首页 > 最新文献

The Journal of Neuroscience最新文献

英文 中文
Rhythmic Information Sampling in the Brain during Visual Recognition 视觉识别过程中大脑的节奏信息采样
Pub Date : 2023-04-25 DOI: 10.1101/2022.06.30.498324
Laurent Caplette, K. Jerbi, F. Gosselin
When we fixate an object, visual information is continuously received on the retina. Several studies observed behavioral oscillations in perceptual sensitivity across such stimulus time, and these fluctuations have been linked to brain oscillations. However, whether specific brain areas show oscillations across stimulus time (i.e., different time points of the stimulus being more or less processed, in a rhythmic fashion) has not been investigated. Here, we revealed random areas of face images at random moments across time and recorded the brain activity of male and female human participants using MEG while they performed two recognition tasks. This allowed us to quantify how each snapshot of visual information coming from the stimulus is processed across time and across the brain. Oscillations across stimulus time (rhythmic sampling) were mostly visible in early visual areas, at theta, alpha, and low beta frequencies. We also found that they contributed to brain activity more than previously investigated rhythmic processing (oscillations in the processing of a single snapshot of visual information). Nonrhythmic sampling was also visible at later latencies across the visual cortex, either in the form of a transient processing of early stimulus time points or of a sustained processing of the whole stimulus. Our results suggest that successive cycles of ongoing brain oscillations process stimulus information incoming at successive moments. Together, these results advance our understanding of the oscillatory neural dynamics associated with visual processing and show the importance of considering the temporal dimension of stimuli when studying visual recognition. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Several behavioral studies have observed oscillations in perceptual sensitivity over the duration of stimulus presentation, and these fluctuations have been linked to brain oscillations. However, oscillations across stimulus time in the brain have not been studied. Here, we developed an MEG paradigm to quantify how visual information received at each moment during fixation is processed through time and across the brain. We showed that different snapshots of a stimulus are distinctly processed in many brain areas and that these fluctuations are oscillatory in early visual areas. Oscillations across stimulus time were more prevalent than previously studied oscillations across processing time. These results increase our understanding of how neural oscillations interact with the visual processing of temporal stimuli.
当我们注视一个物体时,视网膜会不断地接收视觉信息。几项研究观察到知觉敏感性在这种刺激时间内的行为振荡,这些波动与大脑振荡有关。然而,特定的大脑区域是否在刺激时间内显示振荡(即,刺激的不同时间点或多或少以有节奏的方式被处理)尚未被研究。在这里,我们揭示了随机时刻的随机面部图像区域,并使用MEG记录了男性和女性参与者在执行两项识别任务时的大脑活动。这使我们能够量化来自刺激的视觉信息的每个快照是如何在不同的时间和大脑中处理的。在刺激时间的振荡(有节奏的采样)主要可见于早期视觉区域,在θ, α和低β频率。我们还发现,它们对大脑活动的贡献比之前研究的节奏处理(处理单个视觉信息快照时的振荡)更大。非节律性采样在视觉皮层的后期潜伏期也可见,要么以对早期刺激时间点的短暂处理的形式出现,要么以对整个刺激的持续处理的形式出现。我们的结果表明,持续的大脑振荡的连续周期处理在连续时刻传入的刺激信息。总之,这些结果促进了我们对与视觉处理相关的振荡神经动力学的理解,并显示了在研究视觉识别时考虑刺激的时间维度的重要性。一些行为研究已经观察到知觉敏感性在刺激呈现的持续时间内的振荡,这些波动与大脑振荡有关。然而,大脑中刺激时间的振荡尚未被研究过。在这里,我们开发了一种脑磁图范式来量化在注视过程中每个时刻接收到的视觉信息是如何随着时间和大脑的变化而被处理的。我们发现,刺激的不同快照在许多大脑区域被明显地处理,这些波动在早期视觉区域是振荡的。刺激时间的振荡比先前研究的加工时间的振荡更为普遍。这些结果增加了我们对神经振荡如何与时间刺激的视觉处理相互作用的理解。
{"title":"Rhythmic Information Sampling in the Brain during Visual Recognition","authors":"Laurent Caplette, K. Jerbi, F. Gosselin","doi":"10.1101/2022.06.30.498324","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.06.30.498324","url":null,"abstract":"When we fixate an object, visual information is continuously received on the retina. Several studies observed behavioral oscillations in perceptual sensitivity across such stimulus time, and these fluctuations have been linked to brain oscillations. However, whether specific brain areas show oscillations across stimulus time (i.e., different time points of the stimulus being more or less processed, in a rhythmic fashion) has not been investigated. Here, we revealed random areas of face images at random moments across time and recorded the brain activity of male and female human participants using MEG while they performed two recognition tasks. This allowed us to quantify how each snapshot of visual information coming from the stimulus is processed across time and across the brain. Oscillations across stimulus time (rhythmic sampling) were mostly visible in early visual areas, at theta, alpha, and low beta frequencies. We also found that they contributed to brain activity more than previously investigated rhythmic processing (oscillations in the processing of a single snapshot of visual information). Nonrhythmic sampling was also visible at later latencies across the visual cortex, either in the form of a transient processing of early stimulus time points or of a sustained processing of the whole stimulus. Our results suggest that successive cycles of ongoing brain oscillations process stimulus information incoming at successive moments. Together, these results advance our understanding of the oscillatory neural dynamics associated with visual processing and show the importance of considering the temporal dimension of stimuli when studying visual recognition. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Several behavioral studies have observed oscillations in perceptual sensitivity over the duration of stimulus presentation, and these fluctuations have been linked to brain oscillations. However, oscillations across stimulus time in the brain have not been studied. Here, we developed an MEG paradigm to quantify how visual information received at each moment during fixation is processed through time and across the brain. We showed that different snapshots of a stimulus are distinctly processed in many brain areas and that these fluctuations are oscillatory in early visual areas. Oscillations across stimulus time were more prevalent than previously studied oscillations across processing time. These results increase our understanding of how neural oscillations interact with the visual processing of temporal stimuli.","PeriodicalId":22786,"journal":{"name":"The Journal of Neuroscience","volume":"17 1","pages":"4487 - 4497"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80069878","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Instant Effects of Semantic Information on Visual Perception 语义信息对视觉感知的即时效应
