首页 > 最新文献

2019 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience最新文献

英文 中文
Functional connectivity of fractal and oscillatory cortical activity is distinct 分形和振荡皮层活动的功能连通性是明显的
Pub Date : 1900-01-01 DOI: 10.32470/ccn.2019.1341-0
Andrea Ibarra Chaoul, M. Siegel
Electrophysiological signals of cortical population activity contain oscillatory and fractal (1/frequency) components. However, the relationship between these components is unclear. To address this, we investigated human resting-state MEG recordings. We applied combined source-analysis, signal orthogonalization and irregular-resampling autospectral analysis (IRASA) to separate oscillatory and fractal components of the MEG signals at the cortical source-level. We then compared the spatial correlation structure of fractal and oscillatory components across the human cortex. We found that these correlation structures differed, which suggests different mechanisms underlying fractal and oscillatory population signal components.
皮层群体活动的电生理信号包含振荡和分形(1/频率)成分。然而,这些成分之间的关系尚不清楚。为了解决这个问题,我们研究了人类静息状态的MEG记录。采用源分析、信号正交化和不规则重采样自谱分析(IRASA)相结合的方法,在皮层源水平上分离MEG信号的振荡成分和分形成分。然后,我们比较了人类皮层分形和振荡成分的空间相关结构。我们发现这些相关结构不同,表明分形和振荡种群信号成分的机制不同。
{"title":"Functional connectivity of fractal and oscillatory cortical activity is distinct","authors":"Andrea Ibarra Chaoul, M. Siegel","doi":"10.32470/ccn.2019.1341-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32470/ccn.2019.1341-0","url":null,"abstract":"Electrophysiological signals of cortical population activity contain oscillatory and fractal (1/frequency) components. However, the relationship between these components is unclear. To address this, we investigated human resting-state MEG recordings. We applied combined source-analysis, signal orthogonalization and irregular-resampling autospectral analysis (IRASA) to separate oscillatory and fractal components of the MEG signals at the cortical source-level. We then compared the spatial correlation structure of fractal and oscillatory components across the human cortex. We found that these correlation structures differed, which suggests different mechanisms underlying fractal and oscillatory population signal components.","PeriodicalId":281121,"journal":{"name":"2019 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience","volume":"3 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"130147781","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stopping actions by suppressing striatal plateau potentials 通过抑制纹状体平台电位来停止动作
Pub Date : 1900-01-01 DOI: 10.32470/ccn.2019.1205-0
M. M. Nejad, Daniel Trpevski, J. Kotaleski, R. Schmidt
Striatal projection neurons (SPNs) in the basal ganglia gradually increase their firing rate during movement initiation. Arkypallidal neurons in globus pallidus briefly increase their firing rate upon a Stop signal, which cues movement cancellation. This increase potentially leads to the suppression of movement-related activity in striatum by inhibiting SPNs. However, this brief inhibition from arkypallidal neurons may be too short to completely prevent the gradual firing rate increase in SPNs. Here, we investigated the impact of the brief inhibition on the gradual increase in a multi-compartmental model of a SPN. We reproduced the movement-related firing pattern in the SPN model neuron by brief clustered excitation added to a baseline, subthreshold excitation. This brief clustered excitation evoked a dendritic plateau potential leading to a long-lasting depolarization at the soma, which enhanced the somatic excitability and evoked spikes upon the baseline excitation that was formerly subthreshold. A brief inhibition, representing arkypallidal stop responses, applied on the dendritic site where the clustered excitation was present, suppressed the somatic depolarization and attenuated the movement-related activity similar to the firing pattern observed in rats for successful action suppression. We conclude that arkypallidal Stop responses can suppress movement-related activity in the striatum by suppressing the dendritic plateau potentials.
