首页 > 最新文献

Autism Research最新文献

英文 中文
Characterizing social communication among minimally verbal children with autism: An application of item response theory 描述自闭症儿童中言语表达能力较弱者的社会交往特点:项目反应理论的应用。
IF 5.3 2区 医学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-16 DOI: 10.1002/aur.3218
Andrew Schlink, Connie Kasari

Minimally verbal children constitute a portion of the autism spectrum. The paucity of proper measurement tools that sensitively and accurately assess behaviors has been one limiting factor in the improved knowledge of these children. Short of creating and validating a new measurement tool for this subpopulation, this study took an alternative and more immediate approach: conduct a secondary data analysis and examine an existing social communication measure, the Early Social Communication Scales (ESCS), with item response theory. The final sample consisted of 453 minimally verbal children culled from four different completed studies. The IRT models analyzed the frequency of social communication gestures from the ESCS and returned an objective difficulty hierarchy regarding initiations of joint attention and behavior regulation gestures. The best-fitting and final model was a zero-inflated negative binomial model (ZINBM), which determined that joint attention gestures were, on average, more difficult than behavior regulation gestures. Joint attentional shows and gives were essentially absent in the children's repertoire, and behavior regulation reaches were the easiest gestures for this sample. The ZINBM separately modeled children with some gestures and children who did not present with any gestures and determined that behavior regulation reaches and gives were likely the first gestures a child will eventually exhibit among children with no gestures. Methodological contributions and potential future applications of IRT are discussed.

自闭症谱系中有一部分儿童是言语表达能力低下的。缺乏适当的测量工具来敏感而准确地评估这些儿童的行为,一直是阻碍人们加深对这些儿童了解的因素之一。本研究没有为这一亚群体创建和验证新的测量工具,而是采取了另一种更直接的方法:进行二次数据分析,并利用项目反应理论检查现有的社会交流测量工具--早期社会交流量表(ESCS)。最终样本包括从四项已完成的研究中筛选出的 453 名言语能力较弱的儿童。IRT 模型分析了 ESCS 中社会交流手势的频率,并返回了有关发起共同注意和行为调节手势的客观难度等级。最终得出的最佳拟合模型是零膨胀负二项模型(ZINBM),该模型确定了共同注意手势的平均难度高于行为调节手势。在儿童的手势中基本上没有共同注意的表演和给予,而行为调节手势是该样本中最简单的手势。ZINBM 分别对有一些手势的儿童和没有任何手势的儿童进行了建模,并确定在没有任何手势的儿童中,行为调节的 "伸 "和 "给 "很可能是儿童最终会表现出的第一种手势。本文讨论了 IRT 在方法论上的贡献和未来的潜在应用。
{"title":"Characterizing social communication among minimally verbal children with autism: An application of item response theory","authors":"Andrew Schlink,&nbsp;Connie Kasari","doi":"10.1002/aur.3218","DOIUrl":"10.1002/aur.3218","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Minimally verbal children constitute a portion of the autism spectrum. The paucity of proper measurement tools that sensitively and accurately assess behaviors has been one limiting factor in the improved knowledge of these children. Short of creating and validating a new measurement tool for this subpopulation, this study took an alternative and more immediate approach: conduct a secondary data analysis and examine an existing social communication measure, the Early Social Communication Scales (ESCS), with item response theory. The final sample consisted of 453 minimally verbal children culled from four different completed studies. The IRT models analyzed the frequency of social communication gestures from the ESCS and returned an objective difficulty hierarchy regarding initiations of joint attention and behavior regulation gestures. The best-fitting and final model was a zero-inflated negative binomial model (ZINBM), which determined that joint attention gestures were, on average, more difficult than behavior regulation gestures. Joint attentional shows and gives were essentially absent in the children's repertoire, and behavior regulation reaches were the easiest gestures for this sample. The ZINBM separately modeled children with some gestures and children who did not present with any gestures and determined that behavior regulation reaches and gives were likely the first gestures a child will eventually exhibit among children with no gestures. Methodological contributions and potential future applications of IRT are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":131,"journal":{"name":"Autism Research","volume":"17 9","pages":"1830-1843"},"PeriodicalIF":5.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141997003","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of volatility in linguistic input on prediction behavior in autistic toddlers 语言输入的不稳定性对自闭症幼儿预测行为的影响。
IF 5.3 2区 医学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-11 DOI: 10.1002/aur.3212
Kathryn E. Prescott, Janine Mathée-Scott, Daniel Bolt, Jenny Saffran, Susan Ellis Weismer

Domain-general prediction differences have been posited as underlying many aspects of the cognitive-behavioral profile in autism. An interesting potential implication of such differences is hyperplasticity of learning—the idea that autistic individuals may privilege more recent input over the accumulation of prior learning. Because real world language input is highly variable, hyperplasticity could have serious ramifications for language learning. To investigate potential hyperplasticity during a language processing task, we administered an experimental anticipatory eye movement (AEM) task to 2- to 3-year-old autistic children and neurotypical (NT) peers. Autistic children's change in anticipation from before to after a switch in contingencies did not significantly differ from NT counterparts, failing to support claims of hyperplasticity in the linguistic domain. Analysis of individual differences among autistic children revealed that cognitive ability was associated with prediction of the initial, stable contingencies, but neither age nor receptive language related to task performance. Results are discussed in terms of clinical implications and the broader context of research investigating prediction differences in autism.