Pub Date : 2023-04-19 DOI: 10.1101/2023.04.19.537469
Alexander Enge, Franziska Süß, Rasha Abdel Rahman
Does our perception of an object change once we discover what function it serves? We showed human participants (n = 48, 31 females and 17 males) pictures of unfamiliar objects either together with keywords matching their function, leading to semantically informed perception, or together with nonmatching keywords, resulting in uninformed perception. We measured event-related potentials to investigate at which stages in the visual processing hierarchy these two types of object perception differed from one another. We found that semantically informed compared with uninformed perception was associated with larger amplitudes in the N170 component (150-200 ms), reduced amplitudes in the N400 component (400-700 ms), and a late decrease in alpha/beta band power. When the same objects were presented once more without any information, the N400 and event-related power effects persisted, and we also observed enlarged amplitudes in the P1 component (100-150 ms) in response to objects for which semantically informed perception had taken place. Consistent with previous work, this suggests that obtaining semantic information about previously unfamiliar objects alters aspects of their lower-level visual perception (P1 component), higher-level visual perception (N170 component), and semantic processing (N400 component, event-related power). Our study is the first to show that such effects occur instantly after semantic information has been provided for the first time, without requiring extensive learning. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT There has been a long-standing debate about whether or not higher-level cognitive capacities, such as semantic knowledge, can influence lower-level perceptual processing in a top-down fashion. Here we could show, for the first time, that information about the function of previously unfamiliar objects immediately influences cortical processing within less than 200 ms. Of note, this influence does not require training or experience with the objects and related semantic information. Therefore, our study is the first to show effects of cognition on perception while ruling out the possibility that prior knowledge merely acts by preactivating or altering stored visual representations. Instead, this knowledge seems to alter perception online, thus providing a compelling case against the impenetrability of perception by cognition.
一旦我们发现了一个物体的功能,我们对它的感知会改变吗?我们向人类参与者(n = 48, 31名女性和17名男性)展示了不熟悉物体的图片,其中一种是与功能匹配的关键词一起出现,导致语义知情的感知,另一种是与不匹配的关键词一起出现,导致语义不知情的感知。我们测量了事件相关电位,以研究这两种类型的物体感知在视觉处理层次的哪个阶段彼此不同。我们发现,与不知情感知相比,语义知情感知与N170分量(150-200 ms)的振幅较大,N400分量(400-700 ms)的振幅减小以及α / β波段功率的后期下降有关。当在没有任何信息的情况下再次呈现相同的物体时,N400和事件相关的功率效应持续存在,我们还观察到P1分量的振幅(100-150 ms)在对语义信息感知的物体做出反应时增大。与之前的研究一致,这表明获取先前不熟悉的物体的语义信息会改变其低级视觉感知(P1分量)、高级视觉感知(N170分量)和语义处理(N400分量,事件相关功率)的各个方面。我们的研究首次表明,这种效果在第一次提供语义信息后立即发生,而不需要大量的学习。关于更高层次的认知能力,如语义知识,是否能够以自上而下的方式影响较低层次的感知加工,一直存在着长期的争论。在这里,我们可以首次证明,关于以前不熟悉的物体的功能的信息在不到200毫秒的时间内立即影响皮层的处理。值得注意的是,这种影响不需要对物体和相关语义信息进行训练或经验。因此,我们的研究首次展示了认知对感知的影响,同时排除了先验知识仅仅通过预先激活或改变存储的视觉表征来起作用的可能性。相反,这种知识似乎改变了在线感知,从而提供了一个令人信服的案例,反对认知无法穿透感知。
{"title":"Instant Effects of Semantic Information on Visual Perception","authors":"Alexander Enge, Franziska Süß, Rasha Abdel Rahman","doi":"10.1101/2023.04.19.537469","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.04.19.537469","url":null,"abstract":"Does our perception of an object change once we discover what function it serves? We showed human participants (n = 48, 31 females and 17 males) pictures of unfamiliar objects either together with keywords matching their function, leading to semantically informed perception, or together with nonmatching keywords, resulting in uninformed perception. We measured event-related potentials to investigate at which stages in the visual processing hierarchy these two types of object perception differed from one another. We found that semantically informed compared with uninformed perception was associated with larger amplitudes in the N170 component (150-200 ms), reduced amplitudes in the N400 component (400-700 ms), and a late decrease in alpha/beta band power. When the same objects were presented once more without any information, the N400 and event-related power effects persisted, and we also observed enlarged amplitudes in the P1 component (100-150 ms) in response to objects for which semantically informed perception had taken place. Consistent with previous work, this suggests that obtaining semantic information about previously unfamiliar objects alters aspects of their lower-level visual perception (P1 component), higher-level visual perception (N170 component), and semantic processing (N400 component, event-related power). Our study is the first to show that such effects occur instantly after semantic information has been provided for the first time, without requiring extensive learning. