首页 > 最新文献

Child development research最新文献

英文 中文
Metasynthesis of Factors Contributing to Children’s Communication Development: Influence on Reading and Mathematics 影响儿童沟通发展因素的综合:对阅读和数学的影响
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2017-02-19 DOI: 10.1155/2017/4506098
Amber J. Godwin, M. M. Capraro, W. Rupley, R. Capraro
The purpose of this study is to determine what previous studies have found to be factors that contribute to a child’s initial communication development and previously identified effects of reading mathematics storybooks to toddlers or preschoolers. Therefore, it follows that the earlier a preschooler is exposed to mathematics vocabulary, the easier mathematics vocabulary acquisition and understanding can be for that child, which can result in an increase in future academic achievement. This metasynthesis was conducted to gather information on the effects that interactive relationships with caregivers have on a child’s ability to communicate and then how symbiotic reading and mathematics interventions can affect a child’s ability to think and communicate mathematically. According to the data analyzed for this metasynthesis, caregivers’ language relationships help facilitate a child’s early communication development and reading and mathematics symbiotic instruction can lead to developing a child’s ability to think and communicate mathematically.
本研究的目的是确定之前的研究发现的有助于儿童初始沟通发展的因素,以及之前确定的阅读数学故事书对幼儿或学龄前儿童的影响。因此,学龄前儿童越早接触数学词汇,就越容易习得和理解数学词汇,从而提高未来的学习成绩。这项综合研究的目的是收集与照顾者的互动关系对儿童沟通能力的影响,以及共生阅读和数学干预如何影响儿童的数学思考和沟通能力。根据这一综合分析的数据,照顾者的语言关系有助于促进孩子早期的沟通发展,阅读和数学共生教学可以促进孩子数学思维和沟通能力的发展。
{"title":"Metasynthesis of Factors Contributing to Children’s Communication Development: Influence on Reading and Mathematics","authors":"Amber J. Godwin, M. M. Capraro, W. Rupley, R. Capraro","doi":"10.1155/2017/4506098","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/4506098","url":null,"abstract":"The purpose of this study is to determine what previous studies have found to be factors that contribute to a child’s initial communication development and previously identified effects of reading mathematics storybooks to toddlers or preschoolers. Therefore, it follows that the earlier a preschooler is exposed to mathematics vocabulary, the easier mathematics vocabulary acquisition and understanding can be for that child, which can result in an increase in future academic achievement. This metasynthesis was conducted to gather information on the effects that interactive relationships with caregivers have on a child’s ability to communicate and then how symbiotic reading and mathematics interventions can affect a child’s ability to think and communicate mathematically. According to the data analyzed for this metasynthesis, caregivers’ language relationships help facilitate a child’s early communication development and reading and mathematics symbiotic instruction can lead to developing a child’s ability to think and communicate mathematically.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-02-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2017/4506098","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46006192","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Fourteen-Month-Olds Adapt Their Imitative Behavior in Light of a Model’s Constraints 14个月大的婴儿根据模型的约束调整他们的模仿行为
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2017-01-09 DOI: 10.1155/2017/8080649
K. Gellén, D. Buttelmann
Rather than reenacting every action they observe, preverbal infants adapt their imitative behavior. Although previous studies have revealed the capability of preverbal infants to imitate selectively, the question about the adaptability of this behavior on an individual level did not attract considerable scientific attention until now. In the current study, we investigated whether 14-month-old infants flexibly alternate their imitative response in accordance with a model’s changing physical constraints in a body-part imitation paradigm. Participants were presented with two novel actions whereby a model illuminated a light-box and turned on a sound-box, either by using her forehead (head touch) or by sitting on the apparatus (sit-touch). Each participant observed these tasks in two conditions: once where the model’s hands were occupied and once where her hands were free while executing the head or sit-touch. Participants were more likely to reenact the observed novel behavior when the model had freely chosen to perform it than when she had to do so due to physical constraints. Not only did we replicate a number of previous findings, we show here that preverbal infants adapt their imitative behavior across conditions based on the physical constraints of the model. These results point towards the adaptable nature of imitative behavior also on an individual level. This ability might be one of the building blocks for children for learning their social group’s specific action repertoire.
