, error-related negativity, and lateralized readiness potential) recorded from a set of neurotypical young adults. We also examined four common scoring methods (mean amplitude, peak amplitude, peak latency, and 50% area latency). For each combination of component and scoring methods, we quantified the effects of filtering on data quality (noise level and signal-to-noise ratio) and waveform distortion. This led to recommendations for optimal low-pass and high-pass filter cutoffs. We repeated the analyses after adding artificial noise to provide recommendations for data sets with moderately greater noise levels. For researchers who are analyzing data with similar ERP components, noise levels, and participant populations, using the recommended filter settings should lead to improved data quality and statistical power without creating problematic waveform distortion.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11096077/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139572472","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reduced reward responsiveness and depression vulnerability: Consideration of social contexts and implications for intervention. 奖赏反应性降低与抑郁易感性:考虑社会环境和干预的影响。
Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-23 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14528
Autumn Kujawa

Depression is a prevalent, heterogeneous, and debilitating disorder that often emerges in adolescence, and there is a need to better understand vulnerability processes to inform more targeted intervention efforts. Psychophysiological methods, like event-related potentials (ERPs), can offer unique insights into the cognitive and emotional processes underlying depression vulnerability. I review my and others' research examining ERP measures of reward responsiveness in youth depression and present a conceptual model of the development of low reward responsiveness, its role in depression vulnerability, and potential windows for targeted intervention. There is evidence that a blunted reward positivity (RewP) is observable in children at risk for depression, appears to be shaped in part by early social experiences, and predicts later depressive symptoms in combination with other risk factors like stress exposure. Further, a component consistent with RewP is reliably elicited in response to social acceptance feedback in computerized peer interaction tasks and demonstrates unique associations with social contextual factors and depressive symptoms, supporting the utility of developing psychophysiological tasks that may better capture youths' real-world experiences and social risk processes. In addition, I address the translational implications of clinical psychophysiological research and describe a series of studies showing that a reduced RewP predicts greater reductions in depressive symptoms with treatment but is not modifiable by current treatments like cognitive behavioral therapy. Finally, I describe our preliminary efforts to develop a positive emotion-focused intervention for the offspring of depressed mothers, informed by the RewP literature, and describe future directions for translating psychophysiological research to intervention and prevention.

抑郁症是一种普遍、异质性和使人衰弱的疾病,通常在青少年时期出现,因此有必要更好地了解抑郁症的易感性过程,以提供更有针对性的干预措施。心理生理学方法,如事件相关电位(ERPs),可以为了解抑郁症脆弱性的认知和情感过程提供独特的见解。我将回顾自己和其他人对青少年抑郁症奖赏反应性的ERP测量方法的研究,并提出一个关于低奖赏反应性的发展、其在抑郁症易感性中的作用以及针对性干预的潜在窗口的概念模型。有证据表明,在有抑郁风险的儿童身上可以观察到奖赏积极性(RewP)减弱的现象,这种现象似乎部分是由早期社会经历形成的,并与其他风险因素(如压力暴露)相结合,预示着日后抑郁症状的出现。此外,在计算机化的同伴互动任务中,对社会接纳反馈的反应会可靠地激发出与 RewP 一致的成分,并显示出与社会环境因素和抑郁症状的独特关联,这支持了开发心理生理学任务的实用性,该任务可以更好地捕捉青少年的真实世界经历和社会风险过程。此外,我还探讨了临床心理生理学研究的转化意义,并介绍了一系列研究,这些研究表明,RewP 的降低预示着抑郁症状在治疗后会有更大程度的缓解,但目前的治疗方法(如认知行为疗法)却无法改变这一现象。最后,我介绍了我们根据RewP文献,为抑郁母亲的后代开发以积极情绪为重点的干预措施的初步工作,并描述了将心理生理学研究转化为干预和预防的未来方向。
{"title":"Reduced reward responsiveness and depression vulnerability: Consideration of social contexts and implications for intervention.","authors":"Autumn Kujawa","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14528","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14528","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Depression is a prevalent, heterogeneous, and debilitating disorder that often emerges in adolescence, and there is a need to better understand vulnerability processes to inform more targeted intervention efforts. Psychophysiological methods, like event-related potentials (ERPs), can offer unique insights into the cognitive and emotional processes underlying depression vulnerability. I review my and others' research examining ERP measures of reward responsiveness in youth depression and present a conceptual model of the development of low reward responsiveness, its role in depression vulnerability, and potential windows for targeted intervention. There is evidence that a blunted reward positivity (RewP) is observable in children at risk for depression, appears to be shaped in part by early social experiences, and predicts later depressive symptoms in combination with other risk factors like stress exposure. Further, a component consistent with RewP is reliably elicited in response to social acceptance feedback in computerized peer interaction tasks and demonstrates unique associations with social contextual factors and depressive symptoms, supporting the utility of developing psychophysiological tasks that may better capture youths' real-world experiences and social risk processes. In addition, I address the translational implications of clinical psychophysiological research and describe a series of studies showing that a reduced RewP predicts greater reductions in depressive symptoms with treatment but is not modifiable by current treatments like cognitive behavioral therapy. Finally, I describe our preliminary efforts to develop a positive emotion-focused intervention for the offspring of depressed mothers, informed by the RewP literature, and describe future directions for translating psychophysiological research to intervention and prevention.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11096075/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139543874","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Generalization of savoring to novel positive stimuli. 将品味推广到新的积极刺激。
Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-09 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14537
Kayla A Wilson, Annmarie MacNamara