一般领域的预测差异被认为是自闭症认知行为特征许多方面的基础。这种差异的一个有趣的潜在影响是学习的过度可塑性--自闭症患者可能会优先选择最近的输入,而不是先前的学习积累。由于现实世界的语言输入变化很大,超可塑性可能会对语言学习产生严重影响。为了研究语言处理任务中潜在的超可塑性,我们对 2 到 3 岁的自闭症儿童和神经畸形(NT)儿童进行了一项实验性预期眼动(AEM)任务。自闭症儿童与神经正常儿童的预期从应急转换前到转换后的变化没有显著差异,因此不能支持语言领域超可塑性的说法。对自闭症儿童个体差异的分析表明,认知能力与对初始、稳定情景的预测有关,但年龄和接受性语言都与任务表现无关。本文从临床意义和自闭症预测差异研究的更广泛背景两个方面对研究结果进行了讨论。
{"title":"The effect of volatility in linguistic input on prediction behavior in autistic toddlers","authors":"Kathryn E. Prescott,&nbsp;Janine Mathée-Scott,&nbsp;Daniel Bolt,&nbsp;Jenny Saffran,&nbsp;Susan Ellis Weismer","doi":"10.1002/aur.3212","DOIUrl":"10.1002/aur.3212","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Domain-general prediction differences have been posited as underlying many aspects of the cognitive-behavioral profile in autism. An interesting potential implication of such differences is hyperplasticity of learning—the idea that autistic individuals may privilege more recent input over the accumulation of prior learning. Because real world language input is highly variable, hyperplasticity could have serious ramifications for language learning. To investigate potential hyperplasticity during a language processing task, we administered an experimental anticipatory eye movement (AEM) task to 2- to 3-year-old autistic children and neurotypical (NT) peers. Autistic children's change in anticipation from before to after a switch in contingencies did not significantly differ from NT counterparts, failing to support claims of hyperplasticity in the linguistic domain. Analysis of individual differences among autistic children revealed that cognitive ability was associated with prediction of the initial, stable contingencies, but neither age nor receptive language related to task performance. Results are discussed in terms of clinical implications and the broader context of research investigating prediction differences in autism.</p>","PeriodicalId":131,"journal":{"name":"Autism Research","volume":"17 11","pages":"2305-2318"},"PeriodicalIF":5.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11568938/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141918222","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Metaphor comprehension and production in verbally able children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 自闭症谱系障碍儿童对隐喻的理解和创作。
IF 5.3 2区 医学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-09 DOI: 10.1002/aur.3210
Stella Lampri, Eleni Peristeri, Theodoros Marinis, Maria Andreou

Research in the field of figurative language processing in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) has demonstrated that autistic individuals experience systematic difficulties in the comprehension of different types of metaphors. However, there is scarce evidence regarding metaphor production skills in ASD. Importantly, the exact source of metaphor processing difficulties in ASD remains largely controversial. The debate has mainly focused on the mediating role of structural language skills (i.e., lexical knowledge) and cognitive abilities (i.e., Theory of Mind and executive functions) in ASD individuals' ability to comprehend and generate metaphors. The present study examines metaphor comprehension and production in 18 Greek-speaking verbally able children with ASD and 31 typically-developing (TD) controls. Participants completed two tasks, namely, a low-verbal multiple-choice sentence-picture matching task that tested their ability to comprehend conventional predicate metaphors, and a sentence continuation task that assessed their ability to generate metaphors. The study also included measures of fluid intelligence, expressive vocabulary, and working memory within the sample. The results show that the ASD group had significantly lower performance than the TD group in both metaphor comprehension and production. The findings also reveal that expressive vocabulary skills were a key factor in the metaphor comprehension and production performance of the children with ASD. Working memory capacity was also found to correlate significantly with metaphor comprehension performance in the ASD group. Conversely, no correlations were found in the TD group with neither of the above factors. Of note, children with ASD generated significantly more inappropriate responses and no-responses to the metaphor production task compared with the control group. The overall results reveal that children with ASD had difficulty with both comprehending and using metaphorical language. The findings also indicate that TD children may employ diverse cognitive strategies or rely on different underlying skills when processing metaphors compared with children with ASD.

自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)中形象语言处理领域的研究表明,自闭症患者在理解不同类型的隐喻时会遇到系统性的困难。然而,有关自闭症患者隐喻产生技能的证据却很少。重要的是,自闭症患者隐喻加工困难的确切来源在很大程度上仍存在争议。争论的焦点主要集中在结构性语言技能(即词汇知识)和认知能力(即心智理论和执行功能)在 ASD 患者理解和产生隐喻的能力中所起的中介作用。本研究考察了 18 名希腊语言语能力强的 ASD 儿童和 31 名发育正常 (TD) 对照组儿童的隐喻理解和生成能力。受试者完成了两项任务,即测试他们理解传统谓词隐喻能力的低语言多选句子-图片匹配任务,以及评估他们生成隐喻能力的续句任务。研究还对样本中的流体智力、词汇表达能力和工作记忆进行了测量。结果显示,ASD 组在隐喻理解和生成方面的表现明显低于 TD 组。研究结果还显示,词汇表达能力是影响 ASD 儿童隐喻理解和创作能力的关键因素。研究还发现,工作记忆能力与 ASD 组的隐喻理解能力有显著相关性。相反,在 TD 组中,上述两个因素都没有相关性。值得注意的是,与对照组相比,ASD 儿童在隐喻制作任务中产生的不恰当反应和无反应明显更多。总体结果显示,患有 ASD 的儿童在理解和使用隐喻语言方面都存在困难。研究结果还表明,与 ASD 儿童相比,TD 儿童在处理隐喻时可能会采用不同的认知策略或依赖不同的基本技能。
{"title":"Metaphor comprehension and production in verbally able children with Autism Spectrum Disorder","authors":"Stella Lampri,&nbsp;Eleni Peristeri,&nbsp;Theodoros Marinis,&nbsp;Maria Andreou","doi":"10.1002/aur.3210","DOIUrl":"10.1002/aur.3210","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Research in the field of figurative language processing in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) has demonstrated that autistic individuals experience systematic difficulties in the comprehension of different types of metaphors. However, there is scarce evidence regarding metaphor production skills in ASD. Importantly, the exact source of metaphor processing difficulties in ASD remains largely controversial. The debate has mainly focused on the mediating role of structural language skills (i.e., lexical knowledge) and cognitive abilities (i.e., Theory of Mind and executive functions) in ASD individuals' ability to comprehend and generate metaphors. The present study examines metaphor comprehension and production in 18 Greek-speaking verbally able children with ASD and 31 typically-developing (TD) controls. Participants completed two tasks, namely, a low-verbal multiple-choice sentence-picture matching task that tested their ability to comprehend conventional predicate metaphors, and a sentence continuation task that assessed their ability to generate metaphors. The study also included measures of fluid intelligence, expressive vocabulary, and working memory within the sample. The results show that the ASD group had significantly lower performance than the TD group in both metaphor comprehension and production. The findings also reveal that expressive vocabulary skills were a key factor in the metaphor comprehension and production performance of the children with ASD. Working memory capacity was also found to correlate significantly with metaphor comprehension performance in the ASD group. Conversely, no correlations were found in the TD group with neither of the above factors. Of note, children with ASD generated significantly more inappropriate responses and no-responses to the metaphor production task compared with the control group. The overall results reveal that children with ASD had difficulty with both comprehending and using metaphorical language. The findings also indicate that TD children may employ diverse cognitive strategies or rely on different underlying skills when processing metaphors compared with children with ASD.</p>","PeriodicalId":131,"journal":{"name":"Autism Research","volume":"17 11","pages":"2292-2304"},"PeriodicalIF":5.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/aur.3210","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141914764","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Breastfeeding patterns in infants are associated with a later diagnosis of autism Spectrum disorder 婴儿的母乳喂养模式与日后自闭症谱系障碍的诊断有关。
IF 5.3 2区 医学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-08 DOI: 10.1002/aur.3211
Leena Elbedour, Rewaa Balaum, Einav Alhozyel, Gal Meiri, Dikla Zigdon, Analya Michaelovski, Orly Kerub, Idan Menashe

