Negar Mazloum-Farzaghi, Morgan D. Barense, Jennifer D. Ryan, Craig E. L. Stark, Rosanna K. Olsen
Early stages of Alzheimer's disease (AD) are associated with volume reductions in specific subregions of the medial temporal lobe (MTL). Using a manual segmentation method—the Olsen–Amaral–Palombo (OAP) protocol—previous work in healthy older adults showed that reductions in grey matter volumes in MTL subregions were associated with lower scores on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), suggesting atrophy may occur prior to diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment, a condition that often progresses to AD. However, current “gold standard” manual segmentation methods are labour intensive and time consuming. Here, we examined the utility of Automatic Segmentation of Hippocampal Subfields (ASHS) to detect volumetric differences in MTL subregions of healthy older adults who varied in cognitive status as determined by the MoCA. We trained ASHS on the OAP protocol to create the ASHS-OAP atlas and then examined how well automated segmentation replicated manual segmentation. Volumetric measures obtained from the ASHS-OAP atlas were also contrasted against those from the ASHS-PMC atlas, a widely used atlas provided by the ASHS team. The pattern of volumetric results was similar between the ASHS-OAP atlas and manual segmentation for anterolateral entorhinal cortex and perirhinal cortex, suggesting that ASHS-OAP is a viable alternative to current manual segmentation methods for detecting group differences based on cognitive status. Although ASHS-OAP and ASHS-PMC produced varying volumes for most regions of interest, they both identified early signs of neurodegeneration in CA2/CA3/DG and identified marginal differences in entorhinal cortex. Our findings highlight the utility of automated segmentation methods but still underscore the need for a unified and harmonized MTL segmentation atlas.
{"title":"The Effect of Segmentation Method on Medial Temporal Lobe Subregion Volumes in Aging","authors":"Negar Mazloum-Farzaghi, Morgan D. Barense, Jennifer D. Ryan, Craig E. L. Stark, Rosanna K. Olsen","doi":"10.1002/hbm.70054","DOIUrl":"10.1002/hbm.70054","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Early stages of Alzheimer's disease (AD) are associated with volume reductions in specific subregions of the medial temporal lobe (MTL). Using a manual segmentation method—the Olsen–Amaral–Palombo (OAP) protocol—previous work in healthy older adults showed that reductions in grey matter volumes in MTL subregions were associated with lower scores on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), suggesting atrophy may occur prior to diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment, a condition that often progresses to AD. However, current “gold standard” manual segmentation methods are labour intensive and time consuming. Here, we examined the utility of Automatic Segmentation of Hippocampal Subfields (ASHS) to detect volumetric differences in MTL subregions of healthy older adults who varied in cognitive status as determined by the MoCA. We trained ASHS on the OAP protocol to create the ASHS-OAP atlas and then examined how well automated segmentation replicated manual segmentation. Volumetric measures obtained from the ASHS-OAP atlas were also contrasted against those from the ASHS-PMC atlas, a widely used atlas provided by the ASHS team. The pattern of volumetric results was similar between the ASHS-OAP atlas and manual segmentation for anterolateral entorhinal cortex and perirhinal cortex, suggesting that ASHS-OAP is a viable alternative to current manual segmentation methods for detecting group differences based on cognitive status. Although ASHS-OAP and ASHS-PMC produced varying volumes for most regions of interest, they both identified early signs of neurodegeneration in CA<sub>2</sub>/CA<sub>3</sub>/DG and identified marginal differences in entorhinal cortex. Our findings highlight the utility of automated segmentation methods but still underscore the need for a unified and harmonized MTL segmentation atlas.</p>","PeriodicalId":13019,"journal":{"name":"Human Brain Mapping","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11502966/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142499388","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Marlous M. L. H. Verhulst, Hanneke M. Keijzer, Pauline C. W. van Gils, Caroline M. van Heugten, Frederick J. A. Meijer, Bart A. R. Tonino, Judith L. Bonnes, Thijs S. R. Delnoij, Jeannette Hofmeijer, Rick C. Helmich
Long-term cognitive impairment is common in cardiac arrest survivors. Screening to identify patients at risk is recommended. Functional magnetic resonance brain imaging (fMRI) holds potential to contribute to prediction of cognitive outcomes. In this study, we investigated the possible value of early changes in resting-state networks for predicting short and long-term cognitive functioning of cardiac arrest survivors. We performed a prospective multicenter cohort study in cardiac arrest survivors in three Dutch hospitals. Resting-state fMRI scans were acquired within a month after cardiac arrest. We primarily focused on functional connectivity within the default-mode network (DMN) and salience network (SN), and additionally explored functional connectivity in seven other networks. Cognitive outcome was measured using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) during hospital admission and at 3 and 12 months, and by neuropsychological examination (NPE) at 12 months. We tested mixed effects models to evaluate the value of connectivity within the networks for predicting global cognitive outcomes at the three time points, and long-term cognitive outcomes in the memory, attention, and executive functioning domains. We included 80 patients (age 60 ± 11 years, 72 (90%) male). MoCA scores increased significantly between hospital admission and 3 months (ΔMoCAhospital-3M = 2.89, p < 0.01), but not between 3 and 12 months (ΔMoCA3M–12M = 0.38, p = 0.52). Connectivity within the DMN, SN, and dorsal attention network (DAN) was positively related to global cognitive functioning during hospital admission (βDMN = 0.85, p = 0.03; βSN = 1.48, p < 0.01; βDAN = 0.96, p = 0.01), but not at 3 and 12 months. Network connectivity was also unrelated to long-term memory, attention, or executive functioning. Resting-state functional connectivity in the DMN, SN, and DAN measured in the first month after cardiac arrest is related to short-term global, but not long-term global or domain-specific cognitive performance of survivors. These results do not support the value of functional connectivity within these RSNs for prediction of long-term cognitive performance after cardiac arrest.
