首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Trial and Error最新文献

英文 中文
The GLORIA adherence subproject: problems and randomization mistakes GLORIA依从性子项目:问题和随机化错误
Pub Date : 2021-10-18 DOI: 10.36850/e6
L. Hartman, M. Kok, E. Molenaar, E. Griep, J. Laar, J. M. Woerkom, C. Allaart, H. Raterman, Y. Ruiterman, M. Voshaar, J. Redol, R. Pinto, L. Klausch, W. Lems, M. Boers
Medication adherence, which is the extent to which patients take their medication as prescribed, is essential in treating chronic inflammatory diseases such as rheumatoid arthritis (RA). Therefore, we nested a subproject in the two-year multicenter Glucocorticoid Low-dose Outcome in Rheumatoid Arthritis (GLORIA) trial to add a low-dose prednisolone (5 mg/day) or placebo to the standard care in older people (≥65 years) with RA. Adherence was measured with an electronic monitoring cap that recorded bottle openings in all patients. In the subproject, we performed an adherence intervention with an advanced cap that could communicate with an application on the smart device via Bluetooth. We randomized patients with a smart device to receive or not to receive adherence reminders on the smart device for three months. Multiple problems emerged that precluded an answer to the research question: sample size (overly optimistic estimates of older patients with a smart device), logistic issues (availability of smartcaps, data extraction), randomization and treatment allocation errors (despite training of personnel), and low quality of the data in the intervention group (hardware failure, discovered too late because data was read in batches). For future trials planning to include a subproject, we recommend keeping it simple, starting with a field test before the actual study starts, and monitoring data from the beginning of the study.
药物依从性,即患者按处方服药的程度,在治疗慢性炎症性疾病(如类风湿关节炎)中至关重要。因此,我们在为期两年的多中心类风湿性关节炎低剂量糖皮质激素结局(GLORIA)试验中建立了一个子项目,在老年人(≥65岁)RA患者的标准治疗中添加低剂量强的松龙(5mg /天)或安慰剂。使用电子监测帽来测量依从性,记录所有患者的药瓶开口。在子项目中,我们使用了一种高级帽,可以通过蓝牙与智能设备上的应用程序进行通信。我们随机选择使用智能设备的患者,让他们在三个月内接受或不接受智能设备上的依从提醒。出现了多个问题,排除了研究问题的答案:样本量(对使用智能设备的老年患者的过于乐观的估计),后勤问题(智能帽的可用性,数据提取),随机化和治疗分配错误(尽管人员进行了培训),以及干预组的数据质量低(硬件故障,发现太晚,因为数据是分批读取的)。对于将来计划包括子项目的试验,我们建议保持简单,在实际研究开始之前从现场测试开始,并从研究开始时监测数据。
{"title":"The GLORIA adherence subproject: problems and randomization mistakes","authors":"L. Hartman, M. Kok, E. Molenaar, E. Griep, J. Laar, J. M. Woerkom, C. Allaart, H. Raterman, Y. Ruiterman, M. Voshaar, J. Redol, R. Pinto, L. Klausch, W. Lems, M. Boers","doi":"10.36850/e6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.36850/e6","url":null,"abstract":"Medication adherence, which is the extent to which patients take their medication as prescribed, is essential in treating chronic inflammatory diseases such as rheumatoid arthritis (RA). Therefore, we nested a subproject in the two-year multicenter Glucocorticoid Low-dose Outcome in Rheumatoid Arthritis (GLORIA) trial to add a low-dose prednisolone (5 mg/day) or placebo to the standard care in older people (≥65 years) with RA. Adherence was measured with an electronic monitoring cap that recorded bottle openings in all patients. In the subproject, we performed an adherence intervention with an advanced cap that could communicate with an application on the smart device via Bluetooth. We randomized patients with a smart device to receive or not to receive adherence reminders on the smart device for three months. Multiple problems emerged that precluded an answer to the research question: sample size (overly optimistic estimates of older patients with a smart device), logistic issues (availability of smartcaps, data extraction), randomization and treatment allocation errors (despite training of personnel), and low quality of the data in the intervention group (hardware failure, discovered too late because data was read in batches). For future trials planning to include a subproject, we recommend keeping it simple, starting with a field test before the actual study starts, and monitoring data from the beginning of the study.","PeriodicalId":275817,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Trial and Error","volume":"15 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"125459564","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Real-effort survey designs: Open-ended questions to overcome the challenge of measuring behavior in surveys 真正努力的调查设计:开放式问题,以克服在调查中测量行为的挑战
Pub Date : 2021-09-10 DOI: 10.36850/e5
Carolin Fischer
Based on data triangulation, open-ended questions can be used to overcome a typical problem with data collection using surveys: Human behavior can only be captured as stated or intended, but not as real behavior. In this study on knowledge sharing in the workplace, a quantitative measure of behavioral intention was accompanied by such a qualitative, open-ended measure of behavior. The latter was used as a proxy for real instead of stated behavior. This item was coded according to the effort a participant made in answering. It is assumed that the greater the effort put into answering the open-ended question, the more likely it is that the described behavior will be performed in reality. A factorial experimental design was used to analyze the effect of rewards on employees’ knowledge-sharing behavior. As a within-subject design was used, participants had to answer three open-ended questions referring to different vignettes. A strong order effect appeared, leading to longer answers on average for the first vignette (baseline) compared to subsequent vignettes, independent of treatment. Therefore, this approach to operationalizing behavior in surveys might not be useful in within-subject designs. However, it can be used in between-subject comparisons when participants are asked to answer to a single vignette.Keywords: survey design, survey experiment, real-effort design, human behavior, order effect
基于数据三角测量,开放式问题可用于克服使用调查收集数据的典型问题:人类行为只能按照陈述或预期捕获,而不能作为真实行为捕获。在这项关于工作场所知识共享的研究中,行为意图的定量测量伴随着这种定性的、开放式的行为测量。后者被用作真实行为而不是陈述行为的代理。这个项目是根据参与者在回答中所做的努力进行编码的。假设在回答开放式问题上付出的努力越大,所描述的行为就越有可能在现实中发生。采用因子实验设计分析了奖励对员工知识共享行为的影响。由于采用了主题内设计,参与者必须回答三个涉及不同插图的开放式问题。出现了强烈的顺序效应,导致第一个小插曲(基线)的平均回答时间比随后的小插曲更长,与治疗无关。因此,这种在调查中操作化行为的方法可能不适用于主题内设计。然而,当参与者被要求回答一个小插曲时,它可以用于受试者之间的比较。关键词:调查设计,调查实验,实绩设计,人的行为,秩序效应