言语前的婴儿并没有重现他们观察到的每一个动作,而是调整他们的模仿行为。尽管先前的研究已经揭示了语前婴儿选择性模仿的能力,但直到现在,这种行为在个体层面上的适应性问题才引起科学界的广泛关注。在目前的研究中,我们调查了14个月大的婴儿是否根据身体部位模仿范式中模型不断变化的物理约束灵活地改变他们的模仿反应。参与者被介绍了两个新颖的动作,一个模特通过前额(头部触摸)或坐在设备上(坐着触摸)点亮灯箱并打开音箱。每个参与者在两种情况下观察这些任务:一种是模特的手被占据的地方,另一种是在进行头部或坐姿触摸时她的手空闲的地方。当模特自由选择表演时,参与者更有可能重现观察到的新奇行为,而不是当她因身体限制而不得不这样做时。我们不仅复制了以前的一些发现,而且在这里我们表明,言语前的婴儿根据模型的物理约束,在不同的条件下调整他们的模仿行为。这些结果指向了模仿行为在个体层面上的适应性。这种能力可能是儿童学习其社会群体特定动作的基石之一。
{"title":"Fourteen-Month-Olds Adapt Their Imitative Behavior in Light of a Model’s Constraints","authors":"K. Gellén, D. Buttelmann","doi":"10.1155/2017/8080649","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/8080649","url":null,"abstract":"Rather than reenacting every action they observe, preverbal infants adapt their imitative behavior. Although previous studies have revealed the capability of preverbal infants to imitate selectively, the question about the adaptability of this behavior on an individual level did not attract considerable scientific attention until now. In the current study, we investigated whether 14-month-old infants flexibly alternate their imitative response in accordance with a model’s changing physical constraints in a body-part imitation paradigm. Participants were presented with two novel actions whereby a model illuminated a light-box and turned on a sound-box, either by using her forehead (head touch) or by sitting on the apparatus (sit-touch). Each participant observed these tasks in two conditions: once where the model’s hands were occupied and once where her hands were free while executing the head or sit-touch. Participants were more likely to reenact the observed novel behavior when the model had freely chosen to perform it than when she had to do so due to physical constraints. Not only did we replicate a number of previous findings, we show here that preverbal infants adapt their imitative behavior across conditions based on the physical constraints of the model. These results point towards the adaptable nature of imitative behavior also on an individual level. This ability might be one of the building blocks for children for learning their social group’s specific action repertoire.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2017/8080649","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42625923","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Children Adopt the Traits of Characters in a Narrative 儿童在叙事中具有人物的特点
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2017-01-01 DOI: 10.1155/2017/6838079
Rebecca A. Dore, Eric D. Smith, A. Lillard
Adults adopt the traits of characters in narratives, but little is known about whether children do so. In Study 1, 7- and 10-year-olds () heard a 2.5-minute recording about a professor or cheerleader. Reporting higher engagement in the professor narrative related to more time playing with an analytical toy (a Rubik’s cube), whereas reporting higher engagement in the cheerleader narrative related to less time playing with Rubik’s cube. However, although children were randomly assigned to a narrative, within condition children may have had preexisting personality differences causing them both to become more engaged in that narrative and also to behave more like that character afterwards. To control for this possibility, in Study 2 children () were given perspective-taking or objective instructions. Interestingly, both instructions created higher engagement than in Study 1, resulting in main effects of narrative. Children in the professor condition, compared to those in the cheerleader condition, spent more time playing with Rubik’s cube and self-reported higher levels of professor-relevant characteristics (e.g., smart, good at teaching). These studies show that, by the elementary school years and particularly when highly engaged in a narrative, children adopt the traits of a narrative’s central character.