心脏骤停幸存者普遍存在长期认知障碍。建议对高危患者进行筛查。脑功能磁共振成像(fMRI)有可能有助于预测认知结果。在这项研究中,我们调查了静息态网络的早期变化对预测心脏骤停幸存者短期和长期认知功能的可能价值。我们对荷兰三家医院的心脏骤停幸存者进行了一项前瞻性多中心队列研究。我们在心脏骤停后一个月内采集了静息态 fMRI 扫描。我们主要研究了默认模式网络(DMN)和显著性网络(SN)的功能连接,此外还探讨了其他七个网络的功能连接。认知结果在入院时、3个月和12个月时使用蒙特利尔认知评估(MoCA)进行测量,在12个月时使用神经心理学检查(NPE)进行测量。我们测试了混合效应模型,以评估网络内的连接性对预测三个时间点的整体认知结果以及记忆、注意力和执行功能领域的长期认知结果的价值。我们共纳入了 80 名患者(年龄为 60 ± 11 岁,72 人(90%)为男性)。入院至3个月期间,MoCA得分明显增加(ΔMoCAhospital-3M = 2.89, p 3M-12M = 0.38, p = 0.52)。入院期间,DMN、SN和背侧注意网络(DAN)内的连通性与整体认知功能呈正相关(βDMN = 0.85,p = 0.03;βSN = 1.48,p DAN = 0.96,p = 0.01),但在3个月和12个月时则不相关。网络连通性也与长期记忆、注意力或执行功能无关。在心脏骤停后的第一个月测量的DMN、SN和DAN的静息态功能连接与幸存者的短期整体认知能力有关,但与长期整体认知能力或特定领域认知能力无关。这些结果并不支持这些RSN内的功能连接对预测心脏骤停后长期认知表现的价值。
{"title":"Functional connectivity in resting-state networks relates to short-term global cognitive functioning in cardiac arrest survivors","authors":"Marlous M. L. H. Verhulst, Hanneke M. Keijzer, Pauline C. W. van Gils, Caroline M. van Heugten, Frederick J. A. Meijer, Bart A. R. Tonino, Judith L. Bonnes, Thijs S. R. Delnoij, Jeannette Hofmeijer, Rick C. Helmich","doi":"10.1002/hbm.26769","DOIUrl":"10.1002/hbm.26769","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Long-term cognitive impairment is common in cardiac arrest survivors. Screening to identify patients at risk is recommended. Functional magnetic resonance brain imaging (fMRI) holds potential to contribute to prediction of cognitive outcomes. In this study, we investigated the possible value of early changes in resting-state networks for predicting short and long-term cognitive functioning of cardiac arrest survivors. We performed a prospective multicenter cohort study in cardiac arrest survivors in three Dutch hospitals. Resting-state fMRI scans were acquired within a month after cardiac arrest. We primarily focused on functional connectivity within the default-mode network (DMN) and salience network (SN), and additionally explored functional connectivity in seven other networks. Cognitive outcome was measured using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) during hospital admission and at 3 and 12 months, and by neuropsychological examination (NPE) at 12 months. We tested mixed effects models to evaluate the value of connectivity within the networks for predicting global cognitive outcomes at the three time points, and long-term cognitive outcomes in the memory, attention, and executive functioning domains. We included 80 patients (age 60 ± 11 years, 72 (90%) male). MoCA scores increased significantly between hospital admission and 3 months (ΔMoCA<sub>hospital-3M</sub> = 2.89, <i>p</i> < 0.01), but not between 3 and 12 months (ΔMoCA<sub>3M–12M</sub> = 0.38, <i>p</i> = 0.52). Connectivity within the DMN, SN, and dorsal attention network (DAN) was positively related to global cognitive functioning during hospital admission (<i>β</i><sub>DMN</sub> = 0.85, <i>p</i> = 0.03; <i>β</i><sub>SN</sub> = 1.48, <i>p</i> < 0.01; <i>β</i><sub>DAN</sub> = 0.96, <i>p</i> = 0.01), but not at 3 and 12 months. Network connectivity was also unrelated to long-term memory, attention, or executive functioning. Resting-state functional connectivity in the DMN, SN, and DAN measured in the first month after cardiac arrest is related to short-term global, but not long-term global or domain-specific cognitive performance of survivors. These results do not support the value of functional connectivity within these RSNs for prediction of long-term cognitive performance after cardiac arrest.</p>","PeriodicalId":13019,"journal":{"name":"Human Brain Mapping","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11502408/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142499273","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Afton M. Bierlich, Nanja T. Scheel, Leora S. Traiger, Daniel Keeser, Ralf Tepest, Alexandra L. Georgescu, Jana C. Koehler, Irene Sophia Plank, Christine M. Falter-Wagner
How the temporal dynamics of social interactions are perceived arguably plays an important role in how one engages in social interactions and how difficulties in establishing smooth social interactions may occur. One aspect of temporal dynamics in social interactions is the mutual coordination of individuals' behaviors during social interaction, otherwise known as behavioral interpersonal synchrony (IPS). Behavioral IPS has been studied increasingly in various contexts, such as a feature of the social interaction difficulties inherent to autism. To fully understand the temporal dynamics of social interactions, or reductions thereof in autism, the neural basis of IPS perception needs to be established. Thus, the current study's aim was twofold: to establish the basic neuro-perceptual processing of IPS in social interactions for typical observers and to test whether it might differ for autistic individuals. In a task-based