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT There has been a long-standing debate about whether or not higher-level cognitive capacities, such as semantic knowledge, can influence lower-level perceptual processing in a top-down fashion. Here we could show, for the first time, that information about the function of previously unfamiliar objects immediately influences cortical processing within less than 200 ms. Of note, this influence does not require training or experience with the objects and related semantic information. Therefore, our study is the first to show effects of cognition on perception while ruling out the possibility that prior knowledge merely acts by preactivating or altering stored visual representations. Instead, this knowledge seems to alter perception online, thus providing a compelling case against the impenetrability of perception by cognition.","PeriodicalId":22786,"journal":{"name":"The Journal of Neuroscience","volume":"10 1","pages":"4896 - 4906"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84239407","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
This Week in The Journal 本周华尔街日报
Pub Date : 2023-04-19 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.twij.43.16.2023
{"title":"This Week in The Journal","authors":"","doi":"10.1523/JNEUROSCI.twij.43.16.2023","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.twij.43.16.2023","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":22786,"journal":{"name":"The Journal of Neuroscience","volume":"770 1","pages":"2818 - 2818"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"76941012","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Axon Initial Segment GABA Inhibits Action Potential Generation throughout Periadolescent Development 轴突起始段GABA抑制青春期周围发育过程中动作电位的产生
Pub Date : 2023-04-16 DOI: 10.1101/2023.04.16.537084
Anna M Lipkin, K. Bender
Neurons are remarkably polarized structures: dendrites spread and branch to receive synaptic inputs while a single axon extends and transmits action potentials (APs) to downstream targets. Neuronal polarity is maintained by the axon initial segment (AIS), a region between the soma and axon proper that is also the site of action potential (AP) generation. This polarization between dendrites and axons extends to inhibitory neurotransmission. In adulthood, the neurotransmitter GABA hyperpolarizes dendrites but instead depolarizes axons. These differences in function collide at the AIS. Multiple studies have shown that GABAergic signaling in this region can share properties of either the mature axon or mature dendrite, and that these properties evolve over a protracted period encompassing periadolescent development. Here, we explored how developmental changes in GABAergic signaling affect AP initiation. We show that GABA at the axon initial segment inhibits action potential initiation in layer (L)2/3 pyramidal neurons in prefrontal cortex from mice of either sex across GABA reversal potentials observed in periadolescence. These actions occur largely through current shunts generated by GABAA receptors and changes in voltage-gated channel properties that affected the number of channels that could be recruited for AP electrogenesis. These results suggest that GABAergic neurons targeting the axon initial segment provide an inhibitory “veto” across the range of GABA polarity observed in normal adolescent development, regardless of GABAergic synapse reversal potential. Significance Statement GABA receptors are a major class of neurotransmitter receptors in the brain. Typically, GABA receptors inhibit neurons by allowing influx of negatively charged chloride ions into the cell. However, there are cases where local chloride concentrations promote chloride efflux through GABA receptors. Such conditions exist early in development in neocortical pyramidal cell axon initial segments (AISs), where action potentials (APs) initiate. Here, we examined how chloride efflux in early development interacts with mechanisms that support action potential initiation. We find that this efflux, despite moving membrane potential closer to action potential threshold, is nevertheless inhibitory. Thus, GABA at the axon initial segment is likely to be inhibitory for action potential initiation independent of whether chloride flows out or into neurons via these receptors.
神经元是一种显著的极化结构:树突扩散并分支接收突触输入,而单个轴突延伸并向下游目标传递动作电位(ap)。神经元极性由轴突初始段(AIS)维持,这是一个位于体细胞和轴突之间的区域,也是动作电位(AP)产生的部位。树突和轴突之间的这种极化延伸到抑制性神经传递。在成年期,神经递质GABA使树突过度极化,而不是使轴突去极化。这些功能上的差异在AIS系统上发生了冲突。多项研究表明,该区域的gaba能信号可以共享成熟轴突或成熟树突的特性,并且这些特性在包括青春期周围发育的漫长时期内进化。在这里,我们探讨了gaba能信号的发育变化如何影响AP的启动。我们发现轴突起始段的GABA通过观察青春期前后小鼠的GABA逆转电位,抑制了前额皮质层(L)2/3锥体神经元的动作电位起始。这些作用主要是通过GABAA受体产生的电流分流和电压门控通道特性的变化发生的,这些特性影响了可以为AP电生成募集的通道数量。这些结果表明,针对轴突初始段的GABA能神经元在正常青少年发育中观察到的GABA极性范围内提供了抑制性“否决”,而不管GABA能突触逆转电位如何。GABA受体是大脑中主要的一类神经递质受体。通常,GABA受体通过允许带负电荷的氯离子流入细胞来抑制神经元。然而,也有局部氯离子浓度通过GABA受体促进氯离子外排的情况。这些条件存在于发育早期的新皮质锥体细胞轴突初始段(AISs),在那里动作电位(APs)启动。在这里,我们研究了发育早期氯化物外排如何与支持动作电位启动的机制相互作用。我们发现这种外排,尽管移动膜电位更接近动作电位阈值,但仍然是抑制性的。因此,轴突起始段的GABA可能对动作电位起始具有抑制作用,而与氯离子是通过这些受体流出还是流入神经元无关。