成年人会接受故事中人物的特征,但很少有人知道儿童是否会这样做。在研究1中,7岁和10岁的孩子听了一段关于教授或啦啦队长的2.5分钟录音。教授叙述中较高的粘性与更多时间玩分析玩具(如魔方)有关,而啦啦队长叙述中较高的粘性与较少时间玩魔方有关。然而,尽管孩子们被随机分配到一个故事中,在这种情况下,孩子们可能已经有了预先存在的性格差异,这使得他们更投入到那个故事中,而且之后的行为也更像那个角色。为了控制这种可能性,在研究2中,对儿童()进行了换位思考或客观指导。有趣的是,与研究1相比,这两种指令都创造了更高的参与度,从而产生了叙事的主要效果。与拉拉队的孩子相比,教授组的孩子花更多的时间玩魔方,并且自我报告的教授相关特征水平更高(例如,聪明,擅长教学)。这些研究表明,到了小学阶段,尤其是在高度参与叙事的时候,孩子们会采用叙事中心人物的特征。
{"title":"Children Adopt the Traits of Characters in a Narrative","authors":"Rebecca A. Dore, Eric D. Smith, A. Lillard","doi":"10.1155/2017/6838079","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/6838079","url":null,"abstract":"Adults adopt the traits of characters in narratives, but little is known about whether children do so. In Study 1, 7- and 10-year-olds () heard a 2.5-minute recording about a professor or cheerleader. Reporting higher engagement in the professor narrative related to more time playing with an analytical toy (a Rubik’s cube), whereas reporting higher engagement in the cheerleader narrative related to less time playing with Rubik’s cube. However, although children were randomly assigned to a narrative, within condition children may have had preexisting personality differences causing them both to become more engaged in that narrative and also to behave more like that character afterwards. To control for this possibility, in Study 2 children () were given perspective-taking or objective instructions. Interestingly, both instructions created higher engagement than in Study 1, resulting in main effects of narrative. Children in the professor condition, compared to those in the cheerleader condition, spent more time playing with Rubik’s cube and self-reported higher levels of professor-relevant characteristics (e.g., smart, good at teaching). These studies show that, by the elementary school years and particularly when highly engaged in a narrative, children adopt the traits of a narrative’s central character.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2017/6838079","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64698803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Adults’ Theory of Infants’ Mind: A Comparison between Parents and Nonparents 成人的婴儿心理理论:父母与非父母的比较
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2017-01-01 DOI: 10.1155/2017/8724562
I. Shinohara, Y. Moriguchi
This study examined whether there were parental state differences in interpretations of infants’ behaviours as associated with some mental states. Parents, nonparent women, and nonparent men were shown video clips that displayed several infant behaviours (e.g., playing with his/her mother). Then they were given two tasks. In a rating task, participants were asked to rate the likelihood of the filmed infant to have a mental state. On the other hand, in a description task, participants were instructed to explicitly describe the filmed infants’ mental state in an open-ended manner. Importantly, all participants were asked to report the meaning of infants’ behaviour in specific acts from the same set of infants’ behaviours (e.g., the infants saw mother’s face and smiled). The results revealed that parents and nonparent women significantly higher rated that infants were likely to express a mental state in the rating task than nonparent men did. On the other hand, parents were more likely to describe the filmed infants’ mental states in the description task than nonparent women and nonparent men did. Results suggest that parents interpret more meanings from infants’ behaviours compared to nonparents, even when both parents and nonparents equally focused on infants’ behaviours.
本研究考察了父母对婴儿行为的解释是否存在与某些心理状态相关的状态差异。研究人员向父母、非父母的女性和非父母的男性播放了一些婴儿行为的视频片段(例如,与他/她的母亲玩耍)。然后他们被分配了两项任务。在一项评级任务中,参与者被要求对拍摄的婴儿有精神状态的可能性进行评级。另一方面,在描述任务中,参与者被要求以开放式的方式明确描述被拍摄婴儿的精神状态。重要的是,所有参与者都被要求从同一组婴儿行为中报告婴儿在特定行为中的行为含义(例如,婴儿看到母亲的脸并微笑)。结果显示,父母和非父母的女性对婴儿在评估任务中可能表达的精神状态的评价明显高于非父母的男性。另一方面,在描述任务中,父母比非父母的女性和非父母的男性更有可能描述被拍摄婴儿的精神状态。结果表明,即使父母和非父母都同样关注婴儿的行为,与非父母相比,父母从婴儿的行为中解读出更多的含义。
{"title":"Adults’ Theory of Infants’ Mind: A Comparison between Parents and Nonparents","authors":"I. Shinohara, Y. Moriguchi","doi":"10.1155/2017/8724562","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/8724562","url":null,"abstract":"This study examined whether there were parental state differences in interpretations of infants’ behaviours as associated with some mental states. Parents, nonparent women, and nonparent men were shown video clips that displayed several infant behaviours (e.g., playing with his/her mother). Then they were given two tasks. In a rating task, participants were asked to rate the likelihood of the filmed infant to have a mental state. On the other hand, in a description task, participants were instructed to explicitly describe the filmed infants’ mental state in an open-ended manner. Importantly, all participants were asked to report the meaning of infants’ behaviour in specific acts from the same set of infants’ behaviours (e.g., the infants saw mother’s face and smiled). The results revealed that parents and nonparent women significantly higher rated that infants were likely to express a mental state in the rating task than nonparent men did. On the other hand, parents were more likely to describe the filmed infants’ mental states in the description task than nonparent women and nonparent men did. Results suggest that parents interpret more meanings from infants’ behaviours compared to nonparents, even when both parents and nonparents equally focused on infants’ behaviours.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2017/8724562","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64714968","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Mental State Talk Structure in Children's Narratives: A Cluster Analysis 儿童叙事中的心理状态话语结构:聚类分析