fMRI paradigm, participants viewed short, silent video vignettes of humans during social interactions featuring a variation of behavioral IPS. The results show that observing behavioral IPS modulates the Action Observation Network (AON). Interestingly, autistic participants showed similar neural activation patterns as non-autistic participants which were modulated by the behavioral IPS they observed in the videos, suggesting that the perception of temporal dynamics of social interactions is spared and may not underly reduced behavioral IPS often observed in autism. Nevertheless, a general difference in processing social interactions was found in autistic observers, characterized by decreased neural activation in the right middle frontal gyrus, angular gyrus, and superior temporal areas. These findings demonstrate that although the autistic and non-autistic groups indeed differed in the neural processing of social interaction perception, the temporal dynamics of these social interactions were not the reason for these differences in social interaction perception in autism. Hence, spared recruitment of the AON for processing temporal dynamics of social interactions in autism does not account for the widely reported attenuation of IPS in autism and for the widely reported and presently observed differences in social interaction perception in autism.
{"title":"Neural Mechanisms of Social Interaction Perception: Observing Interpersonal Synchrony Modulates Action Observation Network Activation and Is Spared in Autism","authors":"Afton M. Bierlich, Nanja T. Scheel, Leora S. Traiger, Daniel Keeser, Ralf Tepest, Alexandra L. Georgescu, Jana C. Koehler, Irene Sophia Plank, Christine M. Falter-Wagner","doi":"10.1002/hbm.70052","DOIUrl":"10.1002/hbm.70052","url":null,"abstract":"<p>How the temporal dynamics of social interactions are perceived arguably plays an important role in how one engages in social interactions and how difficulties in establishing smooth social interactions may occur. One aspect of temporal dynamics in social interactions is the mutual coordination of individuals' behaviors during social interaction, otherwise known as behavioral interpersonal synchrony (IPS). Behavioral IPS has been studied increasingly in various contexts, such as a feature of the social interaction difficulties inherent to autism. To fully understand the temporal dynamics of social interactions, or reductions thereof in autism, the neural basis of IPS perception needs to be established. Thus, the current study's aim was twofold: to establish the basic neuro-perceptual processing of IPS in social interactions for typical observers and to test whether it might differ for autistic individuals. In a task-based fMRI paradigm, participants viewed short, silent video vignettes of humans during social interactions featuring a variation of behavioral IPS. The results show that observing behavioral IPS modulates the Action Observation Network (AON). Interestingly, autistic participants showed similar neural activation patterns as non-autistic participants which were modulated by the behavioral IPS they observed in the videos, suggesting that the perception of temporal dynamics of social interactions is spared and may not underly reduced behavioral IPS often observed in autism. Nevertheless, a general difference in processing social interactions was found in autistic observers, characterized by decreased neural activation in the right middle frontal gyrus, angular gyrus, and superior temporal areas. These findings demonstrate that although the autistic and non-autistic groups indeed differed in the neural processing of social interaction perception, the temporal dynamics of these social interactions were not the reason for these differences in social interaction perception in autism. Hence, spared recruitment of the AON for processing temporal dynamics of social interactions in autism does not account for the widely reported attenuation of IPS in autism and for the widely reported and presently observed differences in social interaction perception in autism.</p>","PeriodicalId":13019,"journal":{"name":"Human Brain Mapping","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11502411/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142499386","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Danqi Gao, Xitong Liang, Qi Ting, Emily Sophia Nichols, Zilin Bai, Chaoying Xu, Mingnan Cai, Li Liu