Savoring is a positive emotion up-regulation technique that can increase electrocortical and self-reported valence and arousal to positive and neutral pictures, with effects persisting to increase response to the same stimuli when encountered later. Outside of the lab, emotion regulation techniques that persist to affect not just encounters with the same stimuli but also encounters with similar, but previously unencountered stimuli should save individuals time and effort. Here, we used event-related potentials and picture ratings to test whether savoring would generalize to similar, but previously unseen positive pictures. To this end, 89 participants (56 female; M age = 18.96 years, SD = 1.87) were asked to savor positive pictures from one category (e.g., happy people) and to view positive pictures from another category (e.g., cute animals), as well as to view neutral pictures (e.g., plants). In a subsequent passive picture viewing task, participants viewed novel pictures from all three categories (i.e., happy people, cute animals, plants). In the first task, savoring was effective for pictures of animals throughout picture presentation, but only for pictures of people during the later part of picture presentation. In the second task, savoring generalized to novel pictures of animals, though this was only evident in the early portion of picture processing (and for self-reported ratings). Therefore, savoring holds promise as a useful technique for increasing positive emotion in everyday life, though more work is needed to understand whether effects may vary depending on different types of picture content.

细细品味是一种积极情绪上调技术,它能提高大脑皮层和自我报告对积极和中性图片的情绪和唤醒程度,其效果会持续到以后遇到相同刺激时增加反应。在实验室外,情绪调节技术不仅能持续影响遇到相同刺激时的反应,还能影响遇到类似但之前未遇到过的刺激时的反应,这应该能节省个人的时间和精力。在这里,我们使用事件相关电位和图片评分来测试回味是否会泛化到相似但之前未见过的积极图片上。为此,我们要求 89 名参与者(56 名女性;中位年龄 = 18.96 岁,平均年龄 = 1.87 岁)品尝一类正面图片(如快乐的人),并观看另一类正面图片(如可爱的动物)以及中性图片(如植物)。在随后的被动图片观看任务中,受试者观看所有三个类别(即快乐的人、可爱的动物、植物)的新颖图片。在第一项任务中,在整个图片展示过程中,品味动物图片是有效的,但只有在图片展示的后半段,品味人物图片才是有效的。在第二项任务中,品味对新奇的动物图片有普遍意义,但这只在图片处理的早期阶段(以及自我报告的评分中)表现明显。因此,回味有望成为日常生活中增加积极情绪的一种有用技术,不过还需要更多的工作来了解不同类型的图片内容是否会产生不同的效果。
{"title":"Generalization of savoring to novel positive stimuli.","authors":"Kayla A Wilson, Annmarie MacNamara","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14537","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14537","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Savoring is a positive emotion up-regulation technique that can increase electrocortical and self-reported valence and arousal to positive and neutral pictures, with effects persisting to increase response to the same stimuli when encountered later. Outside of the lab, emotion regulation techniques that persist to affect not just encounters with the same stimuli but also encounters with similar, but previously unencountered stimuli should save individuals time and effort. Here, we used event-related potentials and picture ratings to test whether savoring would generalize to similar, but previously unseen positive pictures. To this end, 89 participants (56 female; M age = 18.96 years, SD = 1.87) were asked to savor positive pictures from one category (e.g., happy people) and to view positive pictures from another category (e.g., cute animals), as well as to view neutral pictures (e.g., plants). In a subsequent passive picture viewing task, participants viewed novel pictures from all three categories (i.e., happy people, cute animals, plants). In the first task, savoring was effective for pictures of animals throughout picture presentation, but only for pictures of people during the later part of picture presentation. In the second task, savoring generalized to novel pictures of animals, though this was only evident in the early portion of picture processing (and for self-reported ratings). Therefore, savoring holds promise as a useful technique for increasing positive emotion in everyday life, though more work is needed to understand whether effects may vary depending on different types of picture content.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11096026/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139708912","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Disentangling associations between impulsivity, compulsivity, and performance monitoring. 消除冲动、强迫和表现监测之间的关联。
Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-09 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14539
Rebecca Overmeyer, Tanja Endrass