基底神经节纹状体投射神经元(SPNs)在运动启动过程中逐渐增加放电频率。白球中的树叶神经元在收到停止信号后会短暂地增加其放电速率,这提示运动取消。这种增加可能通过抑制spn导致纹状体中运动相关活动的抑制。然而,这种来自木葱神经元的短暂抑制可能太短,无法完全阻止spn的逐渐放电速率增加。在这里,我们研究了短暂抑制对SPN多室模型逐渐增加的影响。我们在SPN模型神经元中复制了运动相关的放电模式,通过在基线、阈下兴奋中添加短暂的集群兴奋。这种短暂的聚集性兴奋诱发了树突平台电位,导致了长时间的体细胞去极化,这增强了体细胞的兴奋性,并在基线兴奋上诱发了峰值,而基线兴奋以前是阈下的。一个短暂的抑制,代表木香蒜素停止反应,应用于存在聚集性兴奋的树突部位,抑制了体细胞去极化,减弱了与运动相关的活动,类似于在大鼠成功的动作抑制中观察到的放电模式。我们得出结论,木蒜醛停止反应可以通过抑制树突平台电位来抑制纹状体的运动相关活动。
{"title":"Stopping actions by suppressing striatal plateau potentials","authors":"M. M. Nejad, Daniel Trpevski, J. Kotaleski, R. Schmidt","doi":"10.32470/ccn.2019.1205-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32470/ccn.2019.1205-0","url":null,"abstract":"Striatal projection neurons (SPNs) in the basal ganglia gradually increase their firing rate during movement initiation. Arkypallidal neurons in globus pallidus briefly increase their firing rate upon a Stop signal, which cues movement cancellation. This increase potentially leads to the suppression of movement-related activity in striatum by inhibiting SPNs. However, this brief inhibition from arkypallidal neurons may be too short to completely prevent the gradual firing rate increase in SPNs. Here, we investigated the impact of the brief inhibition on the gradual increase in a multi-compartmental model of a SPN. We reproduced the movement-related firing pattern in the SPN model neuron by brief clustered excitation added to a baseline, subthreshold excitation. This brief clustered excitation evoked a dendritic plateau potential leading to a long-lasting depolarization at the soma, which enhanced the somatic excitability and evoked spikes upon the baseline excitation that was formerly subthreshold. A brief inhibition, representing arkypallidal stop responses, applied on the dendritic site where the clustered excitation was present, suppressed the somatic depolarization and attenuated the movement-related activity similar to the firing pattern observed in rats for successful action suppression. We conclude that arkypallidal Stop responses can suppress movement-related activity in the striatum by suppressing the dendritic plateau potentials.","PeriodicalId":281121,"journal":{"name":"2019 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"125698441","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual representations supporting category-specific information about visual objects in the brain 视觉表征支持大脑中关于视觉对象的特定类别信息
Pub Date : 1900-01-01 DOI: 10.32470/ccn.2019.1404-0
Simon Faghel-Soubeyrand, Arjen Alink, E. Bamps, F. Gosselin, I. Charest
Over recent years, multivariate pattern analysis (“decoding”) approaches have become increasingly used to investigate “when” and “where” our brains conduct meaningful processes about their visual environments. Studies using time-resolved decoding of M/EEG patterns have described numerous processes such as object/face familiarity and the emergence of basic-to-abstract category information. Surprisingly, no study has, to our knowledge, revealed “what” (i.e. the actual visual information that) our brain uses while these computations are examined by decoding algorithms. Here, we revealed the time course at which our brain extracts realistic category-specific information about visual objects (i.e. emotion-type & gender information from faces) with time-resolved decoding of high-density EEG patterns, as well as carefully controlled tasks and visual stimulation. Then, we derived temporal generalization matrices and showed that category-specific information is 1) first diffused across brain areas (250 to 350 ms) and 2) encoded under a stable neural pattern that suggests evidence accumulation (350 to 650 ms after face onset). Finally, we bridged time-resolved decoding with psychophysics and revealed the specific visual information (spatial frequency, feature position & orientation information) that support these brain computations. Doing so, we uncovered interconnected dynamics between visual features, and the accumulation and diffusion of category-specific information in the brain.