Despite being a relatively new cultural phenomenon, the ability to perform letter–sound integration is readily acquired even though it has not had time to evolve in the brain. Leading theories of how the brain accommodates literacy acquisition include the neural recycling hypothesis and the assimilation–accommodation hypothesis. The neural recycling hypothesis proposes that a new cultural skill is developed by “invading” preexisting neural structures to support a similar cognitive function, while the assimilation–accommodation hypothesis holds that a new cognitive skill relies on direct invocation of preexisting systems (assimilation) and adds brain areas based on task requirements (accommodation). Both theories agree that letter–sound integration may be achieved by reusing pre-existing functionally similar neural bases, but differ in their proposals of how this occurs. We examined the evidence for each hypothesis by systematically comparing the similarities and differences between letter–sound integration and two other types of preexisting and functionally similar audiovisual (AV) processes, namely object–sound and speech–sound integration, by performing an activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis. All three types of AV integration recruited the left posterior superior temporal gyrus (STG), while speech–sound integration additionally activated the bilateral middle STG and letter–sound integration directly invoked the AV areas involved in speech–sound integration. These findings suggest that letter–sound integration may reuse the STG for speech–sound and object–sound integration through an assimilation–accommodation mechanism.
{"title":"A meta-analysis of letter–sound integration: Assimilation and accommodation in the superior temporal gyrus","authors":"Danqi Gao, Xitong Liang, Qi Ting, Emily Sophia Nichols, Zilin Bai, Chaoying Xu, Mingnan Cai, Li Liu","doi":"10.1002/hbm.26713","DOIUrl":"10.1002/hbm.26713","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Despite being a relatively new cultural phenomenon, the ability to perform letter–sound integration is readily acquired even though it has not had time to evolve in the brain. Leading theories of how the brain accommodates literacy acquisition include the neural recycling hypothesis and the assimilation–accommodation hypothesis. The neural recycling hypothesis proposes that a new cultural skill is developed by “invading” preexisting neural structures to support a similar cognitive function, while the assimilation–accommodation hypothesis holds that a new cognitive skill relies on direct invocation of preexisting systems (assimilation) and adds brain areas based on task requirements (accommodation). Both theories agree that letter–sound integration may be achieved by reusing pre-existing functionally similar neural bases, but differ in their proposals of how this occurs. We examined the evidence for each hypothesis by systematically comparing the similarities and differences between letter–sound integration and two other types of preexisting and functionally similar audiovisual (AV) processes, namely object–sound and speech–sound integration, by performing an activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis. All three types of AV integration recruited the left posterior superior temporal gyrus (STG), while speech–sound integration additionally activated the bilateral middle STG and letter–sound integration directly invoked the AV areas involved in speech–sound integration. These findings suggest that letter–sound integration may reuse the STG for speech–sound and object–sound integration through an assimilation–accommodation mechanism.</p>","PeriodicalId":13019,"journal":{"name":"Human Brain Mapping","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11501095/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142499271","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Dawei Wang, Zhuangzhuang Li, Kun Zhao, Pindong Chen, Fan Yang, Hongxiang Yao, Bo Zhou, Yongbin Wei, Jie Lu, Yuqi Chen, Xi Zhang, Ying Han, Pan Wang, Yong Liu
Macroscale functional gradient techniques provide a continuous coordinate system that extends from unimodal regions to transmodal higher-order networks. However, the alterations of these functional gradients in AD and their correlations with cognitive terms and gene expression profiles remain to be established. In the present study, we directly studied the functional gradients with functional MRI data from seven scanners. We adopted data-driven meta-analytic techniques to unveil AD-associated changes in the functional gradients. The principal primary-to-transmodal gradient was suppressed in AD. Compared to NCs, AD patients exhibited global connectome gradient alterations, including reduced gradient range and gradient variation, increased gradient scores in the somatomotor, ventral attention, and frontoparietal regions, and decreased in the default mode network. More importantly, the Gene Ontology terms of biological processes were significantly enriched in the potassium ion transport and protein-containing complex remodeling. Our compelling evidence provides a new perspective in understanding the connectome alterations in AD.