{"title":"Real-effort survey designs: Open-ended questions to overcome the challenge of measuring behavior in surveys","authors":"Carolin Fischer","doi":"10.36850/e5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.36850/e5","url":null,"abstract":"Based on data triangulation, open-ended questions can be used to overcome a typical problem with data collection using surveys: Human behavior can only be captured as stated or intended, but not as real behavior. In this study on knowledge sharing in the workplace, a quantitative measure of behavioral intention was accompanied by such a qualitative, open-ended measure of behavior. The latter was used as a proxy for real instead of stated behavior. This item was coded according to the effort a participant made in answering. It is assumed that the greater the effort put into answering the open-ended question, the more likely it is that the described behavior will be performed in reality. A factorial experimental design was used to analyze the effect of rewards on employees’ knowledge-sharing behavior. As a within-subject design was used, participants had to answer three open-ended questions referring to different vignettes. A strong order effect appeared, leading to longer answers on average for the first vignette (baseline) compared to subsequent vignettes, independent of treatment. Therefore, this approach to operationalizing behavior in surveys might not be useful in within-subject designs. However, it can be used in between-subject comparisons when participants are asked to answer to a single vignette.Keywords: survey design, survey experiment, real-effort design, human behavior, order effect","PeriodicalId":275817,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Trial and Error","volume":"10 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"133303213","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rewilding Cognition: Complex Dynamics in Open Experimental Systems 认知重塑:开放实验系统中的复杂动力学
Pub Date : 2021-08-19 DOI: 10.36850/e4
Wendy Ross, F. Vallée‐Tourangeau
Insight problems are sometimes designed to encourage an incorrect and misleading interpretation that veils a simple answer. The socks problem is one such problem: Given black socks and brown socks in a drawer mixed in a ratio of four to five, how many socks will you have to take out to make sure that you have a pair of the same color? The ratio information is misleading since, with only two colors, pulling three socks will guarantee a matching pair. Recently, offered a distinction between first- and second-order problem-solving: The former proceeds with and through a physical model of the problem, while the latter proceeds in the absence of such interactions with the world, in other words on the basis of mental processes alone. Vallée-Tourangeau and March also proposed a thought experiment, suggesting that the ratio information in the socks problem might be quickly abandoned in a first-order environment, that is, one where participants observe the results of drawing socks out of a bag rather than imagining themselves doing so. We tested this prediction by randomly allocating participants to a low- (second-order) or high- (first-order) interactivity condition. Marginally more participants announced the correct answer within a 5-minute period in the high than in the low condition, although the difference was not significant. Detailed analysis of the video recording revealed the challenges of operationalizing a second-order condition, as participants engaged in dialogical interactions with the experimenter. In addition, the manner in which the high-interactivity condition was designed appeared to encourage the physical reification of the misleading ratio, thus anchoring that information more firmly rather than defusing it through interactivity. We close the paper with some reflections on wide, or systemic, cognition in experimental research on creative problem-solving.
洞察力问题有时被设计成鼓励一个不正确和误导性的解释,掩盖了一个简单的答案。袜子问题就是这样一个问题:在一个抽屉里,黑色袜子和棕色袜子以四比五的比例混合在一起,你需要拿出多少只袜子才能确保你有一双相同颜色的袜子?比例信息是误导的,因为只有两种颜色,抽出三只袜子就一定能得到一双。最近,他提出了一阶和二阶问题解决之间的区别:前者通过问题的物理模型进行解决,而后者在没有这种与世界的互动的情况下进行,换句话说,仅基于心理过程。vallsame - tourangeau和March还提出了一个思想实验,表明袜子问题中的比例信息在一阶环境中可能很快就会被抛弃,也就是说,在一阶环境中,参与者观察从袋子里拿出袜子的结果,而不是想象自己这样做。我们通过将参与者随机分配到低(二阶)或高(一阶)交互条件来测试这一预测。在高条件下,在5分钟内宣布正确答案的参与者比在低条件下稍微多一些,尽管差异并不显著。对录像的详细分析揭示了当参与者与实验者进行对话互动时,操作二级条件的挑战。此外,高互动性条件的设计方式似乎鼓励了误导比例的物理具体化,从而更牢固地锚定了信息,而不是通过互动性来消除它。最后,我们对创造性问题解决实验研究中的广泛或系统认知进行了一些反思。
{"title":"Rewilding Cognition: Complex Dynamics in Open Experimental Systems","authors":"Wendy Ross, F. Vallée‐Tourangeau","doi":"10.36850/e4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.36850/e4","url":null,"abstract":"Insight problems are sometimes designed to encourage an incorrect and misleading interpretation that veils a simple answer. The socks problem is one such problem: Given black socks and brown socks in a drawer mixed in a ratio of four to five, how many socks will you have to take out to make sure that you have a pair of the same color? The ratio information is misleading since, with only two colors, pulling three socks will guarantee a matching pair. Recently, offered a distinction between first- and second-order problem-solving: The former proceeds with and through a physical model of the problem, while the latter proceeds in the absence of such interactions with the world, in other words on the basis of mental processes alone. Vallée-Tourangeau and March also proposed a thought experiment, suggesting that the ratio information in the socks problem might be quickly abandoned in a first-order environment, that is, one where participants observe the results of drawing socks out of a bag rather than imagining themselves doing so. We tested this prediction by randomly allocating participants to a low- (second-order) or high- (first-order) interactivity condition. Marginally more participants announced the correct answer within a 5-minute period in the high than in the low condition, although the difference was not significant. Detailed analysis of the video recording revealed the challenges of operationalizing a second-order condition, as participants engaged in dialogical interactions with the experimenter. In addition, the manner in which the high-interactivity condition was designed appeared to encourage the physical reification of the misleading ratio, thus anchoring that information more firmly rather than defusing it through interactivity. We close the paper with some reflections on wide, or systemic, cognition in experimental research on creative problem-solving.","PeriodicalId":275817,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Trial and Error","volume":"8 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"130135234","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
An Introduction to Complementary Explanation 补充解释导论
Pub Date : 2021-04-08 DOI: 10.31235/osf.io/tvcb4
Joeri van Hugten
Complementary explanation is an approach to falsify more theories. This counteracts the current bias toward supporting theories.