{"title":"Axon Initial Segment GABA Inhibits Action Potential Generation throughout Periadolescent Development","authors":"Anna M Lipkin, K. Bender","doi":"10.1101/2023.04.16.537084","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.04.16.537084","url":null,"abstract":"Neurons are remarkably polarized structures: dendrites spread and branch to receive synaptic inputs while a single axon extends and transmits action potentials (APs) to downstream targets. Neuronal polarity is maintained by the axon initial segment (AIS), a region between the soma and axon proper that is also the site of action potential (AP) generation. This polarization between dendrites and axons extends to inhibitory neurotransmission. In adulthood, the neurotransmitter GABA hyperpolarizes dendrites but instead depolarizes axons. These differences in function collide at the AIS. Multiple studies have shown that GABAergic signaling in this region can share properties of either the mature axon or mature dendrite, and that these properties evolve over a protracted period encompassing periadolescent development. Here, we explored how developmental changes in GABAergic signaling affect AP initiation. We show that GABA at the axon initial segment inhibits action potential initiation in layer (L)2/3 pyramidal neurons in prefrontal cortex from mice of either sex across GABA reversal potentials observed in periadolescence. These actions occur largely through current shunts generated by GABAA receptors and changes in voltage-gated channel properties that affected the number of channels that could be recruited for AP electrogenesis. These results suggest that GABAergic neurons targeting the axon initial segment provide an inhibitory “veto” across the range of GABA polarity observed in normal adolescent development, regardless of GABAergic synapse reversal potential. Significance Statement GABA receptors are a major class of neurotransmitter receptors in the brain. Typically, GABA receptors inhibit neurons by allowing influx of negatively charged chloride ions into the cell. However, there are cases where local chloride concentrations promote chloride efflux through GABA receptors. Such conditions exist early in development in neocortical pyramidal cell axon initial segments (AISs), where action potentials (APs) initiate. Here, we examined how chloride efflux in early development interacts with mechanisms that support action potential initiation. We find that this efflux, despite moving membrane potential closer to action potential threshold, is nevertheless inhibitory. Thus, GABA at the axon initial segment is likely to be inhibitory for action potential initiation independent of whether chloride flows out or into neurons via these receptors.","PeriodicalId":22786,"journal":{"name":"The Journal of Neuroscience","volume":"18 1","pages":"6357 - 6368"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85058498","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Neural α Oscillations and Pupil Size Differentially Index Cognitive Demand under Competing Audiovisual Task Conditions 竞争性视听任务条件下神经α振荡和瞳孔大小差异指示认知需求
Pub Date : 2023-04-12 DOI: 10.1101/2022.11.25.517931
Frauke Kraus, Sarah Tune, J. Obleser, Björn Herrmann
Cognitive demand is thought to modulate two often used, but rarely combined, measures: pupil size and neural α (8–12 Hz) oscillatory power. However, it is unclear whether these two measures capture cognitive demand in a similar way under complex audiovisual-task conditions. Here we recorded pupil size and neural α power (using electroencephalography), while human participants of both sexes concurrently performed a visual multiple object-tracking task and an auditory gap detection task. Difficulties of the two tasks were manipulated independent of each other. Participants' performance decreased in accuracy and speed with increasing cognitive demand. Pupil size increased with increasing difficulty for both the auditory and the visual task. In contrast, α power showed diverging neural dynamics: parietal α power decreased with increasing difficulty in the visual task, but not with increasing difficulty in the auditory task. Furthermore, independent of task difficulty, within-participant trial-by-trial fluctuations in pupil size were negatively correlated with α power. Difficulty-induced changes in pupil size and α power, however, did not correlate, which is consistent with their different cognitive-demand sensitivities. Overall, the current study demonstrates that the dynamics of the neurophysiological indices of cognitive demand and associated effort are multifaceted and potentially modality-dependent under complex audiovisual-task conditions. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Pupil size and oscillatory α power are associated with cognitive demand and effort, but their relative sensitivity under complex audiovisual-task conditions is unclear, as is the extent to which they share underlying mechanisms. Using an audiovisual dual-task paradigm, we show that pupil size increases with increasing cognitive demands for both audition and vision. In contrast, changes in oscillatory α power depend on the respective task demands: parietal α power decreases with visual demand but not with auditory task demand. Hence, pupil size and α power show different sensitivity to cognitive demands, perhaps suggesting partly different underlying neural mechanisms.
认知需求被认为可以调节瞳孔大小和神经α(8-12赫兹)振荡功率这两个经常使用但很少结合使用的测量指标。然而,在复杂的视听任务条件下,这两种测量方法是否以相似的方式捕捉认知需求尚不清楚。在此研究中,我们记录了瞳孔大小和神经α功率(使用脑电图),而男性和女性参与者同时执行视觉多目标跟踪任务和听觉间隙检测任务。这两项任务的难度是相互独立的。随着认知需求的增加,参与者的表现在准确性和速度上有所下降。瞳孔大小随着听觉和视觉任务难度的增加而增加。相比之下,α功率表现出分化的神经动力学特征:顶叶α功率随视觉任务难度的增加而降低,而不随听觉任务难度的增加而降低。此外,独立于任务难度,受试者内部的瞳孔大小波动与α幂呈负相关。然而,困难引起的瞳孔大小和α能力的变化并不相关,这与他们不同的认知需求敏感性是一致的。总的来说,目前的研究表明,在复杂的视听任务条件下,认知需求和相关努力的神经生理指标的动态是多方面的,并且可能依赖于模式。瞳孔大小和振荡α功率与认知需求和努力有关,但它们在复杂视听任务条件下的相对敏感性尚不清楚,它们共享潜在机制的程度也不清楚。使用视听双任务范式,我们发现瞳孔大小随着听觉和视觉认知需求的增加而增加。相反,振荡α功率的变化依赖于各自的任务需求:顶叶α功率随视觉任务需求而降低,而不随听觉任务需求而降低。因此,瞳孔大小和α能量对认知需求表现出不同的敏感性,这可能部分暗示了不同的潜在神经机制。