宏观功能梯度技术提供了一个连续的坐标系,从单模区域延伸到跨模高阶网络。然而,这些功能梯度在 AD 中的改变及其与认知条件和基因表达谱的相关性仍有待确定。在本研究中,我们利用七台扫描仪的功能磁共振成像数据直接研究了这些功能梯度。我们采用数据驱动的元分析技术来揭示与 AD 相关的功能梯度变化。在AD患者中,主要的初级-跨模态梯度受到抑制。与NCs相比,AD患者表现出整体连接组梯度改变,包括梯度范围和梯度变化减小,躯体运动区、腹侧注意区和额顶区的梯度得分增加,默认模式网络的梯度得分降低。更重要的是,生物过程的基因本体术语在钾离子转运和含蛋白复合物重塑中显著富集。我们令人信服的证据为理解AD的连接组改变提供了一个新的视角。
{"title":"Macroscale Gradient Dysfunction in Alzheimer's Disease: Patterns With Cognition Terms and Gene Expression Profiles","authors":"Dawei Wang, Zhuangzhuang Li, Kun Zhao, Pindong Chen, Fan Yang, Hongxiang Yao, Bo Zhou, Yongbin Wei, Jie Lu, Yuqi Chen, Xi Zhang, Ying Han, Pan Wang, Yong Liu","doi":"10.1002/hbm.70046","DOIUrl":"10.1002/hbm.70046","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Macroscale functional gradient techniques provide a continuous coordinate system that extends from unimodal regions to transmodal higher-order networks. However, the alterations of these functional gradients in AD and their correlations with cognitive terms and gene expression profiles remain to be established. In the present study, we directly studied the functional gradients with functional MRI data from seven scanners. We adopted data-driven meta-analytic techniques to unveil AD-associated changes in the functional gradients. The principal primary-to-transmodal gradient was suppressed in AD. Compared to NCs, AD patients exhibited global connectome gradient alterations, including reduced gradient range and gradient variation, increased gradient scores in the somatomotor, ventral attention, and frontoparietal regions, and decreased in the default mode network. More importantly, the Gene Ontology terms of biological processes were significantly enriched in the potassium ion transport and protein-containing complex remodeling. Our compelling evidence provides a new perspective in understanding the connectome alterations in AD.</p>","PeriodicalId":13019,"journal":{"name":"Human Brain Mapping","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11502409/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142499275","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
There is still little research on the consistency among the subcortical volume estimates of different software packages. It is also unclear whether there are age-related differences in the inter-software consistency. The current study aimed to examine the consistency of three commonly used automated software packages and the effect of age on inter-software consistency. We analyzed T1-weighted structural images from two public datasets, in which the subjects were divided into four age groups ranging from childhood and adolescence to late adulthood. We chose three mainstream automated software packages including FreeSurfer, CAT, and FSL, to estimate the volumes of seven subcortical structures, including thalamus, caudate, putamen, pallidum, hippocampus, amygdala, and accumbens. We used the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) and Pearson correlation coefficient (PCC) to quantify inter-software consistency and compared the consistency measures among the age groups. As a measure of validity, we additionally evaluated the predictive power of each software package's estimates for predicting age. The results showed good inter-software consistency in the thalamus, caudate, putamen, and hippocampus, moderate consistency in the pallidum, and poor consistency in the amygdala and accumbens. Significant differences in the inter-software consistency were not observed among the age groups in most cases. FreeSurfer exhibited higher age prediction accuracy than CAT and FSL. The current study showed that the inter-software consistency on the subcortical volume estimation varies with structures but generally not with age groups, which has important implications for the interpretation and reproducibility of neuroimaging findings.
{"title":"Inter-Software Consistency on the Estimation of Subcortical Structure Volume in Different Age Groups","authors":"Lei Zhang, Yang Hu","doi":"10.1002/hbm.70055","DOIUrl":"10.1002/hbm.70055","url":null,"abstract":"<p>There is still little research on the consistency among the subcortical volume estimates of different software packages. It is also unclear whether there are age-related differences in the inter-software consistency. The current study aimed to examine the consistency of three commonly used automated software packages and the effect of age on inter-software consistency. We analyzed T1-weighted structural images from two public datasets, in which the subjects were divided into four age groups ranging from childhood and adolescence to late adulthood. We chose three mainstream automated software packages including FreeSurfer, CAT, and FSL, to estimate the volumes of seven subcortical structures, including thalamus, caudate, putamen, pallidum, hippocampus, amygdala, and accumbens. We used the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) and Pearson correlation coefficient (PCC) to quantify inter-software consistency and compared the consistency measures among the age groups. As a measure of validity, we additionally evaluated the predictive power of each software package's estimates for predicting age. The results showed good inter-software consistency in the thalamus, caudate, putamen, and hippocampus, moderate consistency in the pallidum, and poor consistency in the amygdala and accumbens. Significant differences in the inter-software consistency were not observed among the age groups in most cases. FreeSurfer exhibited higher age prediction accuracy than CAT and FSL. The current study showed that the inter-software consistency on the subcortical volume estimation varies with structures but generally not with age groups, which has important implications for the interpretation and reproducibility of neuroimaging findings.</p>","PeriodicalId":13019,"journal":{"name":"Human Brain Mapping","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-10-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11496994/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142499274","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Edmund T. Rolls, Ruohan Zhang, Gustavo Deco, Deniz Vatansever, Jianfeng Feng