补充解释是一种可以证伪更多理论的方法。这抵消了目前对支持理论的偏见。
{"title":"An Introduction to Complementary Explanation","authors":"Joeri van Hugten","doi":"10.31235/osf.io/tvcb4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/tvcb4","url":null,"abstract":"Complementary explanation is an approach to falsify more theories. This counteracts the current bias toward supporting theories.","PeriodicalId":275817,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Trial and Error","volume":"10 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"129365386","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Complexity of Joint Regeneration: How an Advanced Implant could Fail by Its In Vivo Proven Bone Component 关节再生的复杂性:一个先进的植入物如何在体内被证实的骨成分失败
Pub Date : 2021-03-23 DOI: 10.36850/E3
P. Diloksumpan, F. Abinzano, M. de Ruijter, A. Mensinga, S. Plomp, I. Khan, H. Brommer, I. Smit, Miguel Dias Castilho, P. R. van Weeren, J. Malda, R. Levato
Articular cartilage damage is a major challenge in healthcare due to the lack of long-term repair options. There are several promising regenerative implant-based approaches for the treatment, but the fixation of the implant remains a significant challenge. This study evaluated the potential for repair of an osteochondral implant produced through a novel combined bioprinting-based chondral-bone integration, with and without cells, in an equine model. Implants consisted of a melt electrowritten polycaprolactone (PCL) framework for the chondral compartment, which was firmly integrated with a bone anchor. The bone anchor was produced by extrusion-based printing of a low-temperature setting bioceramic material that had been proven to be effective for osteo-regeneration in an orthotopic, non-load bearing and non-articular site in the same species in an earlier in vivo study. Articular cartilage-derived progenitor cells were seeded into the PCL framework and cultured for 28 days in vitro in the presence of bone morphogenetic protein-9 (BMP-9), resulting in the formation of abundant extracellular matrix rich in glycosaminoglycans (GAGs) and type II collagen. The constructs were implanted in the stifle joints of Shetland ponies with cell-free scaffolds as controls. Clinical signs were monitored, and progression of healing was observed non-invasively through radiographic examinations and quantitative gait analysis. Biochemical and histological analyses 6 months after implantation revealed minimal deposition of GAGs and type II collagen in the chondral compartment of the defect site for both types of implants. Quantitative micro-computed tomography showed collapse of the bone anchor with low volume of mineralized neo-bone formation in both groups. Histology confirmed that the PCL framework within the chondral compartment was still present. It was concluded that the collapse of the osteal anchor, resulting in loss of the mechanical support of the chondral compartment, strongly affected overall outcome, precluding evaluation of the influence of BMP-9 stimulated cells on in vivo cartilage regeneration.