Disorders marked by high levels of impulsivity and compulsivity have been linked to changes in performance monitoring, specifically the error-related negativity (ERN). We investigated the relationship between performance monitoring and individual differences in impulsivity and compulsivity. A total of 142 participants were recruited into four groups, each with different combinations of impulsivity and compulsivity, and they performed a flanker task to assess error-related brain activity. We defined error-related brain activity as ERN amplitude and theta power. Single-trial regression was employed to analyze the amplitude differences between incorrect and correct trials within the ERN time window. The findings revealed that impulsivity, compulsivity, and different measures of response processing exhibited distinct interactions, which were influenced by the configuration of impulsivity and compulsivity, but also depended on the measure of response processing. Specifically, high compulsivity predicted larger ERN amplitudes in individuals with low impulsivity, whereas high impulsivity had no significant effect on ERN amplitude in individuals with low compulsivity. Furthermore, when both impulsivity and compulsivity were high, no significant increase in ERN amplitude was observed; instead, there was a reduced difference between incorrect and correct trials. No significant differences were found for theta power. While the association between error-related brain activity and transdiagnostic markers or psychopathology may be smaller than generally assumed, considering the interaction between different transdiagnostic markers and their facets can enhance our understanding of the complex associations that arise during the investigation of neural correlates of performance monitoring, specifically the ERN.

以高度冲动和强迫为特征的失调症与表现监测的变化有关,特别是与错误有关的负性(ERN)。我们研究了表现监测与冲动和强迫的个体差异之间的关系。我们共招募了 142 名参与者,分成四组,每组都有不同的冲动和强迫组合,他们进行了侧翼任务,以评估与错误相关的大脑活动。我们将错误相关脑活动定义为ERN振幅和θ功率。我们采用单次试验回归分析了ERN时间窗内错误试验和正确试验之间的振幅差异。研究结果显示,冲动性、强迫性和反应处理的不同测量方法表现出不同的交互作用,这些交互作用受冲动性和强迫性配置的影响,但也取决于反应处理的测量方法。具体来说,高强迫性预示着低强迫性个体的ERN振幅更大,而高强迫性对低强迫性个体的ERN振幅没有显著影响。此外,当冲动性和强迫性都很高时,ERN 振幅没有明显增加;相反,错误试验和正确试验之间的差异缩小了。θ功率没有发现明显差异。虽然错误相关的大脑活动与跨诊断标记或精神病理学之间的关联可能比一般假设的要小,但考虑不同跨诊断标记及其方面之间的相互作用,可以加深我们对在研究表现监测(特别是ERN)的神经相关性时所产生的复杂关联的理解。
{"title":"Disentangling associations between impulsivity, compulsivity, and performance monitoring.","authors":"Rebecca Overmeyer, Tanja Endrass","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14539","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14539","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Disorders marked by high levels of impulsivity and compulsivity have been linked to changes in performance monitoring, specifically the error-related negativity (ERN). We investigated the relationship between performance monitoring and individual differences in impulsivity and compulsivity. A total of 142 participants were recruited into four groups, each with different combinations of impulsivity and compulsivity, and they performed a flanker task to assess error-related brain activity. We defined error-related brain activity as ERN amplitude and theta power. Single-trial regression was employed to analyze the amplitude differences between incorrect and correct trials within the ERN time window. The findings revealed that impulsivity, compulsivity, and different measures of response processing exhibited distinct interactions, which were influenced by the configuration of impulsivity and compulsivity, but also depended on the measure of response processing. Specifically, high compulsivity predicted larger ERN amplitudes in individuals with low impulsivity, whereas high impulsivity had no significant effect on ERN amplitude in individuals with low compulsivity. Furthermore, when both impulsivity and compulsivity were high, no significant increase in ERN amplitude was observed; instead, there was a reduced difference between incorrect and correct trials. No significant differences were found for theta power. While the association between error-related brain activity and transdiagnostic markers or psychopathology may be smaller than generally assumed, considering the interaction between different transdiagnostic markers and their facets can enhance our understanding of the complex associations that arise during the investigation of neural correlates of performance monitoring, specifically the ERN.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139708911","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pupil dilation reveals the intensity of touch. 瞳孔放大可显示触摸的强度。
Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-16 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14538
Antonia F Ten Brink, Iris Heiner, H Chris Dijkerman, Christoph Strauch

Touch is important for many aspects of our daily activities. One of the most important tactile characteristics is its perceived intensity. However, quantifying the intensity of perceived tactile stimulation is not always possible using overt responses. Here, we show that pupil responses can objectively index the intensity of tactile stimulation in the absence of overt participant responses. In Experiment 1 (n = 32), we stimulated three reportedly differentially sensitive body locations (finger, forearm, and calf) with a single tap of a tactor while tracking pupil responses. Tactile stimulation resulted in greater pupil dilation than a baseline without stimulation. Furthermore, pupils dilated more for the more sensitive location (finger) than for the less sensitive location (forearm and calf). In Experiment 2 (n = 20) we extended these findings by manipulating the intensity of the stimulation with three different intensities, here a short vibration, always at the little finger. Again, pupils dilated more when being stimulated at higher intensities as compared to lower intensities. In summary, pupils dilated more for more sensitive parts of the body at constant stimulation intensity and for more intense stimulation at constant location. Taken together, the results show that the intensity of perceived tactile stimulation can be objectively measured with pupil responses - and that such responses are a versatile marker for touch research. Our findings may pave the way for previously impossible objective tests of tactile sensitivity, for example in minimally conscious state patients.