Breastfeeding is associated with medical and developmental benefits. This study aimed to assess associations between nutritional patterns in the first year of life and the likelihood of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). 270 children diagnosed with ASD (cases) and 500 neurotypical children (controls) matched to cases by sex, ethnicity, and birth date (± 3 months) were included in this retrospective case–control study. Both groups were ascertained from children born between 2014 and 2017 whose development/nutrition were monitored at mother–child health clinics in southern Israel. Conditional logistic regression was used to determine the independent association of nutritional patterns with ASD while adjusting for socio-demographic and clinical characteristics. Both exclusive and partial breastfeeding modes were associated with decreased odds of ASD diagnosis (aOR = 0.221, 95%CI = 0.136–0.360; aOR = 0.494, 95%CI = 0.328–0.743, respectively). A breastfeeding duration of >12 months was associated with lower ASD odds (aOR = 0.418, 95%CI = 0.204–0.855), while the introduction of solids after 6 months of age was associated with higher ASD odds than the introduction of solids at 6 months (aOR = 2.455, 95%CI = 1.116–4.201). These findings suggest that a longer period of exclusive breastfeeding is associated with a subsequent reduced likelihood of ASD diagnosis, thus reiterating the importance of proper post-natal nutrition for infant neurodevelopment.

母乳喂养具有医疗和发育方面的益处。本研究旨在评估婴儿出生后第一年的营养模式与自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)可能性之间的关系。这项回顾性病例对照研究纳入了 270 名被诊断患有自闭症谱系障碍的儿童(病例)和 500 名与病例在性别、种族和出生日期(± 3 个月)方面匹配的神经正常儿童(对照组)。这两组儿童均来自 2014 年至 2017 年间出生的儿童,以色列南部的母婴健康诊所对他们的发育/营养状况进行了监测。研究采用条件逻辑回归法确定营养模式与 ASD 的独立关联,同时对社会人口学和临床特征进行调整。纯母乳喂养和部分母乳喂养模式均与 ASD 诊断几率下降有关(aOR = 0.221,95%CI = 0.136-0.360; aOR = 0.494,95%CI = 0.328-0.743)。母乳喂养时间超过12个月与较低的ASD几率相关(aOR = 0.418,95%CI = 0.204-0.855),而6个月后开始喂养固体食物比6个月开始喂养固体食物与较高的ASD几率相关(aOR = 2.455,95%CI = 1.116-4.201)。这些研究结果表明,较长时间的纯母乳喂养与随后ASD诊断几率的降低有关,从而重申了产后适当营养对婴儿神经发育的重要性。
{"title":"Breastfeeding patterns in infants are associated with a later diagnosis of autism Spectrum disorder","authors":"Leena Elbedour,&nbsp;Rewaa Balaum,&nbsp;Einav Alhozyel,&nbsp;Gal Meiri,&nbsp;Dikla Zigdon,&nbsp;Analya Michaelovski,&nbsp;Orly Kerub,&nbsp;Idan Menashe","doi":"10.1002/aur.3211","DOIUrl":"10.1002/aur.3211","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Breastfeeding is associated with medical and developmental benefits. This study aimed to assess associations between nutritional patterns in the first year of life and the likelihood of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). 270 children diagnosed with ASD (cases) and 500 neurotypical children (controls) matched to cases by sex, ethnicity, and birth date (± 3 months) were included in this retrospective case–control study. Both groups were ascertained from children born between 2014 and 2017 whose development/nutrition were monitored at mother–child health clinics in southern Israel. Conditional logistic regression was used to determine the independent association of nutritional patterns with ASD while adjusting for socio-demographic and clinical characteristics. Both exclusive and partial breastfeeding modes were associated with decreased odds of ASD diagnosis (aOR = 0.221, 95%CI = 0.136–0.360; aOR = 0.494, 95%CI = 0.328–0.743, respectively). A breastfeeding duration of &gt;12 months was associated with lower ASD odds (aOR = 0.418, 95%CI = 0.204–0.855), while the introduction of solids after 6 months of age was associated with higher ASD odds than the introduction of solids at 6 months (aOR = 2.455, 95%CI = 1.116–4.201). These findings suggest that a longer period of exclusive breastfeeding is associated with a subsequent reduced likelihood of ASD diagnosis, thus reiterating the importance of proper post-natal nutrition for infant neurodevelopment.</p>","PeriodicalId":131,"journal":{"name":"Autism Research","volume":"17 8","pages":"1696-1704"},"PeriodicalIF":5.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/aur.3211","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141903750","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Higher or lower? Interpersonal behavioral and neural synchronization of movement imitation in autistic children 更高还是更低?自闭症儿童动作模仿的人际行为和神经同步性
IF 5.3 2区 医学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-08 DOI: 10.1002/aur.3205
Bang Du, Wenjun Zhang, Liu Chen, Xiaorui Deng, Kaiyun Li, Fengxun Lin, Fanlu Jia, Shuhua Su, Wanzhi Tang