由于缺乏长期的修复选择,关节软骨损伤是医疗保健的主要挑战。有几种很有前途的基于再生种植体的治疗方法,但是种植体的固定仍然是一个重大的挑战。本研究评估了在马模型中,通过一种新型的基于生物打印的软骨-骨结合(有和没有细胞)产生的骨软骨植入物的修复潜力。植入物包括用于软骨室的熔融电写聚己内酯(PCL)框架,该框架与骨锚牢固地结合在一起。该骨锚是由低温固化生物陶瓷材料挤压打印而成,该材料在早期的体内研究中已被证明对同一物种的正位、非承重和非关节部位的骨再生有效。将关节软骨源性祖细胞植入PCL框架,在骨形态发生蛋白-9 (BMP-9)存在下体外培养28天,形成丰富的富含糖胺聚糖(GAGs)和II型胶原的细胞外基质。将这些结构植入设得兰矮种马的膝关节,并以无细胞支架作为对照。监测临床症状,并通过x线检查和定量步态分析无创观察愈合进展。植入后6个月的生化和组织学分析显示,两种类型的植入物在缺损部位的软骨腔室中都有少量的GAGs和II型胶原沉积。定量显微计算机断层扫描显示两组骨锚塌陷,矿化新生骨形成体积小。组织学证实软骨腔室内的PCL框架仍然存在。由此得出结论,骨锚的塌陷导致软骨间室的机械支持丧失,严重影响了整体结果,因此无法评估BMP-9刺激细胞对体内软骨再生的影响。
{"title":"The Complexity of Joint Regeneration: How an Advanced Implant could Fail by Its In Vivo Proven Bone Component","authors":"P. Diloksumpan, F. Abinzano, M. de Ruijter, A. Mensinga, S. Plomp, I. Khan, H. Brommer, I. Smit, Miguel Dias Castilho, P. R. van Weeren, J. Malda, R. Levato","doi":"10.36850/E3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.36850/E3","url":null,"abstract":"Articular cartilage damage is a major challenge in healthcare due to the lack of long-term repair options. There are several promising regenerative implant-based approaches for the treatment, but the fixation of the implant remains a significant challenge. This study evaluated the potential for repair of an osteochondral implant produced through a novel combined bioprinting-based chondral-bone integration, with and without cells, in an equine model. Implants consisted of a melt electrowritten polycaprolactone (PCL) framework for the chondral compartment, which was firmly integrated with a bone anchor. The bone anchor was produced by extrusion-based printing of a low-temperature setting bioceramic material that had been proven to be effective for osteo-regeneration in an orthotopic, non-load bearing and non-articular site in the same species in an earlier in vivo study. Articular cartilage-derived progenitor cells were seeded into the PCL framework and cultured for 28 days in vitro in the presence of bone morphogenetic protein-9 (BMP-9), resulting in the formation of abundant extracellular matrix rich in glycosaminoglycans (GAGs) and type II collagen. The constructs were implanted in the stifle joints of Shetland ponies with cell-free scaffolds as controls. Clinical signs were monitored, and progression of healing was observed non-invasively through radiographic examinations and quantitative gait analysis. Biochemical and histological analyses 6 months after implantation revealed minimal deposition of GAGs and type II collagen in the chondral compartment of the defect site for both types of implants. Quantitative micro-computed tomography showed collapse of the bone anchor with low volume of mineralized neo-bone formation in both groups. Histology confirmed that the PCL framework within the chondral compartment was still present. It was concluded that the collapse of the osteal anchor, resulting in loss of the mechanical support of the chondral compartment, strongly affected overall outcome, precluding evaluation of the influence of BMP-9 stimulated cells on in vivo cartilage regeneration.","PeriodicalId":275817,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Trial and Error","volume":"5 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-03-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"127412215","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Peer Review of "Trial and Error (-Related Negativity): An Odyssey of Integrating Different Experimental Paradigms “试错(相关消极性):整合不同实验范式的奥德赛”的同行评议
Pub Date : 2020-12-16 DOI: 10.36850/e2.pr1
Ilona Domen, K. Groot
Pain can be considered as a signal of “bodily error”: Errors – discrepancies between actual and optimal/targeted state – can put organisms at danger and activate behavioral defensive systems. If the error relates to the body, pain is the warning signal that motivates protective action such as avoidance behavior to safeguard our body’s integrity. Hence, pain shares the functionality of errors. On the neural level, an important error processing component is the error-related negativity (ERN), a negative deflection in the electroencephalographic (EEG) signal generated primarily in the anterior cingulate cortex within 100 ms after error commission. Despite compelling evidence that the ERN plays an important role in the development of various psychopathologies and is implicated in learning and adjustment of behavior, its relation to pain-related avoidance has not yet been examined. Based on findings from anxiety research, it seems conceivable that individuals with elevated ERN amplitudes are more prone to engage in pain-related avoidance behavior, which may, under certain conditions, be a risk factor for developing chronic pain. Consequently, this new line of research promises to contribute to our understanding of human pain. As in most novel research areas, a first crucial step for integrating the scientific fields of ERN and pain is developing a paradigm suited to address the needs from both fields. The present manuscript presents the development and piloting of an experimental task measuring both ERN and avoidance behavior in response to painful mistakes, as well as the challenges encountered herein. A total of 12 participants underwent one of six different task versions. We describe in detail each of these versions, including their results, shortcomings, our solutions, and subsequent steps. Finally, we provide some advice for researchers aiming at developing novel paradigms.
疼痛可以被认为是“身体错误”的信号:错误-实际和最佳/目标状态之间的差异-可以使生物体处于危险之中并激活行为防御系统。如果错误与身体有关,疼痛是一种警告信号,它会激发保护行为,如回避行为,以保护我们身体的完整性。因此,痛苦分享了错误的功能。在神经层面上,错误处理的一个重要组成部分是错误相关的负性(error-related negative, ERN),即错误发生后100 ms内主要在前扣带皮层产生的脑电图(EEG)信号的负偏转。尽管有令人信服的证据表明,ERN在各种精神病理学的发展中起着重要作用,并与学习和行为调整有关,但其与疼痛相关回避的关系尚未得到研究。根据焦虑研究的结果,似乎可以想象,ERN振幅升高的个体更容易从事与疼痛相关的回避行为,这在某些情况下可能是发展为慢性疼痛的危险因素。因此,这一新的研究路线有望有助于我们对人类疼痛的理解。与大多数新研究领域一样,整合神经神经网络和疼痛科学领域的第一个关键步骤是开发一种适合这两个领域需求的范式。目前的手稿提出了一个实验任务的发展和试点测量ERN和回避行为,以应对痛苦的错误,以及在这里遇到的挑战。共有12名参与者接受了6个不同版本的任务。我们将详细描述这些版本,包括它们的结果、缺点、我们的解决方案和后续步骤。最后,我们对旨在开发新范式的研究人员提出了一些建议。