触觉对我们日常活动的许多方面都很重要。最重要的触觉特征之一是其感知强度。然而,量化感知到的触觉刺激强度并不总能通过公开的反应来实现。在这里,我们展示了瞳孔反应可以在没有公开参与者反应的情况下客观地反映触觉刺激的强度。在实验 1(n = 32)中,我们在追踪瞳孔反应的同时,用触觉器轻触三个据报道敏感度不同的身体部位(手指、前臂和小腿)。与没有刺激的基线相比,触觉刺激导致的瞳孔放大更大。此外,与敏感度较低的部位(前臂和小腿)相比,敏感度较高的部位(手指)的瞳孔放大程度更大。在实验 2(n = 20)中,我们通过三种不同强度的刺激来扩展这些研究结果。同样,与较低强度的刺激相比,较高强度的刺激会使瞳孔放大。总之,在恒定的刺激强度和恒定的刺激位置下,瞳孔在身体更敏感的部位放大得更多。总之,研究结果表明,可以通过瞳孔反应客观地测量所感知的触觉刺激强度,而且这种反应是触觉研究的一种多功能标记。我们的研究结果可能会为以前无法进行的触觉灵敏度客观测试铺平道路,例如在微意识状态下对病人进行测试。
{"title":"Pupil dilation reveals the intensity of touch.","authors":"Antonia F Ten Brink, Iris Heiner, H Chris Dijkerman, Christoph Strauch","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14538","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14538","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Touch is important for many aspects of our daily activities. One of the most important tactile characteristics is its perceived intensity. However, quantifying the intensity of perceived tactile stimulation is not always possible using overt responses. Here, we show that pupil responses can objectively index the intensity of tactile stimulation in the absence of overt participant responses. In Experiment 1 (n = 32), we stimulated three reportedly differentially sensitive body locations (finger, forearm, and calf) with a single tap of a tactor while tracking pupil responses. Tactile stimulation resulted in greater pupil dilation than a baseline without stimulation. Furthermore, pupils dilated more for the more sensitive location (finger) than for the less sensitive location (forearm and calf). In Experiment 2 (n = 20) we extended these findings by manipulating the intensity of the stimulation with three different intensities, here a short vibration, always at the little finger. Again, pupils dilated more when being stimulated at higher intensities as compared to lower intensities. In summary, pupils dilated more for more sensitive parts of the body at constant stimulation intensity and for more intense stimulation at constant location. Taken together, the results show that the intensity of perceived tactile stimulation can be objectively measured with pupil responses - and that such responses are a versatile marker for touch research. Our findings may pave the way for previously impossible objective tests of tactile sensitivity, for example in minimally conscious state patients.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139743071","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Conditional deviant repetition in the oddball paradigm modulates processing at the level of P3a but not MMN. 奇球范式中的条件性偏差重复会调节P3a水平的加工过程,但不会调节MMN水平的加工过程。
Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-17 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14545
Nina Coy, Alexandra Bendixen, Sabine Grimm, Urte Roeber, Erich Schröger

The auditory system has an amazing ability to rapidly encode auditory regularities. Evidence comes from the popular oddball paradigm, in which frequent (standard) sounds are occasionally exchanged for rare deviant sounds, which then elicit signs of prediction error based on their unexpectedness (e.g., MMN and P3a). Here, we examine the widely neglected characteristics of deviants being bearers of predictive information themselves; naive participants listened to sound sequences constructed according to a new, modified version of the oddball paradigm including two types of deviants that followed diametrically opposed rules: one deviant sound occurred mostly in pairs (repetition rule), the other deviant sound occurred mostly in isolation (non-repetition rule). Due to this manipulation, the sound following a first deviant (either the same deviant or a standard) was either predictable or unpredictable based on its conditional probability associated with the preceding deviant sound. Our behavioral results from an active deviant detection task replicate previous findings that deviant repetition rules (based on conditional probability) can be extracted when behaviorally relevant. Our electrophysiological findings obtained in a passive listening setting indicate that conditional probability also translates into differential processing at the P3a level. However, MMN was confined to global deviants and was not sensitive to conditional probability. This suggests that higher-level processing concerned with stimulus selection and/or evaluation (reflected in P3a) but not lower-level sensory processing (reflected in MMN) considers rarely encountered rules.