Different cortical systems to the hippocampus were activated using fMRI during different types of episodic memory task. For object with scene location episodic memory, the activations were high in cortical systems involved in spatial processing, including the ventromedial visual and medial parahippocampal system. These activations for the medial parahippocampal system were higher in the right hemisphere. The activations in the face and object processing ventrolateral visual cortical stream regions FFC, PIT, V8 and TE2p were higher in the object-location in scene task than the reward-location task, and were higher in the right hemisphere. For reward-location in scene episodic memory, activations were also high in the ventromedial visual cortical spatial stream to the hippocampus, but were also selectively high in storage in key reward cortical regions (ventromedial prefrontal 10r, 10v, 10d; pregenual anterior cingulate d32, p24, p32, s32; and medial orbitofrontal cortex reward-related pOFC, 11l, OFC). For word-pair episodic memory, activations were lower in the ventromedial visual and medial parahippocampal spatial cortical stream, and were higher in language-related regions in Broca's area (44, 45, 47l), and were higher in the left hemisphere for these regions and for the many highly connected inferior frontal gyrus regions in the left hemisphere. Further, effective connectivity analyses during the episodic memory tasks showed that the direction of connectivity for these systems was from early visual cortical regions V2–V4 to the ventromedial visual cortical regions VMV1–3 and VVC for spatial scene processing; was from the pregenual anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex reward systems to the hippocampal system; and was from the FFC/V8/PIT system to TE2p in the visual inferior temporal visual cortex, which has connectivity to lateral parahippocampal TF, which in turn has forward effective connectivity to the hippocampus.
{"title":"Selective Brain Activations and Connectivities Related to the Storage and Recall of Human Object-Location, Reward-Location, and Word-Pair Episodic Memories","authors":"Edmund T. Rolls, Ruohan Zhang, Gustavo Deco, Deniz Vatansever, Jianfeng Feng","doi":"10.1002/hbm.70056","DOIUrl":"10.1002/hbm.70056","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Different cortical systems to the hippocampus were activated using fMRI during different types of episodic memory task. For object with scene location episodic memory, the activations were high in cortical systems involved in spatial processing, including the ventromedial visual and medial parahippocampal system. These activations for the medial parahippocampal system were higher in the right hemisphere. The activations in the face and object processing ventrolateral visual cortical stream regions FFC, PIT, V8 and TE2p were higher in the object-location in scene task than the reward-location task, and were higher in the right hemisphere. For reward-location in scene episodic memory, activations were also high in the ventromedial visual cortical spatial stream to the hippocampus, but were also selectively high in storage in key reward cortical regions (ventromedial prefrontal 10r, 10v, 10d; pregenual anterior cingulate d32, p24, p32, s32; and medial orbitofrontal cortex reward-related pOFC, 11l, OFC). For word-pair episodic memory, activations were lower in the ventromedial visual and medial parahippocampal spatial cortical stream, and were higher in language-related regions in Broca's area (44, 45, 47l), and were higher in the left hemisphere for these regions and for the many highly connected inferior frontal gyrus regions in the left hemisphere. Further, effective connectivity analyses during the episodic memory tasks showed that the direction of connectivity for these systems was from early visual cortical regions V2–V4 to the ventromedial visual cortical regions VMV1–3 and VVC for spatial scene processing; was from the pregenual anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex reward systems to the hippocampal system; and was from the FFC/V8/PIT system to TE2p in the visual inferior temporal visual cortex, which has connectivity to lateral parahippocampal TF, which in turn has forward effective connectivity to the hippocampus.</p>","PeriodicalId":13019,"journal":{"name":"Human Brain Mapping","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-10-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11494686/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142464100","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
In temporomandibular disorder (TMD), the effects of standard interventions such as using an occlusal splint and its impact on pain relief and pain catastrophizing are poorly understood. Earlier work pointed to a crucial role of insula activation with changes in pain relief by occlusal splint treatment. We performed a functional imaging study using specially developed splint systems to allow for a placebo-controlled longitudinal design. Using functional MRI we examined 20 TMD patients during repetitive occlusal movements at baseline and over the course of splint therapy and also collected self-reported pain catastrophizing. For balancing performance between baseline and after intervention we used occlusion force measures in an individualized fMRI-splint system. Splint therapy lasted for approximately 7 weeks with one group selected by randomization wearing a palatine placebo splint over the first 3 weeks (delayed start; 11 individuals). As expected, fMRI activation in areas involved in pain processing (insula, primary and secondary somatosensory cortex) decreased with intervention. At baseline a positive correlation between activation of the left anterior insula and pain catastrophizing was present. Both parameters decreased over intervention while associations were primarily observable for patients with rather mild TMD.