How well autistic children can imitate movements and how their brain activity synchronizes with the person they are imitating have been understudied. The current study adopted functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) hyperscanning and employed a task involving real interactions involving meaningful and meaningless movement imitation to explore the fundamental nature of imitation as a dynamic and interactive process. Experiment 1 explored meaningful and meaningless gesture imitation. The results revealed that autistic children exhibited lower imitation accuracy and behavioral synchrony than non-autistic children when imitating both meaningful and meaningless gestures. Specifically, compared to non-autistic children, autistic children displayed significantly higher interpersonal neural synchronization (INS) in the right inferior parietal lobule (r-IPL) (channel 12) when imitating meaningful gestures but lower INS when imitating meaningless gestures. Experiment 2 further investigated the imitation of four types of meaningless movements (orofacial movements, transitive movements, limb movements, and gestures). The results revealed that across all four movement types, autistic children exhibited significantly lower imitation accuracy, behavioral synchrony, and INS in the r-IPL (channel 12) than non-autistic children. This study is the first to identify INS as a biomarker of movement imitation difficulties in autistic individuals. Furthermore, an intra- and interindividual imitation mechanism model was proposed to explain the underlying causes of movement imitation difficulties in autistic individuals.

自闭症儿童的动作模仿能力如何,以及他们的大脑活动如何与模仿对象同步,这些问题一直未得到充分研究。本研究采用了功能性近红外光谱(fNIRS)超扫描技术,并使用了涉及有意义和无意义动作模仿的真实互动任务,以探索模仿作为动态互动过程的基本性质。实验 1 探讨了有意义和无意义的手势模仿。结果显示,在模仿有意义和无意义手势时,自闭症儿童的模仿准确性和行为同步性均低于非自闭症儿童。具体而言,与非自闭症儿童相比,自闭症儿童在模仿有意义手势时,右侧顶叶下叶(r-IPL)(通道 12)的人际神经同步性(INS)明显较高,但在模仿无意义手势时,INS 则较低。实验 2 进一步研究了四种无意义动作(口部动作、转体动作、肢体动作和手势)的模仿。结果发现,在所有四种动作类型中,自闭症儿童的模仿准确性、行为同步性和 r-IPL(通道 12)的 INS 都明显低于非自闭症儿童。本研究首次发现 INS 是自闭症儿童动作模仿困难的生物标记。此外,该研究还提出了个体内和个体间模仿机制模型,以解释自闭症儿童动作模仿困难的根本原因。
{"title":"Higher or lower? Interpersonal behavioral and neural synchronization of movement imitation in autistic children","authors":"Bang Du,&nbsp;Wenjun Zhang,&nbsp;Liu Chen,&nbsp;Xiaorui Deng,&nbsp;Kaiyun Li,&nbsp;Fengxun Lin,&nbsp;Fanlu Jia,&nbsp;Shuhua Su,&nbsp;Wanzhi Tang","doi":"10.1002/aur.3205","DOIUrl":"10.1002/aur.3205","url":null,"abstract":"<p>How well autistic children can imitate movements and how their brain activity synchronizes with the person they are imitating have been understudied. The current study adopted functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) hyperscanning and employed a task involving real interactions involving meaningful and meaningless movement imitation to explore the fundamental nature of imitation as a dynamic and interactive process. Experiment 1 explored meaningful and meaningless gesture imitation. The results revealed that autistic children exhibited lower imitation accuracy and behavioral synchrony than non-autistic children when imitating both meaningful and meaningless gestures. Specifically, compared to non-autistic children, autistic children displayed significantly higher interpersonal neural synchronization (INS) in the right inferior parietal lobule (r-IPL) (channel 12) when imitating meaningful gestures but lower INS when imitating meaningless gestures. Experiment 2 further investigated the imitation of four types of meaningless movements (orofacial movements, transitive movements, limb movements, and gestures). The results revealed that across all four movement types, autistic children exhibited significantly lower imitation accuracy, behavioral synchrony, and INS in the r-IPL (channel 12) than non-autistic children. This study is the first to identify INS as a biomarker of movement imitation difficulties in autistic individuals. Furthermore, an intra- and interindividual imitation mechanism model was proposed to explain the underlying causes of movement imitation difficulties in autistic individuals.</p>","PeriodicalId":131,"journal":{"name":"Autism Research","volume":"17 9","pages":"1876-1901"},"PeriodicalIF":5.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141908840","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship between distress tolerance and behavioral activation on anxiety and depression symptomatology in autistic youth: Leveraging self and caregiver perspectives 自闭症青少年焦虑和抑郁症状的痛苦容忍度与行为激活之间的关系:从自我和照顾者的角度看问题。
IF 5.3 2区 医学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-06 DOI: 10.1002/aur.3208
Jessica M. Schwartzman, Ligia Antezana, Caitlin M. Conner