{"title":"Peer Review of \"Trial and Error (-Related Negativity): An Odyssey of Integrating Different Experimental Paradigms","authors":"Ilona Domen, K. Groot","doi":"10.36850/e2.pr1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.36850/e2.pr1","url":null,"abstract":"Pain can be considered as a signal of “bodily error”: Errors – discrepancies between actual and optimal/targeted state – can put organisms at danger and activate behavioral defensive systems. If the error relates to the body, pain is the warning signal that motivates protective action such as avoidance behavior to safeguard our body’s integrity. Hence, pain shares the functionality of errors. On the neural level, an important error processing component is the error-related negativity (ERN), a negative deflection in the electroencephalographic (EEG) signal generated primarily in the anterior cingulate cortex within 100 ms after error commission. Despite compelling evidence that the ERN plays an important role in the development of various psychopathologies and is implicated in learning and adjustment of behavior, its relation to pain-related avoidance has not yet been examined. Based on findings from anxiety research, it seems conceivable that individuals with elevated ERN amplitudes are more prone to engage in pain-related avoidance behavior, which may, under certain conditions, be a risk factor for developing chronic pain. Consequently, this new line of research promises to contribute to our understanding of human pain. As in most novel research areas, a first crucial step for integrating the scientific fields of ERN and pain is developing a paradigm suited to address the needs from both fields. The present manuscript presents the development and piloting of an experimental task measuring both ERN and avoidance behavior in response to painful mistakes, as well as the challenges encountered herein. A total of 12 participants underwent one of six different task versions. We describe in detail each of these versions, including their results, shortcomings, our solutions, and subsequent steps. Finally, we provide some advice for researchers aiming at developing novel paradigms.","PeriodicalId":275817,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Trial and Error","volume":"36 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-12-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"128366882","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Peer Review of "Burst Beliefs - Methodological Problems in the Balloon Analogue Risk Task and Implications for its Use" “突发信念-气球模拟风险任务的方法问题及其使用意义”的同行评议
Pub Date : 2020-12-16 DOI: 10.36850/mr1.pr1
M. Young, Sihua Xu
Studies in the field of psychology often employ (computerised) behavioural tasks, aimed at mimicking real-world situations that elicit certain actions in participants. Such tasks are for example used to study risk propensity, a trait-like tendency towards taking or avoiding risk. One of the most popular tasks for gauging risk propensity is the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART; Lejuez et al., 2002), which has been shown to relate well to self-reported risk-taking and to real-world risk behaviours. However, despite its popularity and qualities, the BART has several methodological shortcomings, most of which have been reported before, but none of which are widely known. In the present paper, four such problems are explained and elaborated on: a lack of clarity as to whether decisions are characterised by uncertainty or risk; censoring of observations; confounding of risk and expected value; and poor decomposability into adaptive and maladaptive risk behaviour. Furthermore, for every problem, a range of possible solutions is discussed, which overall can be divided into three categories: using a different, more informative outcome index than the standard average pump score; modifying one or more task elements; or using a different task, either an alternative risk-taking task (sequential or otherwise), or a custom-made instrument. It is important to make use of these solutions, as applying the BART without accounting for its shortcomings may lead to interpretational problems, including false positive and false negative results. Depending on the research aims of a given study, certain shortcomings are more pressing than others, indicating the (type of) solutions most needed. By combining solutions and openly discussing shortcomings, researchers may be able to modify the BART in such a way that it can operationalise risk propensity without substantial methodological problems.
心理学领域的研究经常采用(计算机化的)行为任务,旨在模仿现实世界的情况,从而引发参与者的某些行为。例如,这些任务用于研究风险倾向,即承担或避免风险的倾向。衡量风险倾向的最流行的任务之一是气球模拟风险任务(BART;Lejuez et al., 2002),这已被证明与自我报告的冒险行为和现实世界的风险行为有很好的关系。然而,尽管BART广受欢迎,质量也很好,但它在方法上有几个缺点,其中大部分都是以前报道过的,但没有一个是广为人知的。在本文中,解释和阐述了四个这样的问题:决策是否具有不确定性或风险的特征缺乏明确性;对观察结果的审查;风险与期望值的混淆;难以分解为适应和不适应的风险行为。此外,对于每个问题,讨论了一系列可能的解决方案,总体上可分为三类:使用不同的、比标准平均泵评分更有信息量的结果指标;修改一个或多个任务元素;或者使用不同的任务,要么是另一项冒险任务(顺序或其他),要么是定制的工具。重要的是要利用这些解决方案,因为应用BART而不考虑其缺点可能导致解释问题,包括假阳性和假阴性结果。根据特定研究的研究目的,某些缺点比其他缺点更紧迫,这表明最需要的解决方案(类型)。通过结合解决方案和公开讨论缺点,研究人员可能能够以这样一种方式修改BART,使其能够在不存在实质性方法问题的情况下操作风险倾向。