近年来,多元模式分析(“解码”)方法越来越多地用于研究我们的大脑在“何时”和“何地”对视觉环境进行有意义的处理。使用时间分辨解码M/EEG模式的研究描述了许多过程,如物体/面孔熟悉和基本到抽象类别信息的出现。令人惊讶的是,据我们所知,没有研究揭示了当解码算法检查这些计算时,我们的大脑使用了“什么”(即实际的视觉信息)。在这里,我们揭示了我们的大脑通过对高密度脑电图模式进行时间分辨解码,以及精心控制的任务和视觉刺激,提取视觉对象的真实类别特定信息(即面部的情绪类型和性别信息)的时间过程。然后,我们推导了时间泛化矩阵,并表明类别特异性信息首先在250至350毫秒的大脑区域扩散,2)在一个稳定的神经模式下编码,表明证据积累(面部出现后350至650毫秒)。最后,我们将时间分辨解码与心理物理学结合起来,揭示了支持这些大脑计算的特定视觉信息(空间频率、特征位置和方向信息)。通过这样做,我们发现了视觉特征与大脑中特定类别信息的积累和扩散之间的相互联系的动态。
{"title":"Visual representations supporting category-specific information about visual objects in the brain","authors":"Simon Faghel-Soubeyrand, Arjen Alink, E. Bamps, F. Gosselin, I. Charest","doi":"10.32470/ccn.2019.1404-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32470/ccn.2019.1404-0","url":null,"abstract":"Over recent years, multivariate pattern analysis (“decoding”) approaches have become increasingly used to investigate “when” and “where” our brains conduct meaningful processes about their visual environments. Studies using time-resolved decoding of M/EEG patterns have described numerous processes such as object/face familiarity and the emergence of basic-to-abstract category information. Surprisingly, no study has, to our knowledge, revealed “what” (i.e. the actual visual information that) our brain uses while these computations are examined by decoding algorithms. Here, we revealed the time course at which our brain extracts realistic category-specific information about visual objects (i.e. emotion-type & gender information from faces) with time-resolved decoding of high-density EEG patterns, as well as carefully controlled tasks and visual stimulation. Then, we derived temporal generalization matrices and showed that category-specific information is 1) first diffused across brain areas (250 to 350 ms) and 2) encoded under a stable neural pattern that suggests evidence accumulation (350 to 650 ms after face onset). Finally, we bridged time-resolved decoding with psychophysics and revealed the specific visual information (spatial frequency, feature position & orientation information) that support these brain computations. Doing so, we uncovered interconnected dynamics between visual features, and the accumulation and diffusion of category-specific information in the brain.","PeriodicalId":281121,"journal":{"name":"2019 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience","volume":"44 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"125924891","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The orbitofrontal cortex as a negative feedback control system: computational modeling and fMRI 眼窝额叶皮层作为负反馈控制系统:计算模型和功能磁共振成像
Pub Date : 1900-01-01 DOI: 10.32470/ccn.2019.1070-0
N. Zarr, Joshua W. Brown
In this work we address two inter-related issues. First, the computational roles of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and hippocampus in value-based decision-making have been unclear, with various proposed roles in value representation, cognitive maps, and prospection. Second, reinforcement learning models have been slow to adapt to more general problems in which the reward values of states may change over time, thus requiring different Q values for a given state at different times. We have developed a model of artificial general intelligence that treats much of the brain as a high dimensional control system in the framework of control theory. We show with computational modeling and combined fMRI and representational similarity analysis (RSA) that the model can autonomously learn to solve problems and provides a clear computational account of how a number of brain regions, particularly the OFC, interact to guide behavior to achieve arbitrary goals.
在这项工作中,我们解决了两个相互关联的问题。首先,眼窝前额皮质(OFC)和海马体在基于价值的决策中的计算作用尚不清楚,在价值表征、认知地图和前景方面有各种各样的作用。其次,强化学习模型在适应更普遍的问题时速度很慢,在这些问题中,状态的奖励值可能会随着时间的推移而变化,因此在不同的时间,给定的状态需要不同的Q值。我们已经开发了一个人工通用智能模型,在控制理论的框架下,将大脑的大部分视为一个高维控制系统。我们通过计算建模,结合fMRI和表征相似性分析(RSA)表明,该模型可以自主学习解决问题,并提供了一个清晰的计算说明,说明一些大脑区域,特别是OFC,如何相互作用,指导行为以实现任意目标。
{"title":"The orbitofrontal cortex as a negative feedback control system: computational modeling and fMRI","authors":"N. Zarr, Joshua W. Brown","doi":"10.32470/ccn.2019.1070-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32470/ccn.2019.1070-0","url":null,"abstract":"In this work we address two inter-related issues. First, the computational roles of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and hippocampus in value-based decision-making have been unclear, with various proposed roles in value representation, cognitive maps, and prospection. Second, reinforcement learning models have been slow to adapt to more general problems in which the reward values of states may change over time, thus requiring different Q values for a given state at different times. We have developed a model of artificial general intelligence that treats much of the brain as a high dimensional control system in the framework of control theory. We show with computational modeling and combined fMRI and representational similarity analysis (RSA) that the model can autonomously learn to solve problems and provides a clear computational account of how a number of brain regions, particularly the OFC, interact to guide behavior to achieve arbitrary goals.","PeriodicalId":281121,"journal":{"name":"2019 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"125496322","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
NMDA-Receptor Dysfunction Disrupts Serial Biases in Spatial Working Memory nmda受体功能障碍破坏空间工作记忆的序列偏差
Pub Date : 1900-01-01 DOI: 10.32470/ccn.2019.1304-0
H. Stein, Joao Barbosa, J. Dalmau, A. Compte
In working memory (WM) tasks, attractive biases to previous items are evidence for continuous temporal integration of memories. These serial biases have been modeled as a product of synaptic short-term plasticity, allowing WM representations to endure in a synaptic trace and interfere with the next trial even when neural activity returns to baseline values. We hypothesized that the NMDAR, a key component of both short-term potentiation (STP) and stable WM delay activity, would be of central importance to serial biases in a visuospatial WM task. Confirming this hypothesis, we found drastically reduced biases in patients with anti-NMDAR encephalitis and schizophrenia, both diseases that have been related to NMDAR hypofunction. We simulated serial biases in a spiking neural network supported by a Hebbian STP mechanism that builds up during persistent delay-activity. We found a close correspondence between patient and model behavior when gradually lowering levels of STP, suggesting a disruption of short-term plasticity in associative cortices of schizophrenic and anti-NMDAR encephalitis patients. Further, we explored the capability of the model to explain reduced biases in light of the disinhibition theory of schizophrenia.