听觉系统具有快速编码听觉规律的惊人能力。流行的 "怪音范式"(oddball paradigm)就是证明,在该范式中,经常出现的(标准)声音偶尔会被换成罕见的偏差声音,而偏差声音会根据其出乎意料的程度(如 MMN 和 P3a)引起预测错误的迹象。在这里,我们要研究的是被广泛忽视的偏差声音本身作为预测信息载体的特点;天真的参与者聆听的声音序列是根据一种新的、改进版的奇异球范式构建的,其中包括两种遵循截然相反规则的偏差声音:一种偏差声音大多成对出现(重复规则),另一种偏差声音大多单独出现(非重复规则)。在这种操纵下,第一个异常声音(相同的异常声音或标准声音)之后的声音要么是可预测的,要么是不可预测的,这取决于其与前面异常声音相关的条件概率。我们从主动偏差检测任务中得出的行为结果重复了之前的研究结果,即偏差重复规则(基于条件概率)在与行为相关时可以被提取出来。我们在被动聆听环境中获得的电生理结果表明,条件概率也会转化为 P3a 水平的差异处理。然而,MMN 仅限于全局偏差,对条件概率并不敏感。这表明,与刺激选择和/或评估有关的高层次处理(反映在 P3a 中),而不是低层次的感觉处理(反映在 MMN 中)会考虑很少遇到的规则。
{"title":"Conditional deviant repetition in the oddball paradigm modulates processing at the level of P3a but not MMN.","authors":"Nina Coy, Alexandra Bendixen, Sabine Grimm, Urte Roeber, Erich Schröger","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14545","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14545","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The auditory system has an amazing ability to rapidly encode auditory regularities. Evidence comes from the popular oddball paradigm, in which frequent (standard) sounds are occasionally exchanged for rare deviant sounds, which then elicit signs of prediction error based on their unexpectedness (e.g., MMN and P3a). Here, we examine the widely neglected characteristics of deviants being bearers of predictive information themselves; naive participants listened to sound sequences constructed according to a new, modified version of the oddball paradigm including two types of deviants that followed diametrically opposed rules: one deviant sound occurred mostly in pairs (repetition rule), the other deviant sound occurred mostly in isolation (non-repetition rule). Due to this manipulation, the sound following a first deviant (either the same deviant or a standard) was either predictable or unpredictable based on its conditional probability associated with the preceding deviant sound. Our behavioral results from an active deviant detection task replicate previous findings that deviant repetition rules (based on conditional probability) can be extracted when behaviorally relevant. Our electrophysiological findings obtained in a passive listening setting indicate that conditional probability also translates into differential processing at the P3a level. However, MMN was confined to global deviants and was not sensitive to conditional probability. This suggests that higher-level processing concerned with stimulus selection and/or evaluation (reflected in P3a) but not lower-level sensory processing (reflected in MMN) considers rarely encountered rules.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139748005","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A longitudinal study on the impact of high-altitude hypoxia on perceptual processes. 高海拔缺氧对感知过程影响的纵向研究。
Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14548
Fumei Guo, Changming Wang, Getong Tao, Hailin Ma, Jiaxing Zhang, Yan Wang

This study aimed to explore the neural mechanisms underlying high-altitude (HA) adaptation and deadaptation in perceptual processes in lowlanders. Eighteen healthy lowlanders were administered a facial S1-S2 matching task that included incomplete face (S1) and complete face (S2) photographs combined with ERP technology. Participants were tested at four time points: shortly before they departed the HA (Test 1), twenty-five days after entering the HA (Test 2), and one week (Test 3) and one month (Test 4) after returning to the lowlands. Compared with those at sea level (SL), shorter reaction times (RTs), shorter latencies of P1 and N170, and larger amplitudes of complete face N170 were found in HAs. After returning to SL, compared with that of HA, the amplitude of the incomplete face P1 was smaller after one week, and the complete face was smaller after one month. The right hemisphere N170 amplitude was greater after entering HA and one week after returning to SL than at baseline, but it returned to baseline after one month. Taken together, the current findings suggest that HA adaptation increases visual cortex excitation to accelerate perceptual processing. More mental resources are recruited during the configural encoding stage of complete faces after HA exposure. The perceptual processes affected by HA exposure are reversible after returning to SL, but the low-level processing stage differs between incomplete and complete faces due to neural compensation mechanisms. The configural encoding stage in the right hemisphere is affected by HA exposure and requires more than one week but less than one month to recover to baseline.