{"title":"Pain Catastrophizing and Functional Activation During Occlusion in TMD Patients—An Interventional Study","authors":"K. Klepzig, M. Domin, B. Kordass, M. Lotze","doi":"10.1002/hbm.70051","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.70051","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In temporomandibular disorder (TMD), the effects of standard interventions such as using an occlusal splint and its impact on pain relief and pain catastrophizing are poorly understood. Earlier work pointed to a crucial role of insula activation with changes in pain relief by occlusal splint treatment. We performed a functional imaging study using specially developed splint systems to allow for a placebo-controlled longitudinal design. Using functional MRI we examined 20 TMD patients during repetitive occlusal movements at baseline and over the course of splint therapy and also collected self-reported pain catastrophizing. For balancing performance between baseline and after intervention we used occlusion force measures in an individualized fMRI-splint system. Splint therapy lasted for approximately 7 weeks with one group selected by randomization wearing a palatine placebo splint over the first 3 weeks (delayed start; 11 individuals). As expected, fMRI activation in areas involved in pain processing (insula, primary and secondary somatosensory cortex) decreased with intervention. At baseline a positive correlation between activation of the left anterior insula and pain catastrophizing was present. Both parameters decreased over intervention while associations were primarily observable for patients with rather mild TMD.</p>","PeriodicalId":13019,"journal":{"name":"Human Brain Mapping","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-10-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/hbm.70051","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142449150","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
R. N. Henson, W. Olszowy, K. A. Tsvetanov, P. S. Yadav, Cam-CAN, P. Zeidman
Neural activity cannot be directly observed using fMRI; rather it must be inferred from the hemodynamic responses that neural activity causes. Solving this inverse problem is made possible through the use of forward models, which generate predicted hemodynamic responses given hypothesised underlying neural activity. Commonly-used hemodynamic models were developed to explain data from healthy young participants; however, studies of ageing and dementia are increasingly shifting the focus toward elderly populations. We evaluated the validity of a range of hemodynamic models across the healthy adult lifespan: from basis sets for the linear convolution models commonly used to analyse fMRI studies, to more advanced models including nonlinear fitting of a parameterised hemodynamic response function (HRF) and nonlinear fitting of a biophysical generative model (hemodynamic modelling, HDM). Using an exceptionally large sample of participants, and a sensorimotor task optimized for detecting the shape of the BOLD response to brief stimulation, we first characterised the effects of age on descriptive features of the response (e.g., peak amplitude and latency). We then compared these to features from more complex nonlinear models, fit to four regions of interest engaged by the task, namely left auditory cortex, bilateral visual cortex, left (contralateral) motor cortex and right (ipsilateral) motor cortex. Finally, we validated the extent to which parameter estimates from these models have predictive validity, in terms of how well they predict age in cross-validated multiple regression. We conclude that age-related differences in the BOLD response can be captured effectively by models with three free parameters. Furthermore, we show that biophysical models like the HDM have predictive validity comparable to more common models, while additionally providing insights into underlying mechanisms, which go beyond descriptive features like peak amplitude or latency, and include estimation of nonlinear effects. Here, the HDM revealed that most of the effects of age on the BOLD response could be explained by an increased rate of vasoactive signal decay and decreased transit rate of blood, rather than changes in neural activity per se. However, in the absence of other types of neural/hemodynamic data, unique interpretation of HDM parameters is difficult from fMRI data alone, and some brain regions in some tasks (e.g., ipsilateral motor cortex) can show responses that are more difficult to capture using current models.