在工作记忆(WM)任务中,对先前项目的吸引力偏差是记忆持续时间整合的证据。这些连续偏差被建模为突触短期可塑性的产物,允许WM表征在突触痕迹中持续存在,即使神经活动恢复到基线值也会干扰下一次试验。我们假设NMDAR是短时增强(STP)和稳定WM延迟活动的关键组成部分,对视觉空间WM任务中的序列偏差至关重要。证实了这一假设,我们发现抗NMDAR脑炎和精神分裂症患者的偏倚大大减少,这两种疾病都与NMDAR功能减退有关。我们模拟了在持续延迟活动期间建立的Hebbian STP机制支持的尖峰神经网络中的序列偏差。我们发现,当STP水平逐渐降低时,患者和模型行为之间存在密切的对应关系,这表明精神分裂症和抗nmdar脑炎患者联合皮层的短期可塑性受到破坏。此外,我们还探讨了该模型在精神分裂症去抑制理论的基础上解释减少偏差的能力。
{"title":"NMDA-Receptor Dysfunction Disrupts Serial Biases in Spatial Working Memory","authors":"H. Stein, Joao Barbosa, J. Dalmau, A. Compte","doi":"10.32470/ccn.2019.1304-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32470/ccn.2019.1304-0","url":null,"abstract":"In working memory (WM) tasks, attractive biases to previous items are evidence for continuous temporal integration of memories. These serial biases have been modeled as a product of synaptic short-term plasticity, allowing WM representations to endure in a synaptic trace and interfere with the next trial even when neural activity returns to baseline values. We hypothesized that the NMDAR, a key component of both short-term potentiation (STP) and stable WM delay activity, would be of central importance to serial biases in a visuospatial WM task. Confirming this hypothesis, we found drastically reduced biases in patients with anti-NMDAR encephalitis and schizophrenia, both diseases that have been related to NMDAR hypofunction. We simulated serial biases in a spiking neural network supported by a Hebbian STP mechanism that builds up during persistent delay-activity. We found a close correspondence between patient and model behavior when gradually lowering levels of STP, suggesting a disruption of short-term plasticity in associative cortices of schizophrenic and anti-NMDAR encephalitis patients. Further, we explored the capability of the model to explain reduced biases in light of the disinhibition theory of schizophrenia.","PeriodicalId":281121,"journal":{"name":"2019 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience","volume":"17 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"127833123","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How the Human Brain Solves the Symbol-Grounding Problem 人类大脑如何解决符号基础问题
Pub Date : 1900-01-01 DOI: 10.32470/ccn.2019.1145-0
Simone Viganò, V. Borghesani, M. Piazza
A fundamental issue in cognitive science is the so-called “symbol-grounding problem” (Harnad 1980), related to the question of how symbols acquire meaning. One simple view posits that, for concrete words, our brain solves the problem by creating associations between the neural representations of the surface forms of symbols (spoken or written words) to the one(s) evoked by the object, action, or event classes the symbols refer to (e.g., see Pulvermuller 2013; 2018). Evidence supporting this view comes from the observation that words related to well known concepts such as numerical quantities (Piazza et al. 2007; Eger et al. 2009), colors (e.g. Simmons et al. 2007), manipulable objects (Chao et al. 1999), places (Kumar et al. 2017), or actions (Hauk 2004; 2011), automatically re-activate the same brain regions that are active during the perception/execution of those specific object features/actions. These data, however, are informative on the neural bases of symbol grounded representations, but not on those underlying symbol grounding: i) they fall short in assessing the role of memory systems implicated in this kind of symbol-toconcept associative learning, and ii) they do not provide a full picture of the effects that symbol grounding has on the brain. Here, to investigate the neural changes generated by this process, we adopted an artificial learning paradigm where 21 adult subjects learned to categorize novel multisensory objects by giving them specific symbolic labels.