{"title":"Neural α Oscillations and Pupil Size Differentially Index Cognitive Demand under Competing Audiovisual Task Conditions","authors":"Frauke Kraus, Sarah Tune, J. Obleser, Björn Herrmann","doi":"10.1101/2022.11.25.517931","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.11.25.517931","url":null,"abstract":"Cognitive demand is thought to modulate two often used, but rarely combined, measures: pupil size and neural α (8–12 Hz) oscillatory power. However, it is unclear whether these two measures capture cognitive demand in a similar way under complex audiovisual-task conditions. Here we recorded pupil size and neural α power (using electroencephalography), while human participants of both sexes concurrently performed a visual multiple object-tracking task and an auditory gap detection task. Difficulties of the two tasks were manipulated independent of each other. Participants' performance decreased in accuracy and speed with increasing cognitive demand. Pupil size increased with increasing difficulty for both the auditory and the visual task. In contrast, α power showed diverging neural dynamics: parietal α power decreased with increasing difficulty in the visual task, but not with increasing difficulty in the auditory task. Furthermore, independent of task difficulty, within-participant trial-by-trial fluctuations in pupil size were negatively correlated with α power. Difficulty-induced changes in pupil size and α power, however, did not correlate, which is consistent with their different cognitive-demand sensitivities. Overall, the current study demonstrates that the dynamics of the neurophysiological indices of cognitive demand and associated effort are multifaceted and potentially modality-dependent under complex audiovisual-task conditions. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Pupil size and oscillatory α power are associated with cognitive demand and effort, but their relative sensitivity under complex audiovisual-task conditions is unclear, as is the extent to which they share underlying mechanisms. Using an audiovisual dual-task paradigm, we show that pupil size increases with increasing cognitive demands for both audition and vision. In contrast, changes in oscillatory α power depend on the respective task demands: parietal α power decreases with visual demand but not with auditory task demand. Hence, pupil size and α power show different sensitivity to cognitive demands, perhaps suggesting partly different underlying neural mechanisms.","PeriodicalId":22786,"journal":{"name":"The Journal of Neuroscience","volume":"36 1","pages":"4352 - 4364"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86833576","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
This Week in The Journal 本周华尔街日报
Pub Date : 2023-04-12 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.twij.43.15.2023
{"title":"This Week in The Journal","authors":"","doi":"10.1523/JNEUROSCI.twij.43.15.2023","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.twij.43.15.2023","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":22786,"journal":{"name":"The Journal of Neuroscience","volume":"65 1","pages":"2630 - 2630"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77153169","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Two Ascending Thermosensory Pathways from the Lateral Parabrachial Nucleus That Mediate Behavioral and Autonomous Thermoregulation 来自侧臂旁核的两条上行热感觉通路介导行为和自主热调节
Pub Date : 2023-04-11 DOI: 10.1101/2023.04.10.536301
Takaki Yahiro, N. Kataoka, Kazuhiro Nakamura
Thermoregulatory behavior in homeothermic animals is an innate behavior to defend body core temperature from environmental thermal challenges in coordination with autonomous thermoregulatory responses. In contrast to the progress in understanding the central mechanisms of autonomous thermoregulation, those of behavioral thermoregulation remain poorly understood. We have previously shown that the lateral parabrachial nucleus (LPB) mediates cutaneous thermosensory afferent signaling for thermoregulation. To understand the thermosensory neural network for behavioral thermoregulation, in the present study, we investigated the roles of ascending thermosensory pathways from the LPB in avoidance behavior from innocuous heat and cold in male rats. Neuronal tracing revealed two segregated groups of LPB neurons projecting to the median preoptic nucleus (MnPO), a thermoregulatory center (LPB→MnPO neurons), and those projecting to the central amygdaloid nucleus (CeA), a limbic emotion center (LPB→CeA neurons). While LPB→MnPO neurons include separate subgroups activated by heat or cold exposure of rats, LPB→CeA neurons were only activated by cold exposure. By selectively inhibiting LPB→MnPO or LPB→CeA neurons using tetanus toxin light chain or chemogenetic or optogenetic techniques, we found that LPB→MnPO transmission mediates heat avoidance, whereas LPB→CeA transmission contributes to cold avoidance. In vivo electrophysiological experiments showed that skin cooling-evoked thermogenesis in brown adipose tissue requires not only LPB→MnPO neurons but also LPB→CeA neurons, providing a novel insight into the central mechanism of autonomous thermoregulation. Our findings reveal an important framework of central thermosensory afferent pathways to coordinate behavioral and autonomous thermoregulation and to generate the emotions of thermal comfort and discomfort that drive thermoregulatory behavior. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Coordination of behavioral and autonomous thermoregulation is important for maintaining thermal homeostasis in homeothermic animals. However, the central mechanism of thermoregulatory behaviors remains poorly understood. We have previously shown that the lateral parabrachial nucleus (LPB) mediates ascending thermosensory signaling that drives thermoregulatory behavior. In this study, we found that one pathway from the LPB to the median preoptic nucleus mediates heat avoidance, whereas the other pathway from the LPB to the central amygdaloid nucleus is required for cold avoidance. Surprisingly, both pathways are required for skin cooling-evoked thermogenesis in brown adipose tissue, an autonomous thermoregulatory response. This study provides a central thermosensory network that coordinates behavioral and autonomous thermoregulation and generates thermal comfort and discomfort that drive thermoregulatory behavior.