本研究旨在探索低地人感知过程中的高海拔(HA)适应和死适应的神经机制。18名健康的低地人接受了面部S1-S2匹配任务,其中包括结合ERP技术的不完整面部(S1)和完整面部(S2)照片。受试者在四个时间点接受了测试:离开医管局前不久(测试 1)、进入医管局二十五天后(测试 2)、返回低地一周后(测试 3)和一个月后(测试 4)。与在海平面(SL)时相比,在高地的反应时间(RTs)更短,P1和N170的潜伏期更短,完全面N170的振幅更大。与 HA 相比,返回 SL 后,一周后不完整面孔 P1 的振幅变小,一个月后完整面孔的振幅变小。进入 HA 和返回 SL 一周后,右半球 N170 振幅比基线时更大,但一个月后又恢复到基线。综上所述,目前的研究结果表明,HA 适应会增加视觉皮层的兴奋,从而加速知觉处理。暴露于 HA 后,在完整面孔的构型编码阶段会动用更多的心理资源。受 HA 影响的知觉过程在回到 SL 后是可逆的,但由于神经补偿机制的作用,不完整和完整人脸的低级处理阶段有所不同。右半球的构型编码阶段受到 HA 暴露的影响,需要一周以上但不到一个月的时间才能恢复到基线。
{"title":"A longitudinal study on the impact of high-altitude hypoxia on perceptual processes.","authors":"Fumei Guo, Changming Wang, Getong Tao, Hailin Ma, Jiaxing Zhang, Yan Wang","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14548","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14548","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study aimed to explore the neural mechanisms underlying high-altitude (HA) adaptation and deadaptation in perceptual processes in lowlanders. Eighteen healthy lowlanders were administered a facial S1-S2 matching task that included incomplete face (S1) and complete face (S2) photographs combined with ERP technology. Participants were tested at four time points: shortly before they departed the HA (Test 1), twenty-five days after entering the HA (Test 2), and one week (Test 3) and one month (Test 4) after returning to the lowlands. Compared with those at sea level (SL), shorter reaction times (RTs), shorter latencies of P1 and N170, and larger amplitudes of complete face N170 were found in HAs. After returning to SL, compared with that of HA, the amplitude of the incomplete face P1 was smaller after one week, and the complete face was smaller after one month. The right hemisphere N170 amplitude was greater after entering HA and one week after returning to SL than at baseline, but it returned to baseline after one month. Taken together, the current findings suggest that HA adaptation increases visual cortex excitation to accelerate perceptual processing. More mental resources are recruited during the configural encoding stage of complete faces after HA exposure. The perceptual processes affected by HA exposure are reversible after returning to SL, but the low-level processing stage differs between incomplete and complete faces due to neural compensation mechanisms. The configural encoding stage in the right hemisphere is affected by HA exposure and requires more than one week but less than one month to recover to baseline.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139934888","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interoception as a function of hypnotizability during rest and a heartbeat counting task. 在休息和心跳计数任务中,交互感知是催眠性的函数。
Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-06 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14535
Gioia Giusti, Žan Zelič, Alejandro Luis Callara, Laura Sebastiani, Enrica L Santarcangelo

The hypnotizability-related differences in morpho-functional characteristics of the insula could at least partially account for the differences in interoceptive accuracy (IA) observed between high and low hypnotizable individuals (highs, lows). Our aim was to investigate interoceptive processing in highs, lows, and medium hypnotizable individuals (mediums), who represent most of the population, during a 10-minute open eyes relaxation condition (Part 1) and three repetitions of consecutive 2-minute open eyes, closed eyes, and heartbeat counting conditions, followed by a 2-minute post-counting condition (Part 2). Electrocardiogram and electroencephalogram were recorded in 14 highs, 14 mediums, and 18 lows, classified according to the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale: Form A. Heartbeat-evoked cortical potentials (HEP) were extracted throughout the entire session, and IA index was obtained for the heartbeat counting task (HCT). In Part 1, significant hypnotizability-related differences were observed in the right central region in both early and late HEP components, with lows showing positive amplitudes and highs/mediums showing negative amplitudes. In Part 2, the same group differences were limited to the early component. Moreover, in the left frontal regions, only mediums modified their HEP during the counting task with respect to the open/closed eyes conditions, whereas highs displayed HEP differences between counting and post-counting rest. HCT did not show significant group differences. In conclusion, highs and mediums seem to be more similar than mediums and lows regarding HEP, despite the absence of significant differences in HCT. Nonetheless, a negative correlation between hypnotizability scores and HEP amplitudes was observed in the regions showing group differences.