认知科学中的一个基本问题是所谓的“符号基础问题”(Harnad 1980),与符号如何获得意义的问题有关。一种简单的观点认为,对于具体的单词,我们的大脑通过在符号的表面形式(口语或书面文字)的神经表征与符号所指的对象、动作或事件类所唤起的表征之间建立联系来解决问题(例如,参见粉状穆勒2013;2018)。支持这一观点的证据来自于对与众所周知的概念相关的词汇的观察,如数值量(Piazza et al. 2007;Eger等人,2009),颜色(例如Simmons等人,2007),可操作对象(Chao等人,1999),地点(Kumar等人,2017)或动作(Hauk 2004;2011),自动重新激活在感知/执行这些特定对象特征/动作期间活跃的相同大脑区域。然而,这些数据在符号基础表征的神经基础上提供了信息,但在那些潜在的符号基础上却没有:1)它们在评估涉及这种从符号到概念的联想学习的记忆系统的作用方面存在不足,2)它们没有提供符号基础对大脑的影响的全貌。为了研究这一过程所产生的神经变化,我们采用了人工学习范式,让21名成年受试者通过给予特定的符号标签来学习对新的多感官物体进行分类。
{"title":"How the Human Brain Solves the Symbol-Grounding Problem","authors":"Simone Viganò, V. Borghesani, M. Piazza","doi":"10.32470/ccn.2019.1145-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32470/ccn.2019.1145-0","url":null,"abstract":"A fundamental issue in cognitive science is the so-called “symbol-grounding problem” (Harnad 1980), related to the question of how symbols acquire meaning. One simple view posits that, for concrete words, our brain solves the problem by creating associations between the neural representations of the surface forms of symbols (spoken or written words) to the one(s) evoked by the object, action, or event classes the symbols refer to (e.g., see Pulvermuller 2013; 2018). Evidence supporting this view comes from the observation that words related to well known concepts such as numerical quantities (Piazza et al. 2007; Eger et al. 2009), colors (e.g. Simmons et al. 2007), manipulable objects (Chao et al. 1999), places (Kumar et al. 2017), or actions (Hauk 2004; 2011), automatically re-activate the same brain regions that are active during the perception/execution of those specific object features/actions. These data, however, are informative on the neural bases of symbol grounded representations, but not on those underlying symbol grounding: i) they fall short in assessing the role of memory systems implicated in this kind of symbol-toconcept associative learning, and ii) they do not provide a full picture of the effects that symbol grounding has on the brain. Here, to investigate the neural changes generated by this process, we adopted an artificial learning paradigm where 21 adult subjects learned to categorize novel multisensory objects by giving them specific symbolic labels.","PeriodicalId":281121,"journal":{"name":"2019 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience","volume":"18 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"127946944","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How Aging Shapes Neural Representations of Space: fMRI Evidence for Broader Direction Tuning Functions in Older Adults 衰老如何塑造空间的神经表征:老年人更广泛的方向调节功能的fMRI证据
Pub Date : 1900-01-01 DOI: 10.32470/ccn.2019.1228-0
C. Koch, Shu-Chen Li, T. Polk, Nicolas W. Schuck
Human aging is characterized by losses in spatial cognition as well as reductions in distinctiveness of categoryspecific fMRI activation patterns. One mechanism linking theses two phenomena could be that broader neural tuning functions lead to more signal confusions when tuning-based representations of walking direction are read out. To test this idea, we developed a novel method that allowed us to investigate changes in fMRI-measured pattern similarity while participants navigated in different directions in a virtual spatial navigation task. We expected that adjacent directions are represented more similarly within direction sensitive brain areas, reflecting a tuning-function-like signal. Importantly, heightened similarity might lead downstream areas to become more likely to confuse neighboring directions. We therefore analyzed predictions of a decoder trained on these representations, asking (1) whether decoder confusions between two directions increased proportionally to their angular similarity, (2) and how this differs between age groups. Evidence for tuning-function-like signals was found in the retrosplenial complex and primary visual cortex. Significant age differences in tuning width, however, were only found in the primary visual cortex. Our findings introduce a novel approach to measure tuning specificity using fMRI and suggest broader visual direction tuning in older adults might underlie age-related spatial navigation impairments.