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2017-01-01 DOI: 10.1155/2017/1725487
G. Pinto, C. Primi, C. Tarchi, L. Bigozzi
This study analysed children’s Theory of Mind (ToM) as assessed by mental state talk in oral narratives. We hypothesized that the children’s mental state talk in narratives has an underlying structure, with specific terms organized in clusters. Ninety-eight children attending the last year of kindergarten were asked to tell a story twice, at the beginning and at the end of the school year. Mental state talk was analysed by identifying terms and expressions referring to perceptual, physiological, emotional, willingness, cognitive, moral, and sociorelational states. The cluster analysis showed that children’s mental state talk is organized in two main clusters: perceptual states and affective states. Results from the study confirm the feasibility of narratives as an outlet to inquire mental state talk and offer a more fine-grained analysis of mental state talk structure.
本研究分析了儿童的心理理论(ToM),通过口头叙述中的心理状态谈话来评估。我们假设儿童在叙事中的心理状态对话有一个潜在的结构,特定的术语被组织在集群中。98名参加幼儿园最后一年的孩子被要求在学年开始和结束时讲两次故事。通过识别涉及感知、生理、情感、意愿、认知、道德和社会关系状态的术语和表达来分析心理状态谈话。聚类分析表明,儿童的心理状态话语主要分为两类:感知状态和情感状态。研究结果证实了叙事作为探究心理状态话语出口的可行性,并对心理状态话语结构进行了更细致的分析。
{"title":"Mental State Talk Structure in Children's Narratives: A Cluster Analysis","authors":"G. Pinto, C. Primi, C. Tarchi, L. Bigozzi","doi":"10.1155/2017/1725487","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/1725487","url":null,"abstract":"This study analysed children’s Theory of Mind (ToM) as assessed by mental state talk in oral narratives. We hypothesized that the children’s mental state talk in narratives has an underlying structure, with specific terms organized in clusters. Ninety-eight children attending the last year of kindergarten were asked to tell a story twice, at the beginning and at the end of the school year. Mental state talk was analysed by identifying terms and expressions referring to perceptual, physiological, emotional, willingness, cognitive, moral, and sociorelational states. The cluster analysis showed that children’s mental state talk is organized in two main clusters: perceptual states and affective states. Results from the study confirm the feasibility of narratives as an outlet to inquire mental state talk and offer a more fine-grained analysis of mental state talk structure.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2017/1725487","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64654102","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
How Usual Is "Play as You Usually Would"? A Comparison of Naturalistic Mother-Infant Interactions with Videorecorded Play Sessions in Three Cultural Communities “像往常一样玩”有多常见?三个文化群体中自然主义母婴互动与录像游戏会话的比较
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2017-01-01 DOI: 10.1155/2017/7842030
M. Abels, Z. Papaligoura, Bettina Lamm, Relindis D. Yovsi
In developmental research, mothers are frequently asked to “play as you usually would.” In this study, maternal behavior towards their three-month-olds in three cultural communities (Nso, Cameroon; Gujarati, India; Athens, Greece) was compared between videorecorded “play” situations and naturalistic observations. If there is consistency, videorecorded “play” episodes can be used as a proxy for daily behavior. Body contact, body stimulation, face-to-face situations, and object stimulation were coded. While individual mothers showed consistent levels of body contact and face-to-face and object stimulation in both situations, there were also high correlations across the different types of behaviors. Only body contact and object stimulation correlate significantly across behavioral frames but not with each other across or within either observational frame. They can therefore be understood as behaviors with some discriminatory power. Mothers generally show a higher frequency of behaviors in the videorecorded play situations than during the everyday observations across all three communities. However, the samples differ in the extent to which three of the four behaviors are seen more in the videorecorded play sessions. A broader and general understanding of mothers’ ethnotheories and daily activities in each community is required in order to interpret videographed “play as you usually would” situations.