{"title":"Evaluating Models of the Ageing BOLD Response","authors":"R. N. Henson, W. Olszowy, K. A. Tsvetanov, P. S. Yadav, Cam-CAN, P. Zeidman","doi":"10.1002/hbm.70043","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.70043","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Neural activity cannot be directly observed using fMRI; rather it must be inferred from the hemodynamic responses that neural activity causes. Solving this inverse problem is made possible through the use of forward models, which generate predicted hemodynamic responses given hypothesised underlying neural activity. Commonly-used hemodynamic models were developed to explain data from healthy young participants; however, studies of ageing and dementia are increasingly shifting the focus toward elderly populations. We evaluated the validity of a range of hemodynamic models across the healthy adult lifespan: from basis sets for the linear convolution models commonly used to analyse fMRI studies, to more advanced models including nonlinear fitting of a parameterised hemodynamic response function (HRF) and nonlinear fitting of a biophysical generative model (hemodynamic modelling, HDM). Using an exceptionally large sample of participants, and a sensorimotor task optimized for detecting the shape of the BOLD response to brief stimulation, we first characterised the effects of age on descriptive features of the response (e.g., peak amplitude and latency). We then compared these to features from more complex nonlinear models, fit to four regions of interest engaged by the task, namely left auditory cortex, bilateral visual cortex, left (contralateral) motor cortex and right (ipsilateral) motor cortex. Finally, we validated the extent to which parameter estimates from these models have predictive validity, in terms of how well they predict age in cross-validated multiple regression. We conclude that age-related differences in the BOLD response can be captured effectively by models with three free parameters. Furthermore, we show that biophysical models like the HDM have predictive validity comparable to more common models, while additionally providing insights into underlying mechanisms, which go beyond descriptive features like peak amplitude or latency, and include estimation of nonlinear effects. Here, the HDM revealed that most of the effects of age on the BOLD response could be explained by an increased rate of vasoactive signal decay and decreased transit rate of blood, rather than changes in neural activity per se. However, in the absence of other types of neural/hemodynamic data, unique interpretation of HDM parameters is difficult from fMRI data alone, and some brain regions in some tasks (e.g., ipsilateral motor cortex) can show responses that are more difficult to capture using current models.</p>","PeriodicalId":13019,"journal":{"name":"Human Brain Mapping","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/hbm.70043","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142451291","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Jil Wendt, Antonia Neubauer, Dennis M. Hedderich, Benita Schmitz-Koep, Sevilay Ayyildiz, David Schinz, Rebecca Hippen, Marcel Daamen, Henning Boecker, Claus Zimmer, Dieter Wolke, Peter Bartmann, Christian Sorg, Aurore Menegaux
Despite substantial neuroscience research in the last decade revealing the claustrum's prominent role in mammalian forebrain organization, as evidenced by its extraordinarily widespread connectivity pattern, claustrum studies in humans are rare. This is particularly true for studies focusing on claustrum connections. Two primary reasons may account for this situation: First, the intricate anatomy of the human claustrum located between the external and extreme capsule hinders straightforward and reliable structural delineation. In addition, the few studies that used diffusion-weighted-imaging (DWI)-based tractography could not clarify whether in vivo tractography consistently and reliably identifies claustrum connections in humans across different subjects, cohorts, imaging methods, and connectivity metrics. To address these issues, we combined a recently developed deep-learning-based claustrum segmentation tool with DWI-based tractography in two large adult cohorts: 81 healthy young adults from the human connectome project and 81 further healthy young participants from the Bavarian longitudinal study. Tracts between the claustrum and 13 cortical and 9 subcortical regions were reconstructed in each subject using probabilistic tractography. Probabilistic group average maps and different connectivity metrics were generated to assess the claustrum's connectivity profile as well as consistency and replicability of tractography. We found, across individuals, cohorts, DWI-protocols, and measures, consistent and replicable cortical and subcortical ipsi- and contralateral claustrum connections. This result demonstrates robust in vivo tractography of claustrum connections in humans, providing a base for further examinations of claustrum connectivity in health and disease.
{"title":"Human Claustrum Connections: Robust In Vivo Detection by DWI-Based Tractography in Two Large Samples","authors":"Jil Wendt, Antonia Neubauer, Dennis M. Hedderich, Benita Schmitz-Koep, Sevilay Ayyildiz, David Schinz, Rebecca Hippen, Marcel Daamen, Henning Boecker, Claus Zimmer, Dieter Wolke, Peter Bartmann, Christian Sorg, Aurore Menegaux","doi":"10.1002/hbm.70042","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.70042","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Despite substantial neuroscience research in the last decade revealing the claustrum's prominent role in mammalian forebrain organization, as evidenced by its extraordinarily widespread connectivity pattern, claustrum studies in humans are rare. This is particularly true for studies focusing on claustrum connections. Two primary reasons may account for this situation: First, the intricate anatomy of the human claustrum located between the external and extreme capsule hinders straightforward and reliable structural delineation. In addition, the few studies that used diffusion-weighted-imaging (DWI)-based tractography could not clarify whether in vivo tractography consistently and reliably identifies claustrum connections in humans across different subjects, cohorts, imaging methods, and connectivity metrics. To address these issues, we combined a recently developed deep-learning-based claustrum segmentation tool with DWI-based tractography in two large adult cohorts: 81 healthy young adults from the human connectome project and 81 further healthy young participants from the Bavarian longitudinal study. Tracts between the claustrum and 13 cortical and 9 subcortical regions were reconstructed in each subject using probabilistic tractography. Probabilistic group average maps and different connectivity metrics were generated to assess the claustrum's connectivity profile as well as consistency and replicability of tractography. We found, across individuals, cohorts, DWI-protocols, and measures, consistent and replicable cortical and subcortical ipsi- and contralateral claustrum connections. This result demonstrates robust in vivo tractography of claustrum connections in humans, providing a base for further examinations of claustrum connectivity in health and disease.</p>","PeriodicalId":13019,"journal":{"name":"Human Brain Mapping","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-10-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/hbm.70042","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142435509","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}