人类衰老的特征是空间认知能力的丧失以及类别特异性fMRI激活模式的独特性降低。连接这两种现象的一种机制可能是,当读出基于行走方向的调谐表征时,更广泛的神经调谐功能导致更多的信号混淆。为了验证这一想法,我们开发了一种新方法,使我们能够研究参与者在虚拟空间导航任务中向不同方向导航时,fmri测量的模式相似性的变化。我们期望相邻的方向在方向敏感的大脑区域内表现得更相似,反映出类似调谐功能的信号。重要的是,高度的相似性可能会导致下游地区更容易混淆相邻的方向。因此,我们分析了在这些表征上训练的解码器的预测,询问(1)两个方向之间的解码器混淆是否与它们的角度相似性成比例地增加,(2)以及这在年龄组之间有何不同。在脾后复合体和初级视觉皮层中发现了类似调节功能信号的证据。然而,调谐宽度的显著年龄差异仅在初级视觉皮层中发现。我们的研究结果引入了一种使用功能磁共振成像测量调谐特异性的新方法,并表明老年人更广泛的视觉方向调谐可能是与年龄相关的空间导航障碍的基础。
{"title":"How Aging Shapes Neural Representations of Space: fMRI Evidence for Broader Direction Tuning Functions in Older Adults","authors":"C. Koch, Shu-Chen Li, T. Polk, Nicolas W. Schuck","doi":"10.32470/ccn.2019.1228-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32470/ccn.2019.1228-0","url":null,"abstract":"Human aging is characterized by losses in spatial cognition as well as reductions in distinctiveness of categoryspecific fMRI activation patterns. One mechanism linking theses two phenomena could be that broader neural tuning functions lead to more signal confusions when tuning-based representations of walking direction are read out. To test this idea, we developed a novel method that allowed us to investigate changes in fMRI-measured pattern similarity while participants navigated in different directions in a virtual spatial navigation task. We expected that adjacent directions are represented more similarly within direction sensitive brain areas, reflecting a tuning-function-like signal. Importantly, heightened similarity might lead downstream areas to become more likely to confuse neighboring directions. We therefore analyzed predictions of a decoder trained on these representations, asking (1) whether decoder confusions between two directions increased proportionally to their angular similarity, (2) and how this differs between age groups. Evidence for tuning-function-like signals was found in the retrosplenial complex and primary visual cortex. Significant age differences in tuning width, however, were only found in the primary visual cortex. Our findings introduce a novel approach to measure tuning specificity using fMRI and suggest broader visual direction tuning in older adults might underlie age-related spatial navigation impairments.","PeriodicalId":281121,"journal":{"name":"2019 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience","volume":"19 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"127583323","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Novel Object Scale Differences in Deep Convolutional Neural Networks versus Human Object Recognition Areas 深度卷积神经网络与人类物体识别领域的新目标尺度差异
Pub Date : 1900-01-01 DOI: 10.32470/ccn.2019.1075-0
Astrid Zeman, C. V. Meel, H. O. D. Beeck
Deep Convolutional Neural Networks (CNNs) are lauded for their high accuracy in object classification, as well as their striking similarity to human brain and behaviour. Both humans and CNNs maintain high classification accuracy despite changes in the scale, rotation, and translation of objects. In this study, we present images of novel objects at different scales and compare representational similarity in the human brain versus CNNs. We measure human fMRI responses in primary visual cortex (V1) and the object selective lateral occipital complex (LOC). We also measure the internal representations of CNNs that have been trained for largescale object recognition. Novel objects lack consensus on their name and identity, and therefore do not clearly belong to any specific object category. These novel objects are individuated in LOC, but not V1. V1 and LOC both significantly represent size and pixel information. In contrast, the late layers of CNNs show they are able to individuate objects but do not retain size information. Thus, while the human brain and CNNs are both able to recognise objects in spite of changes to their size, only the human brain retains this size information throughout the later stages of information processing.