Anxiety and depression are prevalent among autistic adolescents and may be difficult to accurately diagnose and treat given various factors (e.g., diagnostic overshadowing, heterogeneity). Therefore, efforts to examine transdiagnostic factors (i.e., distress tolerance, behavioral activation) may afford more parsimonious means for assessment and treatment. To our knowledge, there has been little research on distress tolerance, behavioral activation, and depressive and anxiety symptoms in autistic adolescents to guide diagnostic practices and treatment planning. In the current study, we examined the interrelationships between these transdiagnostic factors and depressive and anxiety symptoms using ratings from 100 verbally fluent autistic adolescents without intellectual disability (Mage = 13.70, SDage = 2.23, Range: 11:00–17:11 years) and 100 of their caregivers. Many adolescents reported male sex assigned at birth (61%), cisgender (87%), not Hispanic/Latinx (90%), and White (80%) identities. A series of correlational analyses were employed to examine associations between these constructs from youth and caregiver perspectives, and multiple linear regression analyses were conducted to explore the mediating roles of distress tolerance and behavioral activation. Preliminary results show that low distress tolerance and behavioral activation were associated with more severe internalizing symptoms per self- and caregiver-report. Some differences by rater emerged, which highlight the importance of multi-informant ratings in autism. Results from mediation analyses may show that behavioral activation may be more salient to assessments and treatment planning for depression than distress tolerance, while distress tolerance may be important for both anxiety and depression; however, findings are preliminary given the cross-sectional nature of the data. Findings suggest that these transdiagnostic concepts may be important to individualizing treatment approaches, including the timing of certain approaches, for anxiety and/or depression in autistic adolescents.

焦虑和抑郁在自闭症青少年中很普遍,由于各种因素(如诊断阴影、异质性),可能难以准确诊断和治疗。因此,努力研究跨诊断因素(即痛苦耐受性、行为激活)可能会为评估和治疗提供更合理的方法。据我们所知,有关自闭症青少年的痛苦耐受性、行为激活、抑郁和焦虑症状的研究很少,因此无法为诊断实践和治疗计划提供指导。在本研究中,我们通过对 100 名言语流利、无智力障碍的自闭症青少年(平均年龄 = 13.70 岁,平均年龄 = 2.23 岁,年龄范围:11:00-17:11 岁)及其 100 名照顾者的评分,研究了这些跨诊断因素与抑郁和焦虑症状之间的相互关系。许多青少年表示,他们出生时的性别为男性(61%)、顺性别(87%)、非西班牙裔/拉丁裔(90%)和白人(80%)。我们采用了一系列相关分析,从青少年和照顾者的角度研究了这些概念之间的关联,并进行了多元线性回归分析,以探讨困扰容忍度和行为激活的中介作用。初步结果显示,根据自我和照顾者的报告,低痛苦容忍度和行为激活与更严重的内化症状有关。不同评分者之间也存在一些差异,这凸显了多信息评分在自闭症中的重要性。中介分析的结果可能表明,行为激活对于抑郁症的评估和治疗计划可能比痛苦耐受力更重要,而痛苦耐受力可能对焦虑和抑郁都很重要;然而,鉴于数据的横断面性质,研究结果还只是初步的。研究结果表明,这些跨诊断概念可能对自闭症青少年焦虑和/或抑郁的个性化治疗方法(包括某些方法的时机选择)非常重要。
{"title":"The relationship between distress tolerance and behavioral activation on anxiety and depression symptomatology in autistic youth: Leveraging self and caregiver perspectives","authors":"Jessica M. Schwartzman,&nbsp;Ligia Antezana,&nbsp;Caitlin M. Conner","doi":"10.1002/aur.3208","DOIUrl":"10.1002/aur.3208","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Anxiety and depression are prevalent among autistic adolescents and may be difficult to accurately diagnose and treat given various factors (e.g., diagnostic overshadowing, heterogeneity). Therefore, efforts to examine transdiagnostic factors (i.e., distress tolerance, behavioral activation) may afford more parsimonious means for assessment and treatment. To our knowledge, there has been little research on distress tolerance, behavioral activation, and depressive and anxiety symptoms in autistic adolescents to guide diagnostic practices and treatment planning. In the current study, we examined the interrelationships between these transdiagnostic factors and depressive and anxiety symptoms using ratings from 100 verbally fluent autistic adolescents without intellectual disability (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 13.70, SD<sub>age</sub> = 2.23, Range: 11:00–17:11 years) and 100 of their caregivers. Many adolescents reported male sex assigned at birth (61%), cisgender (87%), not Hispanic/Latinx (90%), and White (80%) identities. A series of correlational analyses were employed to examine associations between these constructs from youth and caregiver perspectives, and multiple linear regression analyses were conducted to explore the mediating roles of distress tolerance and behavioral activation. Preliminary results show that low distress tolerance and behavioral activation were associated with more severe internalizing symptoms per self- and caregiver-report. Some differences by rater emerged, which highlight the importance of multi-informant ratings in autism. Results from mediation analyses may show that behavioral activation may be more salient to assessments and treatment planning for depression than distress tolerance, while distress tolerance may be important for both anxiety and depression; however, findings are preliminary given the cross-sectional nature of the data. Findings suggest that these transdiagnostic concepts may be important to individualizing treatment approaches, including the timing of certain approaches, for anxiety and/or depression in autistic adolescents.</p>","PeriodicalId":131,"journal":{"name":"Autism Research","volume":"17 11","pages":"2278-2291"},"PeriodicalIF":5.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/aur.3208","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141894984","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What do we know about autism and policing globally? Preliminary findings from an international effort to examine autism and the criminal justice system 我们对全球自闭症和警务工作了解多少?自闭症与刑事司法系统国际研究的初步发现。
IF 5.3 2区 医学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-05 DOI: 10.1002/aur.3203
Dylan Cooper, Shelby Frisbie, Shufang Wang, Jonas Ventimiglia, Vicki Gibbs, Abigail M. A. Love, Melanie Mogavero, Teal W. Benevides, Jordan M. Hyatt, Kate Hooven, Icylee Basketbill, Lindsay Shea