在发展研究中,妈妈们经常被要求“像平时一样玩耍”。在这项研究中,三个文化社区的母亲对三个月大婴儿的行为(喀麦隆Nso;古吉拉特语、印度;雅典,希腊)在录像“游戏”情境和自然观察之间进行了比较。如果有一致性,视频记录的“游戏”片段可以作为日常行为的代表。身体接触、身体刺激、面对面情境和物体刺激被编码。虽然在两种情况下,每个母亲都表现出一致的身体接触、面对面和物体刺激水平,但不同类型的行为之间也存在高度相关性。只有身体接触和物体刺激在行为框架中有显著的相关性,但在任何一个观察框架中都没有。因此,它们可以被理解为具有某种歧视性权力的行为。在所有三个社区中,母亲们在录像中表现出的行为频率通常高于日常观察中的行为频率。然而,样本的不同之处在于,四种行为中的三种在录像游戏过程中出现得更多。为了解释拍摄的“像往常一样玩耍”的情况,需要对每个社区中母亲的民族理论和日常活动有更广泛和普遍的了解。
{"title":"How Usual Is \"Play as You Usually Would\"? A Comparison of Naturalistic Mother-Infant Interactions with Videorecorded Play Sessions in Three Cultural Communities","authors":"M. Abels, Z. Papaligoura, Bettina Lamm, Relindis D. Yovsi","doi":"10.1155/2017/7842030","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/7842030","url":null,"abstract":"In developmental research, mothers are frequently asked to “play as you usually would.” In this study, maternal behavior towards their three-month-olds in three cultural communities (Nso, Cameroon; Gujarati, India; Athens, Greece) was compared between videorecorded “play” situations and naturalistic observations. If there is consistency, videorecorded “play” episodes can be used as a proxy for daily behavior. Body contact, body stimulation, face-to-face situations, and object stimulation were coded. While individual mothers showed consistent levels of body contact and face-to-face and object stimulation in both situations, there were also high correlations across the different types of behaviors. Only body contact and object stimulation correlate significantly across behavioral frames but not with each other across or within either observational frame. They can therefore be understood as behaviors with some discriminatory power. Mothers generally show a higher frequency of behaviors in the videorecorded play situations than during the everyday observations across all three communities. However, the samples differ in the extent to which three of the four behaviors are seen more in the videorecorded play sessions. A broader and general understanding of mothers’ ethnotheories and daily activities in each community is required in order to interpret videographed “play as you usually would” situations.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2017/7842030","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64705659","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Livelihood of Street Children and the Role of Social Intervention: Insights from Literature Using Meta-Analysis 流浪儿童的生计与社会干预的作用:基于文献荟萃分析的见解
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2016-11-17 DOI: 10.1155/2016/3582101
Habtamu Alem, A. Laha
As studies done by different scholars indicate that the present status of street children is remarkably insightful, this invites us to systematically review the existing literature by using meta-analysis. In this paper 31 studies were reviewed by applying a predetermined set of inclusion and exclusion criteria as part of meta-analysis. These studies were compiled mainly from three continents (Africa, Asia, and Latin America), which are often observed to be diversified economically, politically, socially, and environmentally. Empirical evidences based on data generated from reviewed studies provide a holistic picture on the predominance of male street children among a total sample size of 68014 street children. Working as a daily labourer is considered as the most predominant informal occupation for street children. Empirical evidences suggest that majority of street dwellers were categorized into children working on the street in Africa, while in Asia a sizable proportion of them were abandoned from their house. Interestingly, it suggests that children coming to a street may be due to push factors like coercion by family, lack of access to education, and the existence of displeasing life in Africa, while in Asia children were pushed by family to beg and act as a day labourer and street vendor to assure the livelihood of their abandoned families. Statistical evidence based on odds ratio suggested no association between location of the study region and the characteristics of street children. Analysis of variance results showed that there exists a significant variation within a continent for all four variables (living condition, education, gender, and livelihood strategies). In fact, daily labour was the most acceptable means of livelihood earning and it is followed by street vendor and others. The present condition of street children necessitates social intervention to address the present problems of street dwellers by ensuring sustainable livelihood options among them.