深度卷积神经网络(cnn)因其在对象分类方面的高准确性以及与人类大脑和行为的惊人相似性而受到称赞。尽管物体的尺度、旋转和平移发生了变化,但人类和cnn都保持了很高的分类精度。在这项研究中,我们呈现了不同尺度的新物体图像,并比较了人类大脑与cnn的表征相似性。我们测量了人类初级视觉皮层(V1)和客体选择性枕侧复合体(LOC)的fMRI反应。我们还测量了经过大规模目标识别训练的cnn的内部表示。新奇的物体在其名称和身份上缺乏共识,因此不清楚地属于任何特定的物体类别。这些新奇的对象在LOC中是个性化的,而不是V1。V1和LOC都能很好地表示尺寸和像素信息。相比之下,cnn的后期层显示它们能够个性化对象,但不能保留大小信息。因此,虽然人类大脑和cnn都能够在物体大小变化的情况下识别物体,但只有人类大脑在信息处理的后期阶段保留了这个大小信息。
{"title":"Novel Object Scale Differences in Deep Convolutional Neural Networks versus Human Object Recognition Areas","authors":"Astrid Zeman, C. V. Meel, H. O. D. Beeck","doi":"10.32470/ccn.2019.1075-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32470/ccn.2019.1075-0","url":null,"abstract":"Deep Convolutional Neural Networks (CNNs) are lauded for their high accuracy in object classification, as well as their striking similarity to human brain and behaviour. Both humans and CNNs maintain high classification accuracy despite changes in the scale, rotation, and translation of objects. In this study, we present images of novel objects at different scales and compare representational similarity in the human brain versus CNNs. We measure human fMRI responses in primary visual cortex (V1) and the object selective lateral occipital complex (LOC). We also measure the internal representations of CNNs that have been trained for largescale object recognition. Novel objects lack consensus on their name and identity, and therefore do not clearly belong to any specific object category. These novel objects are individuated in LOC, but not V1. V1 and LOC both significantly represent size and pixel information. In contrast, the late layers of CNNs show they are able to individuate objects but do not retain size information. Thus, while the human brain and CNNs are both able to recognise objects in spite of changes to their size, only the human brain retains this size information throughout the later stages of information processing.","PeriodicalId":281121,"journal":{"name":"2019 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience","volume":"56 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"132619615","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Models of allocentric coding for reaching in naturalistic visual scenes 自然视觉场景中伸手的异中心编码模型
Pub Date : 1900-01-01 DOI: 10.32470/ccn.2019.1372-0
Parisa Abedi Khoozani, Paul R. Schrater, Dominik M. Endres, K. Fiehler, Gunnar Blohm
To reach to objects, humans rely on relative positions of target objects to surrounding objects (allocentric) as well as to their own bodies (egocentric). Previous studies demonstrated that scene configuration and object relevancy to the task modulates the combination weights of allocentric and egocentric information. Egocentric coding for reaching is studied extensively; however, how allocentric information is coupled and used in reaching is unknown. Using a computational approach, we show that clustering mechanisms for allocentric coding combined with causal Bayesian integration of allocentric and egocentric information can account for the observed reaching behavior. To further understand allocentric coding, we propose two strategies, global vs. distributed landmark clustering (GLC vs. DLC). Both models can replicate the current data but each has distinct implications. GLC efficiently encodes the scene relative to a single virtual reference but loses all the local structure information. In contrary, DLC stores more redundant inter-object relationship information. Consequently, DLC is more sensitive to the changes of the scene. Further experiments must differentiate between the two proposed strategies.
为了接触到物体,人类依靠目标物体与周围物体的相对位置(非中心)以及与自己身体的相对位置(自我中心)。先前的研究表明,场景配置和目标与任务的相关性调节了非中心和自我中心信息的组合权重。以自我为中心的伸手编码得到了广泛的研究;然而,非中心信息是如何耦合和使用在到达是未知的。通过计算方法,我们证明了非中心编码的聚类机制与非中心和自我中心信息的因果贝叶斯整合可以解释观察到的到达行为。为了进一步理解非中心编码,我们提出了两种策略,全局与分布式地标聚类(GLC vs. DLC)。这两种模型都可以复制当前的数据,但每种模型都有不同的含义。相对于单个虚拟参考,GLC有效地对场景进行编码,但丢失了所有的局部结构信息。相反,DLC存储了更多冗余的对象间关系信息。因此,DLC对场景的变化更加敏感。进一步的实验必须区分这两种策略。
{"title":"Models of allocentric coding for reaching in naturalistic visual scenes","authors":"Parisa Abedi Khoozani, Paul R. Schrater, Dominik M. Endres, K. Fiehler, Gunnar Blohm","doi":"10.32470/ccn.2019.1372-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32470/ccn.2019.1372-0","url":null,"abstract":"To reach to objects, humans rely on relative positions of target objects to surrounding objects (allocentric) as well as to their own bodies (egocentric). Previous studies demonstrated that scene configuration and object relevancy to the task modulates the combination weights of allocentric and egocentric information. Egocentric coding for reaching is studied extensively; however, how allocentric information is coupled and used in reaching is unknown. Using a computational approach, we show that clustering mechanisms for allocentric coding combined with causal Bayesian integration of allocentric and egocentric