与催眠能力相关的脑岛形态功能特征差异至少可以部分解释高催眠能力者和低催眠能力者(高催眠能力者和低催眠能力者)之间在感知间准确性(IA)上的差异。我们的目的是在 10 分钟睁眼放松条件(第一部分)和三次连续 2 分钟睁眼、闭眼和心跳计数条件(第二部分)中,研究高、低和中等催眠能力者(中等)的内感知处理过程。根据斯坦福催眠易感性量表,记录了 14 个高分、14 个中分和 18 个低分的心电图和脑电图:在整个疗程中提取了心跳诱发皮层电位(HEP),并在心跳计数任务(HCT)中获得了 IA 指数。在第一部分中,在右中央区的早期和晚期 HEP 成分中观察到了与催眠相关的显著差异,低频呈正振幅,高频/中频呈负振幅。在第二部分中,同样的群体差异仅限于早期成分。此外,在左额叶区域,只有中位数在计数任务中改变了睁眼/闭眼条件下的 HEP,而高位数在计数和计数后休息时显示出 HEP 差异。HCT 并未显示出明显的组间差异。总之,在 HEP 方面,高分者和中等者似乎比中等者和低分者更相似,尽管在 HCT 方面没有显著差异。然而,在出现组间差异的区域,催眠性评分与 HEP 振幅之间存在负相关。
{"title":"Interoception as a function of hypnotizability during rest and a heartbeat counting task.","authors":"Gioia Giusti, Žan Zelič, Alejandro Luis Callara, Laura Sebastiani, Enrica L Santarcangelo","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14535","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14535","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The hypnotizability-related differences in morpho-functional characteristics of the insula could at least partially account for the differences in interoceptive accuracy (IA) observed between high and low hypnotizable individuals (highs, lows). Our aim was to investigate interoceptive processing in highs, lows, and medium hypnotizable individuals (mediums), who represent most of the population, during a 10-minute open eyes relaxation condition (Part 1) and three repetitions of consecutive 2-minute open eyes, closed eyes, and heartbeat counting conditions, followed by a 2-minute post-counting condition (Part 2). Electrocardiogram and electroencephalogram were recorded in 14 highs, 14 mediums, and 18 lows, classified according to the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale: Form A. Heartbeat-evoked cortical potentials (HEP) were extracted throughout the entire session, and IA index was obtained for the heartbeat counting task (HCT). In Part 1, significant hypnotizability-related differences were observed in the right central region in both early and late HEP components, with lows showing positive amplitudes and highs/mediums showing negative amplitudes. In Part 2, the same group differences were limited to the early component. Moreover, in the left frontal regions, only mediums modified their HEP during the counting task with respect to the open/closed eyes conditions, whereas highs displayed HEP differences between counting and post-counting rest. HCT did not show significant group differences. In conclusion, highs and mediums seem to be more similar than mediums and lows regarding HEP, despite the absence of significant differences in HCT. Nonetheless, a negative correlation between hypnotizability scores and HEP amplitudes was observed in the regions showing group differences.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139693769","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Systemic neurophysiological signals of auditory predictive coding. 听觉预测编码的系统神经生理学信号
Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-13 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14544
Manuel Muñoz-Caracuel, Vanesa Muñoz, Francisco J Ruiz-Martínez, Antonio J Vázquez Morejón, Carlos M Gómez

Predictive coding framework posits that our brain continuously monitors changes in the environment and updates its predictive models, minimizing prediction errors to efficiently adapt to environmental demands. However, the underlying neurophysiological mechanisms of these predictive phenomena remain unclear. The present study aimed to explore the systemic neurophysiological correlates of predictive coding processes during passive and active auditory processing. Electroencephalography (EEG), functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), and autonomic nervous system (ANS) measures were analyzed using an auditory pattern-based novelty oddball paradigm. A sample of 32 healthy subjects was recruited. The results showed shared slow evoked potentials between passive and active conditions that could be interpreted as automatic predictive processes of anticipation and updating, independent of conscious attentional effort. A dissociated topography of the cortical hemodynamic activity and distinctive evoked potentials upon auditory pattern violation were also found between both conditions, whereas only conscious perception leading to imperative responses was accompanied by phasic ANS responses. These results suggest a systemic-level hierarchical reallocation of predictive coding neural resources as a function of contextual demands in the face of sensory stimulation. Principal component analysis permitted to associate the variability of some of the recorded signals.