{"title":"Peer Review of \"Burst Beliefs - Methodological Problems in the Balloon Analogue Risk Task and Implications for its Use\"","authors":"M. Young, Sihua Xu","doi":"10.36850/mr1.pr1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.36850/mr1.pr1","url":null,"abstract":"Studies in the field of psychology often employ (computerised) behavioural tasks, aimed at mimicking real-world situations that elicit certain actions in participants. Such tasks are for example used to study risk propensity, a trait-like tendency towards taking or avoiding risk. One of the most popular tasks for gauging risk propensity is the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART; Lejuez et al., 2002), which has been shown to relate well to self-reported risk-taking and to real-world risk behaviours. However, despite its popularity and qualities, the BART has several methodological shortcomings, most of which have been reported before, but none of which are widely known. In the present paper, four such problems are explained and elaborated on: a lack of clarity as to whether decisions are characterised by uncertainty or risk; censoring of observations; confounding of risk and expected value; and poor decomposability into adaptive and maladaptive risk behaviour. Furthermore, for every problem, a range of possible solutions is discussed, which overall can be divided into three categories: using a different, more informative outcome index than the standard average pump score; modifying one or more task elements; or using a different task, either an alternative risk-taking task (sequential or otherwise), or a custom-made instrument. It is important to make use of these solutions, as applying the BART without accounting for its shortcomings may lead to interpretational problems, including false positive and false negative results. Depending on the research aims of a given study, certain shortcomings are more pressing than others, indicating the (type of) solutions most needed. By combining solutions and openly discussing shortcomings, researchers may be able to modify the BART in such a way that it can operationalise risk propensity without substantial methodological problems.","PeriodicalId":275817,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Trial and Error","volume":"328 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-12-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"133940546","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Peer Review of "Reflection on 'Trial and Error (-Related Negativity)' 同行评议“对‘试错(相关消极)’的反思”
Pub Date : 2020-12-16 DOI: 10.36850/r2.pr1
Stefan Gaillard, S. Devine
This paper is about error on three levels. First of all, it deals with research into the ERN, error-related negativity. The ERN is a negative deflection in the EEG signal, which tends to occur within 100 milliseconds of making an error. The authors hypothesize that physical pain can be considered as a bodily signal that a type of error has been committed: there is a "discrepancy between the actual and optimal/targeted state", as the authors put it (p.1). This raises the question whether the ERN is also associated with pain and the avoidance of pain, and if so how. More specifically the authors want to know whether people with an elevated ERN are more prone to avoidance behaviour, which in turn can lead to chronic pain. I am an historian of psychology with philosophical interests and have no expertise in clinical neuropsychology, so I will not comment on this hypothesis. But the paper also deals with error in two other ways, which I do feel able to reflect on. The authors describe their attempts to develop an experimental paradigm for the study of the role of the ERN in pain avoidance. In these attempts they make errors which they then try to correct in a further attempt, six task versions in total. This is the second way this paper deals with errors -those of the experimenters themselves. But there is a third level too: it is crucial for the experimental task to induce the participant to make the right number of errors -not too many, not too few. The second and third aspects are obviously related: the errors of the experimenters concern, among others, the number of errors the participants make. The authors describe their challenge as an interdisciplinary one: they had to combine elements of neurophysiology (ERN) with clinical psychology (pain avoidance). Specifically: they had to somehow induce an ERN in the participants, and elicit and measure some type of avoidance behaviour at the same time. Moreover, to determine what each participant’s average ERN is, they needed at least six ERN measures per participant, and thus a minimum of six errors. The errors, finally, had to be "inhibition errors", not errors due to lack of knowledge or skill. It wasn’t clear to me why this was important, or what an inhibition error is in the first place, but this is no doubt due to my own lack of knowledge in this field. All in all, the specifications of the task were narrow and demanding: not any type of error would do (only inhibition errors); the errors had to produce a proper ERN; a minimum of six was needed; the participants had to be aware of their error (otherwise they would not show avoidance behaviour); and of course there had to be pain associated with the errors, but not so much pain that the ethics committee would reject the pilot study, or the subjects would refuse to participate. What followed was a kind of dance, or rather a series of dances, with the experimenters leading the participants, successively trying different choreographies in an attempt to
本文从三个层面探讨误差。首先,它涉及到对ERN的研究,错误相关的消极性。ERN是脑电图信号中的负偏转,往往发生在犯错后100毫秒内。作者假设,身体上的疼痛可以被认为是一种身体信号,表明犯了某种错误:正如作者所说,“实际状态与最佳/目标状态之间存在差异”(第1页)。这就提出了一个问题,ERN是否也与疼痛和回避疼痛有关,如果是的话,又是如何关联的。更具体地说,作者想知道,ERN水平升高的人是否更容易出现回避行为,从而导致慢性疼痛。我是一个有哲学兴趣的心理学历史学家,对临床神经心理学没有专业知识,所以我不会对这个假设发表评论。但这篇论文也从另外两个方面处理了错误,我确实觉得这是可以反思的。作者描述了他们试图开发一个实验范式来研究ERN在疼痛回避中的作用。在这些尝试中,他们犯了错误,然后他们试图在进一步的尝试中纠正,总共六个任务版本。这是本文处理错误的第二种方式——实验者自己的错误。但还有第三个层面:对于实验任务来说,诱导参与者犯正确数量的错误——不要太多,也不要太少——是至关重要的。第二个和第三个方面明显相关:实验人员的错误与参与者犯错误的次数有关。作者将他们的挑战描述为跨学科的挑战:他们必须将神经生理学(ERN)与临床心理学(疼痛回避)相结合。具体来说:他们必须以某种方式在参与者中诱导ERN,同时引发并测量某种类型的回避行为。此外,为了确定每个参与者的平均ERN是多少,他们至少需要每个参与者测量6个ERN,因此至少需要6个错误。最后,这些错误必须是“抑制错误”,而不是由于缺乏知识或技能而导致的错误。我不清楚为什么这很重要,或者什么是抑制错误,但毫无疑问,这是由于我自己在这个领域缺乏知识。总而言之,任务的规格是狭窄而苛刻的:任何类型的错误都不行(只有抑制错误);误差必须产生合适的ERN;至少需要六个;参与者必须意识到他们的错误(否则他们不会表现出回避行为);当然,这些错误会带来痛苦,但不会痛苦到伦理委员会会拒绝试点研究,或者受试者会拒绝参与。接下来是一种舞蹈,或者更确切地说是一系列舞蹈,由实验者带领参与者,依次尝试不同的舞蹈编排,试图让他们的搭档做出正确的动作,而不是踩到他们的脚趾。在最近的一篇文章中,Brenninkmeijer、Rietzschel和我报道了我们对心理学研究人员进行的一系列采访(Brenninkmeijer et al., 2019)。我们询问了他们的非正式研究实践,也就是说,那些在论文的方法部分没有明确说明但仍然被认为是重要的实践。我们在他们的回答中看到了两个主题。首先是对专业精神的强烈关注,在有序的实验室和顺利进行的实验中表现出自己的优秀,并尊重参与者的行为。第二个主题是关注通过管理参与者的表现来产生良好的数据。第二个主题与这篇关于“试错”的论文有关。在实验室里完成的大部分工作,包括我们在采访中探索的那种非正式的、不成文的工作,都是为了从参与者那里引出正确的行为。在实验室里发生的事情有一种戏剧的性质:一种特定的表演是被期望从参与者那里得到的,他们被实验的舞台和脚本(或编排)以及实验者的行为所引导。我们的受访者提到