恒温动物的体温调节行为是一种先天行为,以保护身体核心温度免受环境热挑战,并与自主的体温调节反应相协调。与了解自主体温调节的中心机制的进展相反,行为体温调节的机制仍然知之甚少。我们以前已经表明,外侧臂旁核(LPB)介导皮肤热感觉传入信号的体温调节。为了了解热感觉神经网络在行为体温调节中的作用,本研究研究了雄性大鼠LPB上行热感觉通路在躲避无害热和冷行为中的作用。神经元示迹显示两组LPB神经元分别投射到正中视前核(MnPO),一个温度调节中心(LPB→MnPO神经元)和投射到中央杏仁核(CeA),一个边缘情绪中心(LPB→CeA神经元)。LPB→MnPO神经元在大鼠热暴露和冷暴露下均有不同的激活亚组,而LPB→CeA神经元仅在冷暴露下激活。通过破伤风毒素轻链或化学遗传或光遗传技术选择性抑制LPB→MnPO或LPB→CeA神经元,我们发现LPB→MnPO传递介导热回避,而LPB→CeA传递介导冷回避。体内电生理实验表明,皮肤冷却诱导的棕色脂肪组织产热不仅需要LPB→MnPO神经元,还需要LPB→CeA神经元,这为自主体温调节的核心机制提供了新的见解。我们的研究结果揭示了中枢热感觉传入通路的重要框架,以协调行为和自主热调节,并产生驱动热调节行为的热舒适和不适情绪。在恒温动物中,行为和自主热调节的协调对于维持热稳态至关重要。然而,体温调节行为的核心机制仍然知之甚少。我们之前已经表明,外侧臂旁核(LPB)介导上行的热感觉信号,驱动热调节行为。在本研究中,我们发现从LPB到正中视前核的一条通路介导热回避,而从LPB到中央杏仁核的另一条通路则是冷回避所必需的。令人惊讶的是,这两种途径都是皮肤冷却引起的棕色脂肪组织产热所必需的,这是一种自主的体温调节反应。本研究提供了一个协调行为和自主体温调节的中央热感觉网络,并产生驱动体温调节行为的热舒适和不适。
{"title":"Two Ascending Thermosensory Pathways from the Lateral Parabrachial Nucleus That Mediate Behavioral and Autonomous Thermoregulation","authors":"Takaki Yahiro, N. Kataoka, Kazuhiro Nakamura","doi":"10.1101/2023.04.10.536301","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.04.10.536301","url":null,"abstract":"Thermoregulatory behavior in homeothermic animals is an innate behavior to defend body core temperature from environmental thermal challenges in coordination with autonomous thermoregulatory responses. In contrast to the progress in understanding the central mechanisms of autonomous thermoregulation, those of behavioral thermoregulation remain poorly understood. We have previously shown that the lateral parabrachial nucleus (LPB) mediates cutaneous thermosensory afferent signaling for thermoregulation. To understand the thermosensory neural network for behavioral thermoregulation, in the present study, we investigated the roles of ascending thermosensory pathways from the LPB in avoidance behavior from innocuous heat and cold in male rats. Neuronal tracing revealed two segregated groups of LPB neurons projecting to the median preoptic nucleus (MnPO), a thermoregulatory center (LPB→MnPO neurons), and those projecting to the central amygdaloid nucleus (CeA), a limbic emotion center (LPB→CeA neurons). While LPB→MnPO neurons include separate subgroups activated by heat or cold exposure of rats, LPB→CeA neurons were only activated by cold exposure. By selectively inhibiting LPB→MnPO or LPB→CeA neurons using tetanus toxin light chain or chemogenetic or optogenetic techniques, we found that LPB→MnPO transmission mediates heat avoidance, whereas LPB→CeA transmission contributes to cold avoidance. In vivo electrophysiological experiments showed that skin cooling-evoked thermogenesis in brown adipose tissue requires not only LPB→MnPO neurons but also LPB→CeA neurons, providing a novel insight into the central mechanism of autonomous thermoregulation. Our findings reveal an important framework of central thermosensory afferent pathways to coordinate behavioral and autonomous thermoregulation and to generate the emotions of thermal comfort and discomfort that drive thermoregulatory behavior. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Coordination of behavioral and autonomous thermoregulation is important for maintaining thermal homeostasis in homeothermic animals. However, the central mechanism of thermoregulatory behaviors remains poorly understood. We have previously shown that the lateral parabrachial nucleus (LPB) mediates ascending thermosensory signaling that drives thermoregulatory behavior. In this study, we found that one pathway from the LPB to the median preoptic nucleus mediates heat avoidance, whereas the other pathway from the LPB to the central amygdaloid nucleus is required for cold avoidance. Surprisingly, both pathways are required for skin cooling-evoked thermogenesis in brown adipose tissue, an autonomous thermoregulatory response. This study provides a central thermosensory network that coordinates behavioral and autonomous thermoregulation and generates thermal comfort and discomfort that drive thermoregulatory behavior.","PeriodicalId":22786,"journal":{"name":"The Journal of Neuroscience","volume":"37 1","pages":"5221 - 5240"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"82842948","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
High-Fidelity Reproduction of Visual Signals by Electrical Stimulation in the Central Primate Retina 电刺激下灵长类中央视网膜视觉信号的高保真再现
Pub Date : 2023-04-10 DOI: 10.1101/2022.05.24.493162
A. Gogliettino, Sasidhar S. Madugula, Lauren E. Grosberg, R. Vilkhu, Jeff B Brown, Huy Nguyen, Alexandra Kling, P. Hottowy, W. Dąbrowski, A. Sher, A. Litke, E. Chichilnisky
Electrical stimulation of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) with electronic implants provides rudimentary artificial vision to people blinded by retinal degeneration. However, current devices stimulate indiscriminately and therefore cannot reproduce the intricate neural code of the retina. Recent work has demonstrated more precise activation of RGCs using focal electrical stimulation with multielectrode arrays in the peripheral macaque retina, but it is unclear how effective this can be in the central retina, which is required for high-resolution vision. This work probes the neural code and effectiveness of focal epiretinal stimulation in the central macaque retina, using large-scale electrical recording and stimulation ex vivo. The functional organization, light response properties, and electrical properties of the major RGC types in the central retina were mostly similar to the peripheral retina, with some notable differences in density, kinetics, linearity, spiking statistics, and correlations. The major RGC types could be distinguished by their intrinsic electrical properties. Electrical stimulation targeting parasol cells revealed similar activation thresholds and reduced axon bundle activation in the central retina, but lower stimulation selectivity. Quantitative evaluation of the potential for image reconstruction from electrically evoked parasol cell signals revealed higher overall expected image quality in the central retina. An exploration of inadvertent midget cell activation suggested that it could contribute high spatial frequency noise to the visual signal carried by parasol cells. These results support the possibility of reproducing high-acuity visual signals in the central retina with an epiretinal implant. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Artificial restoration of vision with retinal implants is a major treatment for blindness. However, present-day implants do not provide high-resolution visual perception, in part because they do not reproduce the natural neural code of the retina. Here, we demonstrate the level of visual signal reproduction that is possible with a future implant by examining how accurately responses to electrical stimulation of parasol retinal ganglion cells can convey visual signals. Although the precision of electrical stimulation in the central retina was diminished relative to the peripheral retina, the quality of expected visual signal reconstruction in parasol cells was greater. These findings suggest that visual signals could be restored with high fidelity in the central retina using a future retinal implant.