由于不同学者的研究表明,街头儿童的现状是非常有见地的,这就要求我们通过荟萃分析系统地回顾现有的文献。本文采用预先确定的纳入和排除标准作为荟萃分析的一部分,对31项研究进行了综述。这些研究主要来自三大洲(非洲、亚洲和拉丁美洲),人们经常观察到这三大洲在经济、政治、社会和环境方面是多样化的。根据经审查的研究产生的数据得出的经验证据提供了一个整体情况,说明在68014名街头儿童的总样本量中,街头男孩占主导地位。日工被认为是街头儿童最主要的非正式职业。经验证据表明,在非洲,大多数街头居民被归类为在街头工作的儿童,而在亚洲,相当大比例的街头居民被遗弃在家中。有趣的是,研究表明,在非洲,儿童流落街头可能是由于家庭的强迫、缺乏受教育的机会和不愉快的生活等推动因素,而在亚洲,儿童则是被家庭强迫乞讨、做日工和街头小贩,以确保被遗弃家庭的生计。基于优势比的统计证据表明,研究区域的位置与街头儿童的特征之间没有关联。方差分析结果表明,在一个大陆内,所有四个变量(生活条件、教育、性别和生计策略)都存在显著差异。事实上,日工是最可接受的谋生手段,其次是街头小贩和其他人。街头儿童的现状需要社会干预,以确保他们有可持续的生计选择,从而解决街头居民目前的问题。
{"title":"Livelihood of Street Children and the Role of Social Intervention: Insights from Literature Using Meta-Analysis","authors":"Habtamu Alem, A. Laha","doi":"10.1155/2016/3582101","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/3582101","url":null,"abstract":"As studies done by different scholars indicate that the present status of street children is remarkably insightful, this invites us to systematically review the existing literature by using meta-analysis. In this paper 31 studies were reviewed by applying a predetermined set of inclusion and exclusion criteria as part of meta-analysis. These studies were compiled mainly from three continents (Africa, Asia, and Latin America), which are often observed to be diversified economically, politically, socially, and environmentally. Empirical evidences based on data generated from reviewed studies provide a holistic picture on the predominance of male street children among a total sample size of 68014 street children. Working as a daily labourer is considered as the most predominant informal occupation for street children. Empirical evidences suggest that majority of street dwellers were categorized into children working on the street in Africa, while in Asia a sizable proportion of them were abandoned from their house. Interestingly, it suggests that children coming to a street may be due to push factors like coercion by family, lack of access to education, and the existence of displeasing life in Africa, while in Asia children were pushed by family to beg and act as a day labourer and street vendor to assure the livelihood of their abandoned families. Statistical evidence based on odds ratio suggested no association between location of the study region and the characteristics of street children. Analysis of variance results showed that there exists a significant variation within a continent for all four variables (living condition, education, gender, and livelihood strategies). In fact, daily labour was the most acceptable means of livelihood earning and it is followed by street vendor and others. The present condition of street children necessitates social intervention to address the present problems of street dwellers by ensuring sustainable livelihood options among them.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-11-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2016/3582101","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64336184","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 32
Type and Duration of Home Activities of Children with Specific Language Impairment: Case Control Study Based on Parents’ Reports 特殊语言障碍儿童家庭活动的类型和持续时间:基于家长报告的病例对照研究
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2016-10-25 DOI: 10.1155/2016/1709314
Sinikka Hannus, T. Kauppila, Kaisa Launonen
Parents of children with specific language impairment (SLI) are advised to promote language development at home. However, it is not known if children with SLI differ from healthy controls in their daily activities. This study collected prospectively information about the home activities of the children with SLI and their matched controls by using parents’ daily reports. Participants were 20 matched pairs. The ages of children in matched pairs were from 6 to 8 years. During one week, parents filled in daily questionnaires of listed home activities. The observed time was between 5 pm and 9.30 pm each day and it was divided into 30-minute scoring periods. Parents of children with SLI reported more varying home activities and fewer activities of playing outdoors than parents of control children. Home activities with literacy or screen time did not show difference between the two groups, and neither did playing table top games. Parents of children with SLI did more overlapping choices when scoring home activities than parents of control children. Children with SLI seemed to spend somewhat less time with home activities that, in particular, may ask for language and social skills and collaboration with peers.