Research has demonstrated that autistic individuals have higher rates of police contact, however, research has seldom explored the fundamental reasons for these interactions and how this might vary across international contexts. To remedy this, the Global Autism and Criminal Justice Consortium created and disseminated the Global Criminal Justice Survey. Descriptive statistics of survey respondents with and without police contact were compared to glean differential characteristics. Frequency and type of recent police interactions (within the last 5 years) among autistic individuals were also examined to better contextualize the reasons that autistic individuals encounter police. Study findings indicated that across a global sample (i.e., North America, Scandinavia, Europe, and Oceania) nearly half of all autistic individuals had an interaction with police and that those with a history of police contact were usually older, had higher educational qualifications, and were more likely to have a co-occurring mental health or developmental disorder. Among types of interactions, noncriminal encounters, such as welfare checks, traffic incidents, wandering, and behaviors associated with autism, were most common, followed by autistic individuals alleging a crime was committed against them. These findings offer important directions for future research and for targeted policy responses that can address the unique needs of autistic individuals within the justice system.

研究表明,自闭症患者与警察接触的比例较高,但研究很少探讨这些互动的根本原因,以及在不同的国际背景下可能存在的差异。为了弥补这一不足,全球自闭症与刑事司法联合会创建并发布了全球刑事司法调查。我们对与警察有接触和没有接触的调查对象进行了描述性统计比较,以收集不同的特征。此外,还研究了自闭症患者近期(过去 5 年内)与警方互动的频率和类型,以便更好地了解自闭症患者与警方接触的原因。研究结果表明,在全球样本(即北美、斯堪的纳维亚半岛、欧洲和大洋洲)中,近一半的自闭症患者与警方有过互动,而有过与警方接触史的自闭症患者通常年龄较大、学历较高,并且更有可能同时患有精神疾病或发育障碍。在互动类型中,福利检查、交通事故、游荡和与自闭症有关的行为等非刑事遭遇最为常见,其次是自闭症患者声称有人对他们实施了犯罪。这些发现为今后的研究和有针对性的政策应对提供了重要方向,可以满足自闭症患者在司法系统中的独特需求。
{"title":"What do we know about autism and policing globally? Preliminary findings from an international effort to examine autism and the criminal justice system","authors":"Dylan Cooper,&nbsp;Shelby Frisbie,&nbsp;Shufang Wang,&nbsp;Jonas Ventimiglia,&nbsp;Vicki Gibbs,&nbsp;Abigail M. A. Love,&nbsp;Melanie Mogavero,&nbsp;Teal W. Benevides,&nbsp;Jordan M. Hyatt,&nbsp;Kate Hooven,&nbsp;Icylee Basketbill,&nbsp;Lindsay Shea","doi":"10.1002/aur.3203","DOIUrl":"10.1002/aur.3203","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Research has demonstrated that autistic individuals have higher rates of police contact, however, research has seldom explored the fundamental reasons for these interactions and how this might vary across international contexts. To remedy this, the Global Autism and Criminal Justice Consortium created and disseminated the Global Criminal Justice Survey. Descriptive statistics of survey respondents with and without police contact were compared to glean differential characteristics. Frequency and type of recent police interactions (within the last 5 years) among autistic individuals were also examined to better contextualize the reasons that autistic individuals encounter police. Study findings indicated that across a global sample (i.e., North America, Scandinavia, Europe, and Oceania) nearly half of all autistic individuals had an interaction with police and that those with a history of police contact were usually older, had higher educational qualifications, and were more likely to have a co-occurring mental health or developmental disorder. Among types of interactions, noncriminal encounters, such as welfare checks, traffic incidents, wandering, and behaviors associated with autism, were most common, followed by autistic individuals alleging a crime was committed against them. These findings offer important directions for future research and for targeted policy responses that can address the unique needs of autistic individuals within the justice system.</p>","PeriodicalId":131,"journal":{"name":"Autism Research","volume":"17 10","pages":"2133-2143"},"PeriodicalIF":5.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141894985","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pupillary response during social emotion tasks in autism spectrum disorder 自闭症谱系障碍患者在社交情感任务中的瞳孔反应。
IF 5.3 2区 医学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-02 DOI: 10.1002/aur.3206
Juei-Po Lee, Yi-Hsuan Chang, Yi-Li Tseng, Tai-Li Chou, Yi-Ling Chien

Autistic individuals encounter challenges in recognizing emotional expressions of others. Pupillary response has been proposed as an indicator of arousal dysregulation or cognitive load. The pupillary response of autistic individuals during socio-affective tasks remains unclear. This study investigated pupillary response in autistic adults when viewing emotional faces/eyes and recognizing emotions during the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET) and watching interpersonal touch scenes in the social touch task. The study included 98 participants diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder and 37 typically developing controls (TD). Pupil size was measured using the Tobii X2-30 Eye Tracker. The results showed that autistic adults had larger maximal pupil sizes, smaller minimal pupil sizes, and greater change rates of pupil size, particularly during the RMET Eyes task. Clinical correlations revealed that attention switching difficulty positively correlated with mean pupil size in TD participants, while social communication deficits positively correlated with mean pupil size in autistic participants. In conclusion, our findings suggest atypical pupillary responses in autistic adults during socio-affective tasks, indicating heightened cognitive demand. Further investigation is necessary to understand the underlying mechanisms and their association with autistic traits.