利用电子植入物对视网膜神经节细胞(RGCs)进行电刺激,为因视网膜变性而失明的人提供初步的人工视觉。然而,目前的设备不加选择地刺激,因此不能复制视网膜复杂的神经密码。最近的研究表明,在猕猴外围视网膜上使用多电极阵列的局部电刺激可以更精确地激活RGCs,但尚不清楚这种方法在中央视网膜上的效果如何,而中央视网膜是高分辨率视力所必需的。本研究通过大规模电记录和体外刺激,探讨了猕猴中央视网膜局灶性视网膜前刺激的神经编码和有效性。主要RGC类型在中央视网膜的功能组织、光响应特性和电学特性与周围视网膜基本相似,但在密度、动力学、线性、峰值统计和相关性方面存在显著差异。主要的研资局类型可以根据其固有的电学性质来区分。针对遮阳伞细胞的电刺激显示出类似的激活阈值,并降低了视网膜中央轴突束的激活,但刺激选择性较低。对电诱发阳伞细胞信号的图像重建潜力的定量评估显示,中央视网膜的整体预期图像质量更高。对小细胞无意激活的探索表明,它可能对阳伞细胞携带的视觉信号产生高空间频率噪声。这些结果支持在视网膜前植入的中央视网膜中再现高灵敏度视觉信号的可能性。意义声明视网膜植入物人工恢复视力是治疗失明的主要方法。然而,目前的植入物不能提供高分辨率的视觉感知,部分原因是它们不能复制视网膜的自然神经编码。在这里,我们通过检查阳伞视网膜神经节细胞对电刺激的反应如何准确地传递视觉信号,证明了视觉信号复制水平在未来植入物中是可能的。虽然电刺激在中央视网膜的精度相对于周围视网膜降低,但在遮阳伞细胞中预期的视觉信号重建质量更高。这些发现表明,使用未来的视网膜植入物可以高保真地恢复中央视网膜的视觉信号。
{"title":"High-Fidelity Reproduction of Visual Signals by Electrical Stimulation in the Central Primate Retina","authors":"A. Gogliettino, Sasidhar S. Madugula, Lauren E. Grosberg, R. Vilkhu, Jeff B Brown, Huy Nguyen, Alexandra Kling, P. Hottowy, W. Dąbrowski, A. Sher, A. Litke, E. Chichilnisky","doi":"10.1101/2022.05.24.493162","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.05.24.493162","url":null,"abstract":"Electrical stimulation of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) with electronic implants provides rudimentary artificial vision to people blinded by retinal degeneration. However, current devices stimulate indiscriminately and therefore cannot reproduce the intricate neural code of the retina. Recent work has demonstrated more precise activation of RGCs using focal electrical stimulation with multielectrode arrays in the peripheral macaque retina, but it is unclear how effective this can be in the central retina, which is required for high-resolution vision. This work probes the neural code and effectiveness of focal epiretinal stimulation in the central macaque retina, using large-scale electrical recording and stimulation ex vivo. The functional organization, light response properties, and electrical properties of the major RGC types in the central retina were mostly similar to the peripheral retina, with some notable differences in density, kinetics, linearity, spiking statistics, and correlations. The major RGC types could be distinguished by their intrinsic electrical properties. Electrical stimulation targeting parasol cells revealed similar activation thresholds and reduced axon bundle activation in the central retina, but lower stimulation selectivity. Quantitative evaluation of the potential for image reconstruction from electrically evoked parasol cell signals revealed higher overall expected image quality in the central retina. An exploration of inadvertent midget cell activation suggested that it could contribute high spatial frequency noise to the visual signal carried by parasol cells. These results support the possibility of reproducing high-acuity visual signals in the central retina with an epiretinal implant. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Artificial restoration of vision with retinal implants is a major treatment for blindness. However, present-day implants do not provide high-resolution visual perception, in part because they do not reproduce the natural neural code of the retina. Here, we demonstrate the level of visual signal reproduction that is possible with a future implant by examining how accurately responses to electrical stimulation of parasol retinal ganglion cells can convey visual signals. Although the precision of electrical stimulation in the central retina was diminished relative to the peripheral retina, the quality of expected visual signal reconstruction in parasol cells was greater. These findings suggest that visual signals could be restored with high fidelity in the central retina using a future retinal implant.","PeriodicalId":22786,"journal":{"name":"The Journal of Neuroscience","volume":"21 1","pages":"4625 - 4641"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77771349","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
This Week in The Journal 本周华尔街日报
Pub Date : 2023-04-05 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.twij.43.14.2023
{"title":"This Week in The Journal","authors":"","doi":"10.1523/JNEUROSCI.twij.43.14.2023","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.twij.43.14.2023","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":22786,"journal":{"name":"The Journal of Neuroscience","volume":"164 1","pages":"2439 - 2439"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86420463","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neural Representations in Visual and Parietal Cortex Differentiate between Imagined, Perceived, and Illusory Experiences 视觉和顶叶皮层的神经表征区分想象、感知和虚幻体验
Pub Date : 2023-03-31 DOI: 10.1101/2023.03.31.535014
Siyi Li, Xue-ling Zeng, Zhujun Shao, Qing Yu
Humans constantly receive massive amounts of information, both perceived from the external environment and imagined from the internal world. To function properly, the brain needs to correctly identify the origin of information being processed. Recent work has suggested common neural substrates for perception and imagery. However, it has remained unclear how the brain differentiates between external and internal experiences with shared neural codes. Here we tested this question in human participants (male and female) by systematically investigating the neural processes underlying the generation and maintenance of visual information from voluntary imagery, veridical perception, and illusion. The inclusion of illusion allowed us to differentiate between objective and subjective internality: while illusion has an objectively internal origin and can be viewed as involuntary imagery, it is also subjectively perceived as having an external origin like perception. Combining fMRI, eye-tracking, multivariate decoding, and encoding approaches, we observed superior orientation representations in parietal cortex during imagery compared with perception, and conversely in early visual cortex. This imagery dominance gradually developed along a posterior-to-anterior cortical hierarchy from early visual to parietal cortex, emerged in the early epoch of imagery and sustained into the delay epoch, and persisted across varied imagined contents. Moreover, representational strength of illusion was more comparable to imagery in early visual cortex, but more comparable to perception in parietal cortex, suggesting content-specific representations in parietal cortex differentiate between subjectively internal and external experiences, as opposed to early visual cortex. These findings together support a domain-general engagement of parietal cortex in internally generated experience. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT How does the brain differentiate between imagined and perceived experiences? Combining fMRI, eye-tracking, multivariate decoding, and encoding approaches, the current study revealed enhanced stimulus-specific representations in visual imagery originating from parietal cortex, supporting the subjective experience of imagery. This neural principle was further validated by evidence from visual illusion, wherein illusion resembled perception and imagery at different levels of cortical hierarchy. Our findings provide direct evidence for the critical role of parietal cortex as a domain-general region for content-specific imagery, and offer new insights into the neural mechanisms underlying the differentiation between subjectively internal and external experiences.