information can account for the observed reaching behavior. To further understand allocentric coding, we propose two strategies, global vs. distributed landmark clustering (GLC vs. DLC). Both models can replicate the current data but each has distinct implications. GLC efficiently encodes the scene relative to a single virtual reference but loses all the local structure information. In contrary, DLC stores more redundant inter-object relationship information. Consequently, DLC is more sensitive to the changes of the scene. Further experiments must differentiate between the two proposed strategies.","PeriodicalId":281121,"journal":{"name":"2019 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience","volume":"64 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"132290922","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A potential reset mechanism for the modulation of decision processes under uncertainty 不确定性下决策过程调节的潜在重置机制
Pub Date : 1900-01-01 DOI: 10.32470/ccn.2019.1169-0
Krista Bond, Alexis Porter, T. Verstynen
Humans and other mammals flexibly select actions in noisy, uncertain contexts, quickly using feedback to adapt their decision policies to either explore other options or to exploit what they know. Drawing inspiration from the plasticity of cortico-basal ganglia-thalamic circuitry, we recently developed a cognitive model of decision-making that uses both a value-driven learning signal to update an internal estimate of state action-value (i.e., conflict in the probability of reward between two choices) and a change-point-driven learning signal that adapts to changes in reward contingencies (i.e., a previously high value target becoming devalued). In this work, we expand on previous results from our group (Bond, Dunovan, & Verstynen, 2018) to more carefully detail how these environmental signals drive changes in the decision process. Across nine separate behavioral testing sessions, we independently manipulated the level of value-conflict and volatility in action-outcome contingencies. Using a hierarchical drift diffusion model, we found that the belief in the value difference between options had the greatest influence on decision processes, impacting drift rate, while estimates of environmental change had a smaller, but detectable influence on the decision threshold. Taken together, these findings bolster our previous work showing how separate environmental signals impact different aspects of the decision algorithm.
人类和其他哺乳动物在嘈杂、不确定的环境中灵活地选择行动,迅速利用反馈来调整他们的决策政策,以探索其他选择或利用他们所知道的。从皮质-基底神经节-丘脑回路的可塑性中获得灵感,我们最近开发了一种决策的认知模型,该模型既使用价值驱动的学习信号来更新对状态行动价值的内部估计(即,两种选择之间奖励概率的冲突),也使用变化点驱动的学习信号来适应奖励偶然性的变化(即,以前高价值的目标变得贬值)。在这项工作中,我们扩展了我们小组之前的结果(Bond, Dunovan, & Verstynen, 2018),以更仔细地详细说明这些环境信号如何驱动决策过程中的变化。在九个独立的行为测试环节中,我们独立地操纵了行动-结果偶然性中价值冲突和波动性的水平。使用分层漂移扩散模型,我们发现对选项之间价值差异的信念对决策过程的影响最大,影响漂移率,而对环境变化的估计对决策阈值的影响较小,但可检测到。综上所述,这些发现支持了我们之前的工作,即不同的环境信号如何影响决策算法的不同方面。
{"title":"A potential reset mechanism for the modulation of decision processes under uncertainty","authors":"Krista Bond, Alexis Porter, T. Verstynen","doi":"10.32470/ccn.2019.1169-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32470/ccn.2019.1169-0","url":null,"abstract":"Humans and other mammals flexibly select actions in noisy, uncertain contexts, quickly using feedback to adapt their decision policies to either explore other options or to exploit what they know. Drawing inspiration from the plasticity of cortico-basal ganglia-thalamic circuitry, we recently developed a cognitive model of decision-making that uses both a value-driven learning signal to update an internal estimate of state action-value (i.e., conflict in the probability of reward between two choices) and a change-point-driven learning signal that adapts to changes in reward contingencies (i.e., a previously high value target becoming devalued). In this work, we expand on previous results from our group (Bond, Dunovan, & Verstynen, 2018) to more carefully detail how these environmental signals drive changes in the decision process. Across nine separate behavioral testing sessions, we independently manipulated the level of value-conflict and volatility in action-outcome contingencies. Using a hierarchical drift diffusion model, we found that the belief in the value difference between options had the greatest influence on decision processes, impacting drift rate, while estimates of environmental change had a smaller, but detectable influence on the decision threshold. Taken together, these findings bolster our previous work showing how separate environmental signals impact different aspects of the decision algorithm.","PeriodicalId":281121,"journal":{"name":"2019 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"130318531","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
2019 Conference on Cognitive Computational Neuroscience
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1