自闭症患者在识别他人的情绪表达方面面临挑战。瞳孔反应被认为是唤醒失调或认知负荷的指标。自闭症患者在完成社会情感任务时的瞳孔反应仍不清楚。本研究调查了自闭症成人在 "从眼睛读懂心灵 "测试(RMET)中观看情绪化面孔/眼睛和识别情绪时的瞳孔反应,以及在 "社会接触 "任务中观看人际接触场景时的瞳孔反应。这项研究包括 98 名被诊断患有自闭症谱系障碍的参与者和 37 名发育正常的对照组(TD)。使用 Tobii X2-30 眼球追踪器测量了瞳孔大小。结果显示,自闭症成人的最大瞳孔尺寸较大,最小瞳孔尺寸较小,瞳孔尺寸变化率较大,尤其是在 RMET Eyes 任务中。临床相关性显示,注意力转换困难与 TD 参与者的平均瞳孔大小呈正相关,而自闭症参与者的社交沟通障碍与平均瞳孔大小呈正相关。总之,我们的研究结果表明,自闭症成人在社交情感任务中的瞳孔反应不典型,表明认知需求增加。要了解其潜在机制及其与自闭症特征的关联,还需要进一步的研究。
{"title":"Pupillary response during social emotion tasks in autism spectrum disorder","authors":"Juei-Po Lee,&nbsp;Yi-Hsuan Chang,&nbsp;Yi-Li Tseng,&nbsp;Tai-Li Chou,&nbsp;Yi-Ling Chien","doi":"10.1002/aur.3206","DOIUrl":"10.1002/aur.3206","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Autistic individuals encounter challenges in recognizing emotional expressions of others. Pupillary response has been proposed as an indicator of arousal dysregulation or cognitive load. The pupillary response of autistic individuals during socio-affective tasks remains unclear. This study investigated pupillary response in autistic adults when viewing emotional faces/eyes and recognizing emotions during the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET) and watching interpersonal touch scenes in the social touch task. The study included 98 participants diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder and 37 typically developing controls (TD). Pupil size was measured using the Tobii X2-30 Eye Tracker. The results showed that autistic adults had larger maximal pupil sizes, smaller minimal pupil sizes, and greater change rates of pupil size, particularly during the RMET Eyes task. Clinical correlations revealed that attention switching difficulty positively correlated with mean pupil size in TD participants, while social communication deficits positively correlated with mean pupil size in autistic participants. In conclusion, our findings suggest atypical pupillary responses in autistic adults during socio-affective tasks, indicating heightened cognitive demand. Further investigation is necessary to understand the underlying mechanisms and their association with autistic traits.</p>","PeriodicalId":131,"journal":{"name":"Autism Research","volume":"17 10","pages":"2120-2132"},"PeriodicalIF":5.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141879937","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reduced spatial frequency differentiation and sex-related specificities in fearful face detection in autism: Insights from EEG and the predictive brain model 自闭症患者恐惧面孔检测的空间频率分化和性别特异性降低:脑电图和大脑预测模型的启示。
IF 5.3 2区 医学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-02 DOI: 10.1002/aur.3209
Adeline Lacroix, Sylvain Harquel, Leonardo S. Barbosa, Klara Kovarski, Marta I. Garrido, Laurent Vercueil, Louise Kauffmann, Frédéric Dutheil, Marie Gomot, Martial Mermillod

Face processing relies on predictive processes driven by low spatial frequencies (LSF) that convey coarse information prior to fine information conveyed by high spatial frequencies. However, autistic individuals might have atypical predictive processes, contributing to facial processing difficulties. This may be more normalized in autistic females, who often exhibit better socio-communicational abilities than males. We hypothesized that autistic females would display a more typical coarse-to-fine processing for socio-emotional stimuli compared to autistic males. To test this hypothesis, we asked adult participants (44 autistic, 51 non-autistic) to detect fearful faces among neutral faces, filtered in two orders: from coarse-to-fine (CtF) and from fine-to-coarse (FtC). Results show lower d’ values and longer reaction times for fearful detection in autism compared to non-autistic (NA) individuals, regardless of the filtering order. Both groups presented shorter P100 latency after CtF compared to FtC, and larger amplitude for N170 after FtC compared to CtF. However, autistic participants presented a reduced difference in source activity between CtF and FtC in the fusiform. There was also a more spatially spread activation pattern in autistic females compared to NA females. Finally, females had faster P100 and N170 latencies, as well as larger occipital activation for FtC sequences than males, irrespective of the group. Overall, the results do not suggest impaired predictive processes from LSF in autism despite behavioral differences in fear detection. However, they do indicate reduced brain modulation by spatial frequency in autism. In addition, the findings highlight sex differences that warrant consideration in understanding autistic females.