人类不断地接收大量的信息,这些信息既有来自外部环境的感知,也有来自内部世界的想象。为了正常工作,大脑需要正确识别正在处理的信息的来源。最近的研究表明知觉和意象的共同神经基质。然而,目前还不清楚大脑如何区分共享神经代码的外部和内部体验。在这里,我们通过系统地研究从自愿意象、真实感知和幻觉中产生和维持视觉信息的神经过程,在人类参与者(男性和女性)中测试了这个问题。错觉的包含使我们能够区分客观和主观的内在性:虽然错觉有客观的内部起源,可以被视为无意识的意象,但它也被主观上认为具有像感知一样的外部起源。结合fMRI、眼动追踪、多变量解码和编码方法,我们观察到在成像过程中,顶叶皮层的定向表征优于感知,而在早期视觉皮层中相反。从早期视觉皮层到顶叶皮层,这种意象优势沿着后-前皮层层次逐渐发展,出现于意象早期,持续到延迟期,并在各种想象内容中持续存在。此外,幻觉的表征强度与早期视觉皮层的图像更相似,但与顶叶皮层的感知更相似,这表明与早期视觉皮层相反,顶叶皮层的内容特异性表征区分主观上的内部和外部体验。这些发现共同支持顶叶皮层在内部生成经验中的域一般参与。大脑是如何区分想象体验和感知体验的?结合fMRI、眼动追踪、多元解码和编码方法,目前的研究揭示了视觉图像中源自顶叶皮层的刺激特异性表征增强,支持图像的主观体验。视觉错觉的证据进一步证实了这一神经原理,其中幻觉在不同的皮层层次上类似于感知和图像。我们的研究结果为顶叶皮层作为特定内容意象的域一般区域的关键作用提供了直接证据,并为主观内部和外部体验之间差异的神经机制提供了新的见解。
{"title":"Neural Representations in Visual and Parietal Cortex Differentiate between Imagined, Perceived, and Illusory Experiences","authors":"Siyi Li, Xue-ling Zeng, Zhujun Shao, Qing Yu","doi":"10.1101/2023.03.31.535014","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.03.31.535014","url":null,"abstract":"Humans constantly receive massive amounts of information, both perceived from the external environment and imagined from the internal world. To function properly, the brain needs to correctly identify the origin of information being processed. Recent work has suggested common neural substrates for perception and imagery. However, it has remained unclear how the brain differentiates between external and internal experiences with shared neural codes. Here we tested this question in human participants (male and female) by systematically investigating the neural processes underlying the generation and maintenance of visual information from voluntary imagery, veridical perception, and illusion. The inclusion of illusion allowed us to differentiate between objective and subjective internality: while illusion has an objectively internal origin and can be viewed as involuntary imagery, it is also subjectively perceived as having an external origin like perception. Combining fMRI, eye-tracking, multivariate decoding, and encoding approaches, we observed superior orientation representations in parietal cortex during imagery compared with perception, and conversely in early visual cortex. This imagery dominance gradually developed along a posterior-to-anterior cortical hierarchy from early visual to parietal cortex, emerged in the early epoch of imagery and sustained into the delay epoch, and persisted across varied imagined contents. Moreover, representational strength of illusion was more comparable to imagery in early visual cortex, but more comparable to perception in parietal cortex, suggesting content-specific representations in parietal cortex differentiate between subjectively internal and external experiences, as opposed to early visual cortex. These findings together support a domain-general engagement of parietal cortex in internally generated experience. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT How does the brain differentiate between imagined and perceived experiences? Combining fMRI, eye-tracking, multivariate decoding, and encoding approaches, the current study revealed enhanced stimulus-specific representations in visual imagery originating from parietal cortex, supporting the subjective experience of imagery. This neural principle was further validated by evidence from visual illusion, wherein illusion resembled perception and imagery at different levels of cortical hierarchy. Our findings provide direct evidence for the critical role of parietal cortex as a domain-general region for content-specific imagery, and offer new insights into the neural mechanisms underlying the differentiation between subjectively internal and external experiences.","PeriodicalId":22786,"journal":{"name":"The Journal of Neuroscience","volume":"44 1","pages":"6508 - 6524"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85292799","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
The Journal of Neuroscience
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1