有特殊语言障碍(SLI)儿童的家长应在家促进语言发展。然而,目前尚不清楚患有特殊语言障碍的儿童在日常活动中是否与健康对照组不同。本研究通过父母的日常报告,前瞻性地收集了特殊语言障碍儿童及其对照的家庭活动信息。参与者是20对配对的。孩子们的年龄在6到8岁之间。在一周的时间里,家长们填写了列出的家庭活动的日常问卷。观察时间为每天下午5点至9点30分,分为30分钟的评分周期。患有特殊语言障碍儿童的父母报告说,与对照组儿童的父母相比,他们的家庭活动更多,户外活动更少。与识字或屏幕时间有关的家庭活动在两组之间没有表现出差异,玩桌面游戏也没有表现出差异。在对家庭活动进行评分时,特殊语言障碍儿童的父母比对照组儿童的父母做了更多的重叠选择。患有特殊语言障碍的孩子似乎花在家庭活动上的时间更少,特别是那些可能需要语言和社交技能以及与同龄人合作的活动。
{"title":"Type and Duration of Home Activities of Children with Specific Language Impairment: Case Control Study Based on Parents’ Reports","authors":"Sinikka Hannus, T. Kauppila, Kaisa Launonen","doi":"10.1155/2016/1709314","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/1709314","url":null,"abstract":"Parents of children with specific language impairment (SLI) are advised to promote language development at home. However, it is not known if children with SLI differ from healthy controls in their daily activities. This study collected prospectively information about the home activities of the children with SLI and their matched controls by using parents’ daily reports. Participants were 20 matched pairs. The ages of children in matched pairs were from 6 to 8 years. During one week, parents filled in daily questionnaires of listed home activities. The observed time was between 5 pm and 9.30 pm each day and it was divided into 30-minute scoring periods. Parents of children with SLI reported more varying home activities and fewer activities of playing outdoors than parents of control children. Home activities with literacy or screen time did not show difference between the two groups, and neither did playing table top games. Parents of children with SLI did more overlapping choices when scoring home activities than parents of control children. Children with SLI seemed to spend somewhat less time with home activities that, in particular, may ask for language and social skills and collaboration with peers.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2016/1709314","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64238178","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Child Welfare Deprivation in Rural Nigeria: A Counting Approach 尼日利亚农村儿童福利剥夺:一个统计方法
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2016-09-28 DOI: 10.1155/2016/6805485
O. Popoola, Adetola Ibidunni Adeoti
The study applies the counting approach to explain the deprivation concept among children under 5 years of age using the 2008 DHS data. Five dimensions of deprivation were used: safe drinking water, sanitation, housing, health, and nutrition largely recognized in the SDGs. In all, a total of 13561 children were sampled. About half of the children were males with a mean age of 28.27 months old. The assessment of dimensional deprivation showed that children are most deprived in sanitation, health, and access to safe drinking water while they were least deprived in nutrition. The situation is also marked with regional disparities with northern regions reporting higher deprivation rates than the southern regions but this rate was significantly higher in the sanitation dimension across regions. Considering deprivation counts, 33.9% of children suffer from more than three deprivations and approximately 85.2% from at least two deprivations. Child deprivation should be tackled using a holistic approach through social protection programmes to resolve children’s problems in an integrated manner which would in this case be more efficient and effective in safeguarding children’s rights to survival and development. Identifying the children suffering from single and multiple deprivations can help to target the interventions.