{"title":"Peer Review of \"Reflection on 'Trial and Error (-Related Negativity)'","authors":"Stefan Gaillard, S. Devine","doi":"10.36850/r2.pr1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.36850/r2.pr1","url":null,"abstract":"This paper is about error on three levels. First of all, it deals with research into the ERN, error-related negativity. The ERN is a negative deflection in the EEG signal, which tends to occur within 100 milliseconds of making an error. The authors hypothesize that physical pain can be considered as a bodily signal that a type of error has been committed: there is a \"discrepancy between the actual and optimal/targeted state\", as the authors put it (p.1). This raises the question whether the ERN is also associated with pain and the avoidance of pain, and if so how. More specifically the authors want to know whether people with an elevated ERN are more prone to avoidance behaviour, which in turn can lead to chronic pain. I am an historian of psychology with philosophical interests and have no expertise in clinical neuropsychology, so I will not comment on this hypothesis. But the paper also deals with error in two other ways, which I do feel able to reflect on. The authors describe their attempts to develop an experimental paradigm for the study of the role of the ERN in pain avoidance. In these attempts they make errors which they then try to correct in a further attempt, six task versions in total. This is the second way this paper deals with errors -those of the experimenters themselves. But there is a third level too: it is crucial for the experimental task to induce the participant to make the right number of errors -not too many, not too few. The second and third aspects are obviously related: the errors of the experimenters concern, among others, the number of errors the participants make. The authors describe their challenge as an interdisciplinary one: they had to combine elements of neurophysiology (ERN) with clinical psychology (pain avoidance). Specifically: they had to somehow induce an ERN in the participants, and elicit and measure some type of avoidance behaviour at the same time. Moreover, to determine what each participant’s average ERN is, they needed at least six ERN measures per participant, and thus a minimum of six errors. The errors, finally, had to be \"inhibition errors\", not errors due to lack of knowledge or skill. It wasn’t clear to me why this was important, or what an inhibition error is in the first place, but this is no doubt due to my own lack of knowledge in this field. All in all, the specifications of the task were narrow and demanding: not any type of error would do (only inhibition errors); the errors had to produce a proper ERN; a minimum of six was needed; the participants had to be aware of their error (otherwise they would not show avoidance behaviour); and of course there had to be pain associated with the errors, but not so much pain that the ethics committee would reject the pilot study, or the subjects would refuse to participate. What followed was a kind of dance, or rather a series of dances, with the experimenters leading the participants, successively trying different choreographies in an attempt to","PeriodicalId":275817,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Trial and Error","volume":"36 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-12-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"125097582","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Global Giving 全球捐赠
Pub Date : 2020-12-02 DOI: 10.36850/rga2
R. Bekkers
Why do citizens in some countries take more responsibility for the well-being of others than in other countries? This project seeks to understand the genesis of prosociality, investigating its biological foundations, the influence of cultural traditions, and effects of political, economic and legal structure. The dominant theory in economics views philanthropy as a solution to social illnesses that the market and the state are not solving, a view complementary to political science theory on preferences for government provision. Sociologists focus on social norms emerging from religious traditions. Cultural evolutionary theory highlights the instrumental value of trust. Still other theories have suggested a role for natural selection of genes. However, these theories have not been tested stringently nor simultaneously. Also the project includes a very important factor largely ignored thus far: political, legal and economic institutions also affect the level of giving as well as who gives to which causes. Therefore, the objectives of Global Giving are (1) to map country differences in the size and nature of philanthropy across the world; (2) to develop and test multidisciplinary theories explaining these differences; (3) to facilitate international collaboration across disciplinary boundaries in research on philanthropy. The research draws upon 200 surveys recently harmonized by the PI and on new data on philanthropy to be collected among large samples in 145 countries across all continents. Collaboration with international networks of academics safeguards the validity of the questionnaires and experiments. Appropriate multilevel regression models will be used, the lack of which caused biases in previous research. An integrated understanding of philanthropy is useful not only for theory development, but also for government policy makers and practitioners in nonprofit organizations seeking to mobilize philanthropic contributions and make them more effective. The application in practice is ensured through collaboration with a large network of practitioners.