面部处理依赖于由低空间频率(LSF)驱动的预测过程,低空间频率先于高空间频率传递的精细信息传递粗信息。然而,自闭症患者可能有非典型的预测过程,从而导致面部处理困难。这种情况在女性自闭症患者中可能更加正常,因为她们通常比男性自闭症患者表现出更好的社会沟通能力。我们假设,与男性自闭症患者相比,女性自闭症患者在处理社会情感刺激时会表现出更典型的从粗到细的过程。为了验证这一假设,我们要求成年参与者(44 名自闭症患者,51 名非自闭症患者)在中性面孔中检测出恐惧面孔,并按照两种顺序进行过滤:从粗到细(CtF)和从细到粗(FtC)。结果显示,与非自闭症(NA)患者相比,无论采用哪种过滤顺序,自闭症患者检测恐惧面孔的 d'值都更低,反应时间更长。与 FtC 相比,自闭症患者在 CtF 之后的 P100 潜伏期更短,而与 CtF 相比,自闭症患者在 FtC 之后的 N170 振幅更大。然而,自闭症患者在纺锤形区域的 CtF 和 FtC 信号源活动差异较小。与非自闭症女性相比,自闭症女性的激活模式在空间上更为分散。最后,与男性相比,女性的 P100 和 N170 潜伏期更快,枕叶对 FtC 序列的激活也更大,与组别无关。总体而言,尽管自闭症患者在恐惧检测方面存在行为差异,但这些结果并不表明自闭症患者的 LSF 预测过程受损。然而,这些结果确实表明自闭症患者大脑对空间频率的调节能力下降。此外,研究结果还强调了在理解自闭症女性时需要考虑的性别差异。
{"title":"Reduced spatial frequency differentiation and sex-related specificities in fearful face detection in autism: Insights from EEG and the predictive brain model","authors":"Adeline Lacroix,&nbsp;Sylvain Harquel,&nbsp;Leonardo S. Barbosa,&nbsp;Klara Kovarski,&nbsp;Marta I. Garrido,&nbsp;Laurent Vercueil,&nbsp;Louise Kauffmann,&nbsp;Frédéric Dutheil,&nbsp;Marie Gomot,&nbsp;Martial Mermillod","doi":"10.1002/aur.3209","DOIUrl":"10.1002/aur.3209","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Face processing relies on predictive processes driven by low spatial frequencies (LSF) that convey coarse information prior to fine information conveyed by high spatial frequencies. However, autistic individuals might have atypical predictive processes, contributing to facial processing difficulties. This may be more normalized in autistic females, who often exhibit better socio-communicational abilities than males. We hypothesized that autistic females would display a more typical coarse-to-fine processing for socio-emotional stimuli compared to autistic males. To test this hypothesis, we asked adult participants (44 autistic, 51 non-autistic) to detect fearful faces among neutral faces, filtered in two orders: from coarse-to-fine (CtF) and from fine-to-coarse (FtC). Results show lower <i>d’</i> values and longer reaction times for fearful detection in autism compared to non-autistic (NA) individuals, regardless of the filtering order. Both groups presented shorter P100 latency after CtF compared to FtC, and larger amplitude for N170 after FtC compared to CtF. However, autistic participants presented a reduced difference in source activity between CtF and FtC in the fusiform. There was also a more spatially spread activation pattern in autistic females compared to NA females. Finally, females had faster P100 and N170 latencies, as well as larger occipital activation for FtC sequences than males, irrespective of the group. Overall, the results do not suggest impaired predictive processes from LSF in autism despite behavioral differences in fear detection. However, they do indicate reduced brain modulation by spatial frequency in autism. In addition, the findings highlight sex differences that warrant consideration in understanding autistic females.</p>","PeriodicalId":131,"journal":{"name":"Autism Research","volume":"17 9","pages":"1778-1795"},"PeriodicalIF":5.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/aur.3209","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141876804","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Empathy in autistic children: Emotional overarousal in response to others' physical pain 自闭症儿童的同理心:对他人身体疼痛的过度情绪反应。
IF 5.3 2区 医学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1002/aur.3200
Tianbi Li, Jean Decety, Zihui Hua, Guoxiang Li, Li Yi

Different empathic responses are often reported in autism but remain controversial. To investigate which component of empathy is most affected by autism, we examined the affective, cognitive, and motivational components of empathy in 25 5- to 8-year-old autistic and 27 neurotypical children. Participants were presented with visual stimuli depicting people's limbs in painful or nonpainful situations while their eye movements, pupillary responses, and verbal ratings of pain intensity and empathic concern were recorded. The results indicate an emotional overarousal and reduced empathic concern to others' pain in autism. Compared with neurotypical children, autistic children displayed larger pupil dilation accompanied by attentional avoidance to others' pain. Moreover, even though autistic children rated others in painful situations as painful, they felt less sorry than neurotypical children. Interestingly, autistic children felt more sorry in nonpainful situations compared with neurotypical children. These findings demonstrated an emotional overarousal in response to others' pain in autistic children, and provide important implications for clinical practice aiming to promote socio-emotional understanding in autistic children.

据报道,自闭症患者经常会出现不同的移情反应,但这一点仍存在争议。为了研究自闭症对移情的哪种影响最大,我们对 25 名 5 至 8 岁的自闭症儿童和 27 名神经畸形儿童的移情的情感、认知和动机成分进行了研究。我们向参与者展示了描绘人们在疼痛或非疼痛情况下的肢体的视觉刺激,同时记录了他们的眼球运动、瞳孔反应以及对疼痛强度和移情关注的口头评价。研究结果表明,自闭症儿童对他人疼痛的情绪过度亢奋和移情关注减少。与神经畸形儿童相比,自闭症儿童的瞳孔放大程度更大,同时对他人的疼痛表现出注意回避。此外,尽管自闭症儿童认为他人在痛苦的情况下是痛苦的,但他们感到的歉意却少于神经畸形儿童。有趣的是,与神经畸形儿童相比,自闭症儿童在非痛苦情境中感到的歉意更多。这些研究结果表明,自闭症儿童对他人的痛苦会产生过度的情感反应,这对旨在促进自闭症儿童社会情感理解的临床实践具有重要意义。
{"title":"Empathy in autistic children: Emotional overarousal in response to others' physical pain","authors":"Tianbi Li,&nbsp;Jean Decety,&nbsp;Zihui Hua,&nbsp;Guoxiang Li,&nbsp;Li Yi","doi":"10.1002/aur.3200","DOIUrl":"10.1002/aur.3200","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Different empathic responses are often reported in autism but remain controversial. To investigate which component of empathy is most affected by autism, we examined the affective, cognitive, and motivational components of empathy in 25 5- to 8-year-old autistic and 27 neurotypical children. Participants were presented with visual stimuli depicting people's limbs in painful or nonpainful situations while their eye movements, pupillary responses, and verbal ratings of pain intensity and empathic concern were recorded. The results indicate an emotional overarousal and reduced empathic concern to others' pain in autism. Compared with neurotypical children, autistic children displayed larger pupil dilation accompanied by attentional avoidance to others' pain. Moreover, even though autistic children rated others in painful situations as painful, they felt less sorry than neurotypical children. Interestingly, autistic children felt more sorry in nonpainful situations compared with neurotypical children. These findings demonstrated an emotional overarousal in response to others' pain in autistic children, and provide important implications for clinical practice aiming to promote socio-emotional understanding in autistic children.</p>","PeriodicalId":131,"journal":{"name":"Autism Research","volume":"17 8","pages":"1640-1650"},"PeriodicalIF":5.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141861795","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Autism Research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1