预测编码框架认为,我们的大脑会持续监测环境变化并更新其预测模型,最大限度地减少预测误差,从而有效地适应环境需求。然而,这些预测现象的潜在神经生理学机制仍不清楚。本研究旨在探索被动和主动听觉处理过程中预测编码过程的系统神经生理学相关性。研究使用基于听觉模式的新奇怪异范式分析了脑电图(EEG)、功能性近红外光谱(fNIRS)和自律神经系统(ANS)测量。研究人员招募了 32 名健康受试者。结果显示,被动和主动条件下有共同的慢速诱发电位,可解释为预测和更新的自动预测过程,与有意识的注意努力无关。在这两种情况下,大脑皮层血流动力学活动和听觉模式违反时的独特诱发电位也存在差异,而只有导致强制性反应的有意识感知才伴有相位性自律神经系统反应。这些结果表明,在面对感官刺激时,预测编码神经资源会根据情境需求进行系统性的分层重新分配。通过主成分分析,可以将记录到的一些信号的可变性联系起来。
{"title":"Systemic neurophysiological signals of auditory predictive coding.","authors":"Manuel Muñoz-Caracuel, Vanesa Muñoz, Francisco J Ruiz-Martínez, Antonio J Vázquez Morejón, Carlos M Gómez","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14544","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14544","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Predictive coding framework posits that our brain continuously monitors changes in the environment and updates its predictive models, minimizing prediction errors to efficiently adapt to environmental demands. However, the underlying neurophysiological mechanisms of these predictive phenomena remain unclear. The present study aimed to explore the systemic neurophysiological correlates of predictive coding processes during passive and active auditory processing. Electroencephalography (EEG), functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), and autonomic nervous system (ANS) measures were analyzed using an auditory pattern-based novelty oddball paradigm. A sample of 32 healthy subjects was recruited. The results showed shared slow evoked potentials between passive and active conditions that could be interpreted as automatic predictive processes of anticipation and updating, independent of conscious attentional effort. A dissociated topography of the cortical hemodynamic activity and distinctive evoked potentials upon auditory pattern violation were also found between both conditions, whereas only conscious perception leading to imperative responses was accompanied by phasic ANS responses. These results suggest a systemic-level hierarchical reallocation of predictive coding neural resources as a function of contextual demands in the face of sensory stimulation. Principal component analysis permitted to associate the variability of some of the recorded signals.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139731287","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Temporal dynamics of autonomic nervous system responses under cognitive-emotional workload in obsessive-compulsive disorder. 强迫症患者在认知-情绪负荷下自律神经系统反应的时间动态。
Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14549
Galina Portnova, Guzal Khayrullina, Olga Martynova

Dysregulation of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) is commonly observed in various mental disorders, particularly when individuals engage in prolonged cognitive-emotional tasks that require ANS adjustment to workload. Although the understanding of the temporal dynamics of sympathetic and parasympathetic tones in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is limited, analyzing ANS reactions to cognitive-emotional workload could provide valuable insights into one of the underlying causes of OCD. This study investigated the temporal dynamics of heart rate (HR) and pupil area (PA) while participants with OCD and healthy volunteers solved antisaccade tasks, with affective pictures serving as central fixation stimuli. The data of 31 individuals with OCD and 30 healthy volunteers were included in the study, comprising three separate blocks, each lasting approximately 8 min. The results revealed an increase in sympathetic tone in the OCD group, with the most noticeable rise occurring during the middle part of each block, particularly during the presentation of negative stimuli. Healthy volunteers demonstrated adaptive temporal dynamics of HR and PA from the first block to the last block of tasks, whereas individuals with OCD exhibited fewer changes over time, suggesting a reduced adaptation of the ANS sympathetic tone to cognitive-emotional workload in OCD.

自律神经系统(ANS)失调常见于各种精神障碍,尤其是当患者长时间从事需要自律神经系统调整以适应工作量的认知-情感任务时。虽然人们对强迫症(OCD)中交感神经和副交感神经音调的时间动态了解有限,但分析自律神经系统对认知-情绪工作量的反应可以为了解强迫症的根本原因之一提供有价值的见解。本研究调查了强迫症患者和健康志愿者在以情感图片作为中心定点刺激的情况下完成反积累任务时心率(HR)和瞳孔面积(PA)的时间动态变化。研究包括 31 名强迫症患者和 30 名健康志愿者的数据,由三个独立的区块组成,每个区块持续约 8 分钟。结果显示,强迫症组交感神经张力增加,最明显的上升发生在每个区块的中间部分,尤其是在出现负面刺激时。从第一个任务块到最后一个任务块,健康志愿者表现出心率和脉搏加速度的时间动态适应性,而强迫症患者则表现出较少的时间变化,这表明强迫症患者的自律神经系统交感神经张力对认知-情绪工作量的适应性降低。
{"title":"Temporal dynamics of autonomic nervous system responses under cognitive-emotional workload in obsessive-compulsive disorder.","authors":"Galina Portnova, Guzal Khayrullina, Olga Martynova","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14549","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14549","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Dysregulation of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) is commonly observed in various mental disorders, particularly when individuals engage in prolonged cognitive-emotional tasks that require ANS adjustment to workload. Although the understanding of the temporal dynamics of sympathetic and parasympathetic tones in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is limited, analyzing ANS reactions to cognitive-emotional workload could provide valuable insights into one of the underlying causes of OCD. This study investigated the temporal dynamics of heart rate (HR) and pupil area (PA) while participants with OCD and healthy volunteers solved antisaccade tasks, with affective pictures serving as central fixation stimuli. The data of 31 individuals with OCD and 30 healthy volunteers were included in the study, comprising three separate blocks, each lasting approximately 8 min. The results revealed an increase in sympathetic tone in the OCD group, with the most noticeable rise occurring during the middle part of each block, particularly during the presentation of negative stimuli. Healthy volunteers demonstrated adaptive temporal dynamics of HR and PA from the first block to the last block of tasks, whereas individuals with OCD exhibited fewer changes over time, suggesting a reduced adaptation of the ANS sympathetic tone to cognitive-emotional workload in OCD.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139975214","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychophysiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1