为什么有些国家的公民比其他国家的公民对他人的幸福承担更多的责任?该项目旨在了解亲社会的起源,调查其生物学基础、文化传统的影响以及政治、经济和法律结构的影响。经济学中的主流理论认为,慈善是解决市场和国家无法解决的社会疾病的一种方法,这一观点与关于政府提供偏好的政治学理论相辅相成。社会学家关注的是源自宗教传统的社会规范。文化进化理论强调了信任的工具价值。还有其他理论提出了基因自然选择的作用。然而,这些理论并没有得到严格的检验,也没有同时得到检验。此外,该项目还包括一个迄今在很大程度上被忽视的非常重要的因素:政治、法律和经济制度也会影响捐赠的水平以及谁向哪些事业捐赠。因此,全球捐赠的目标是:(1)绘制世界各地慈善事业规模和性质的国家差异;(2)发展和检验解释这些差异的多学科理论;(3)促进慈善研究的国际跨学科合作。这项研究借鉴了PI最近协调的200项调查,以及在各大洲145个国家的大样本中收集的有关慈善事业的新数据。与国际学术网络的合作保障了问卷和实验的有效性。将使用适当的多水平回归模型,这在以往的研究中缺乏导致偏差。对慈善事业的综合理解不仅有助于理论发展,也有助于政府政策制定者和非营利组织从业者寻求动员慈善捐款并使其更有效。通过与大量从业人员的合作,确保在实践中的应用。
{"title":"Global Giving","authors":"R. Bekkers","doi":"10.36850/rga2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.36850/rga2","url":null,"abstract":"Why do citizens in some countries take more responsibility for the well-being of others than in other countries? This project seeks to understand the genesis of prosociality, investigating its biological foundations, the influence of cultural traditions, and effects of political, economic and legal structure. The dominant theory in economics views philanthropy as a solution to social illnesses that the market and the state are not solving, a view complementary to political science theory on preferences for government provision. Sociologists focus on social norms emerging from religious traditions. Cultural evolutionary theory highlights the instrumental value of trust. Still other theories have suggested a role for natural selection of genes. However, these theories have not been tested stringently nor simultaneously. Also the project includes a very important factor largely ignored thus far: political, legal and economic institutions also affect the level of giving as well as who gives to which causes. Therefore, the objectives of Global Giving are (1) to map country differences in the size and nature of philanthropy across the world; (2) to develop and test multidisciplinary theories explaining these differences; (3) to facilitate international collaboration across disciplinary boundaries in research on philanthropy. The research draws upon 200 surveys recently harmonized by the PI and on new data on philanthropy to be collected among large samples in 145 countries across all continents. Collaboration with international networks of academics safeguards the validity of the questionnaires and experiments. Appropriate multilevel regression models will be used, the lack of which caused biases in previous research. An integrated understanding of philanthropy is useful not only for theory development, but also for government policy makers and practitioners in nonprofit organizations seeking to mobilize philanthropic contributions and make them more effective. The application in practice is ensured through collaboration with a large network of practitioners.","PeriodicalId":275817,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Trial and Error","volume":"93 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"128836168","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Peer Review of "Experiment and Fail" “实验与失败”的同行评议
Pub Date : 2020-12-02 DOI: 10.36850/r1.pr1
S. Devine, Stefan Gaillard
Introduction The paper ‘Alcohol cues and aggressive thoughts’ reports a failed attempt at reproducing two experiments. The massive shortcomings of the reported reproduction are obvious. For a moment I was tempted to think that the authors, in the form of a standard psychological paper, were presenting a philosophical critique of this type of experiments. In my comment I will try to formulate such a critique in a more straightforward manner. I will first give a brief and plain description of what happened in the experiment and why it was done, according to the authors. Then I will say a few things about the complexity of producing and reproducing experiments in general, followed by a section on the problems of the specific type of experiments of which this one is a specimen: priming studies, mostly found in the subdiscipline of social psychology and since almost a decade the subject of a vigorous debate among methodologists, philosophers of science, priming researchers, in scientific journals, but also in newspapers, magazines, blogs and on Twitter. I will end with assessing the possibilities and the limits of doing experiments in the human sciences: what can we learn from experiments on alcohol cues if we want to tackle physical, mental and social harm, attributed to the consumption of alcohol?
“酒精暗示和攻击性思想”这篇论文报告了重复两个实验的失败尝试。报道中再现的大量缺陷是显而易见的。有那么一刻,我忍不住想,作者是在以一篇标准的心理学论文的形式,对这类实验进行哲学批判。在我的评论中,我将尝试以一种更直接的方式提出这样的批评。我将首先给出一个简单明了的描述,在实验中发生了什么,以及为什么要这样做,根据作者。然后我会说一些关于制作和复制实验的复杂性的事情,然后是关于特定类型实验的问题的部分,这个实验是一个样本:启动研究,主要是在社会心理学的分支学科中发现的,近十年来,在科学期刊上,在报纸,杂志,博客和推特上,方法学家,科学哲学家,启动研究人员之间激烈辩论的主题。最后,我将评估在人文科学中进行实验的可能性和局限性:如果我们想要解决由饮酒引起的身体、精神和社会伤害,我们可以从酒精暗示的实验中学到什么?
{"title":"Peer Review of \"Experiment and Fail\"","authors":"S. Devine, Stefan Gaillard","doi":"10.36850/r1.pr1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.36850/r1.pr1","url":null,"abstract":"Introduction The paper ‘Alcohol cues and aggressive thoughts’ reports a failed attempt at reproducing two experiments. The massive shortcomings of the reported reproduction are obvious. For a moment I was tempted to think that the authors, in the form of a standard psychological paper, were presenting a philosophical critique of this type of experiments. In my comment I will try to formulate such a critique in a more straightforward manner. I will first give a brief and plain description of what happened in the experiment and why it was done, according to the authors. Then I will say a few things about the complexity of producing and reproducing experiments in general, followed by a section on the problems of the specific type of experiments of which this one is a specimen: priming studies, mostly found in the subdiscipline of social psychology and since almost a decade the subject of a vigorous debate among methodologists, philosophers of science, priming researchers, in scientific journals, but also in newspapers, magazines, blogs and on Twitter. I will end with assessing the possibilities and the limits of doing experiments in the human sciences: what can we learn from experiments on alcohol cues if we want to tackle physical, mental and social harm, attributed to the consumption of alcohol?","PeriodicalId":275817,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Trial and Error","volume":"33 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"124459370","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Trial and Error
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1