该研究使用2008年人口与健康调查数据,采用计数方法来解释5岁以下儿童的剥夺概念。使用了贫困的五个方面:安全饮用水、卫生设施、住房、健康和营养,这在可持续发展目标中得到了广泛认可。总共有13561名儿童被抽样。大约一半的儿童是男性,平均年龄为28.27个月。对维度剥夺的评估表明,儿童在卫生、健康和获得安全饮用水方面最缺乏,而在营养方面最缺乏。这种情况也存在区域差异,北部地区报告的贫困率高于南部地区,但各地区在卫生方面的贫困率明显高于南部地区。考虑到被剥夺的次数,33.9%的儿童遭受三次以上的剥夺,约85.2%的儿童遭受至少两次的剥夺。应通过社会保护方案以综合方式解决儿童问题,以一种全面的办法来解决儿童被剥夺的问题,在这种情况下,这种办法在保障儿童的生存和发展权利方面将更为有效和有效。确定遭受单一和多重剥夺的儿童有助于有针对性地采取干预措施。
{"title":"Child Welfare Deprivation in Rural Nigeria: A Counting Approach","authors":"O. Popoola, Adetola Ibidunni Adeoti","doi":"10.1155/2016/6805485","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/6805485","url":null,"abstract":"The study applies the counting approach to explain the deprivation concept among children under 5 years of age using the 2008 DHS data. Five dimensions of deprivation were used: safe drinking water, sanitation, housing, health, and nutrition largely recognized in the SDGs. In all, a total of 13561 children were sampled. About half of the children were males with a mean age of 28.27 months old. The assessment of dimensional deprivation showed that children are most deprived in sanitation, health, and access to safe drinking water while they were least deprived in nutrition. The situation is also marked with regional disparities with northern regions reporting higher deprivation rates than the southern regions but this rate was significantly higher in the sanitation dimension across regions. Considering deprivation counts, 33.9% of children suffer from more than three deprivations and approximately 85.2% from at least two deprivations. Child deprivation should be tackled using a holistic approach through social protection programmes to resolve children’s problems in an integrated manner which would in this case be more efficient and effective in safeguarding children’s rights to survival and development. Identifying the children suffering from single and multiple deprivations can help to target the interventions.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-09-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2016/6805485","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64490190","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
The use of causal language and filled pauses in children with and without autism 自闭症儿童和非自闭症儿童使用因果语言和填充停顿
Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2016-09-26 DOI: 10.1155/2016/8535868
A. Hallin, G. D. Garcia, Christina Reuterskiöld
This study investigated the relationship between pragmatic ability and two aspects of structural language in conversational language samples from 24 school-age children with and without high-functioning autism (HFA): causal statements and speech disruptions. In contrast to a majority of previous studies, grammatical complexity and mean length of utterance were factored into the analyses, since these are potential confounding variables. The results showed that children with HFA used fewer spontaneous causal statements and fewer filled pauses in conversation compared to children with typical development (TD). There was also a significant and positive relationship between filled pauses and pragmatic ability after controlling for structural language ability. The results may help us understand the conversational patterns of children with HFA better.
本研究调查了24名学龄期高功能自闭症儿童(HFA)会话语言样本的语用能力与结构语言的关系:因果陈述和言语中断。与之前的大多数研究相反,语法复杂性和平均话语长度被纳入分析因素,因为这些是潜在的混淆变量。结果表明,与典型发展(TD)儿童相比,HFA儿童在对话中使用更少的自发因果陈述和更少的填充停顿。在控制结构语言能力后,填充停顿与语用能力之间也存在显著的正相关关系。研究结果可能有助于我们更好地理解HFA患儿的对话模式。
{"title":"The use of causal language and filled pauses in children with and without autism","authors":"A. Hallin, G. D. Garcia, Christina Reuterskiöld","doi":"10.1155/2016/8535868","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/8535868","url":null,"abstract":"This study investigated the relationship between pragmatic ability and two aspects of structural language in conversational language samples from 24 school-age children with and without high-functioning autism (HFA): causal statements and speech disruptions. In contrast to a majority of previous studies, grammatical complexity and mean length of utterance were factored into the analyses, since these are potential confounding variables. The results showed that children with HFA used fewer spontaneous causal statements and fewer filled pauses in conversation compared to children with typical development (TD). There was also a significant and positive relationship between filled pauses and pragmatic ability after controlling for structural language ability. The results may help us understand the conversational patterns of children with HFA better.","PeriodicalId":9783,"journal":{"name":"Child development research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1155/2016/8535868","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"64572928","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
Child development research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1