首页 > 最新文献

Social Psychological Bulletin最新文献

英文 中文
Social position and personal versus social focus: A multinational study of managerial values 社会地位、个人与社会焦点:管理价值观的跨国研究
Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-09-06 DOI: 10.32872/spb.8265
A. Cislak, A. Wójcik, Olga Bialobrzeska
Managerial positions involve influencing others, hence the importance of studying the standards guiding managers' attitudes, decisions, and behavior. Drawing on structural theories and psychological findings on the effects of subjective social status, we predict that holding a managerial position is related to individual value structure via self-perceived social rank of those in managerial positions. We argue that holding a managerial position is associated positively with prioritizing values reflecting personal focus (self-enhancement and openness to change value types) and, as a consequence, negatively with prioritizing values reflecting social focus (self-transcendence and conservation value types). Using data from the European Social Survey 2012 (N = 48,105) from 29 countries, we found a mediating effect of subjective social status between holding a managerial position and personal versus social focus not moderated by the country context. We discuss the implications of these findings for psychological theories of social hierarchy and managerial practice.
管理职位涉及影响他人,因此研究指导管理者态度、决策和行为的标准是很重要的。根据结构理论和主观社会地位影响的心理学研究结果,我们预测管理职位通过管理职位人员的自我感知社会地位与个人价值结构相关。我们认为,担任管理职位与优先考虑反映个人关注的价值观(自我提升和对改变价值类型的开放)呈正相关,因此与优先考虑反映社会关注的价值观(自我超越和保护价值类型)负相关。使用来自29个国家的2012年欧洲社会调查(N = 48,105)的数据,我们发现主观社会地位在持有管理职位和个人与社会焦点之间存在中介效应,而不受国家背景的调节。我们讨论了这些发现对社会等级心理学理论和管理实践的影响。
{"title":"Social position and personal versus social focus: A multinational study of managerial values","authors":"A. Cislak, A. Wójcik, Olga Bialobrzeska","doi":"10.32872/spb.8265","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.8265","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Managerial positions involve influencing others, hence the importance of studying the standards guiding managers' attitudes, decisions, and behavior. Drawing on structural theories and psychological findings on the effects of subjective social status, we predict that holding a managerial position is related to individual value structure via self-perceived social rank of those in managerial positions. We argue that holding a managerial position is associated positively with prioritizing values reflecting personal focus (self-enhancement and openness to change value types) and, as a consequence, negatively with prioritizing values reflecting social focus (self-transcendence and conservation value types). Using data from the European Social Survey 2012 (N = 48,105) from 29 countries, we found a mediating effect of subjective social status between holding a managerial position and personal versus social focus not moderated by the country context. We discuss the implications of these findings for psychological theories of social hierarchy and managerial practice.","PeriodicalId":32922,"journal":{"name":"Social Psychological Bulletin","volume":"35 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-09-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86955639","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Beyond the features: The role of consistency in impressions of trust 特征之外:一致性在信任印象中的作用
Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-09-06 DOI: 10.32872/spb.9233
P. Winkielman, Andrzej Nowak
To be successful in social life, perceivers need to form impressions of other people's trustworthiness. Current models of this process emphasize the role of specific descriptive content–individual verbal and visual features determining trust impressions. In contrast, we describe three lines of our research showing that trust impressions also depend on consistency–a sense of fit–between features. The first line demonstrates that consistency of brief verbal characterizations increases trust judgments. The second line shows that trust judgments and behaviors are boosted by incidental consistency between the foreground and background of visual scenes. The third line observes that consistency between facial features enhances impressions of trustworthiness. In all these studies, consistency (measured via subjective ratings, reaction times, and physiological measures) positively and uniquely predicted trust judgments. Overall, our results, and related findings, show that trust impressions are not a simple sum of the contributing parts, but reflect a “gestalt.” We theoretically locate these findings in frameworks emphasizing the role of fluency, predictive coding, and coherence in social cognition.
为了在社会生活中取得成功,感知者需要形成他人值得信赖的印象。这一过程的当前模型强调具体的描述性内容的作用-个人的语言和视觉特征决定信任印象。相反,我们描述了我们研究的三条线,表明信任印象也取决于一致性——特征之间的契合感。第一行表明,简短的口头描述的一致性增加了信任判断。第二行显示,视觉场景的前景和背景之间偶然的一致性促进了信任判断和行为。第三行观察到面部特征之间的一致性增强了可信度的印象。在所有这些研究中,一致性(通过主观评分、反应时间和生理测量来衡量)积极而独特地预测了信任判断。总的来说,我们的结果和相关的发现表明,信任印象不是贡献部分的简单总和,而是反映了一个“完形”。从理论上讲,我们将这些发现定位在强调流畅性、预测性编码和连贯性在社会认知中的作用的框架中。
{"title":"Beyond the features: The role of consistency in impressions of trust","authors":"P. Winkielman, Andrzej Nowak","doi":"10.32872/spb.9233","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.9233","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 To be successful in social life, perceivers need to form impressions of other people's trustworthiness. Current models of this process emphasize the role of specific descriptive content–individual verbal and visual features determining trust impressions. In contrast, we describe three lines of our research showing that trust impressions also depend on consistency–a sense of fit–between features. The first line demonstrates that consistency of brief verbal characterizations increases trust judgments. The second line shows that trust judgments and behaviors are boosted by incidental consistency between the foreground and background of visual scenes. The third line observes that consistency between facial features enhances impressions of trustworthiness. In all these studies, consistency (measured via subjective ratings, reaction times, and physiological measures) positively and uniquely predicted trust judgments. Overall, our results, and related findings, show that trust impressions are not a simple sum of the contributing parts, but reflect a “gestalt.” We theoretically locate these findings in frameworks emphasizing the role of fluency, predictive coding, and coherence in social cognition.","PeriodicalId":32922,"journal":{"name":"Social Psychological Bulletin","volume":"19 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-09-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81131854","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Followers forever: Prior commitment predicts post-scandal support of a social media celebrity 永远的粉丝:先前的承诺预示着丑闻后社交媒体名人的支持
Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-09-06 DOI: 10.32872/spb.8283
S. Karg, Michelle Lim, S. Schnall
When learning about wrongdoings of others, people are quick to condemn them and make negative inferences about their character. This tends to not be the case, however, when they hold strong positive feelings toward a transgressor, or consider this person to be part of their ingroup. We investigated the extent to which followers of a social media celebrity, Logan Paul, would still support him after a highly publicized scandal, thus exploring whether they would remain loyal given their prior commitment, or instead, feel especially betrayed and therefore revise their previously positive evaluation of him. Using Distributed Dictionary Representations on a large dataset of YouTube followers (N = 36,464) who commented both before and after the scandal, we found that the more often a person had publicly expressed their approval of the protagonist prior to the scandal, the stronger their post-scandal support was. Similarly, prior engagement was also associated with fewer negative moral emotions, and more positive emotions and attempts to defend the transgressor. Furthermore, compared to non-followers of the celebrity, followers were substantially more supportive of him after the scandal. Thus, highly committed fans failed to update existing moral character evaluations even in light of an extreme moral norm violation, a pattern that is consistent with attempts to reduce cognitive dissonance to maintain a positive evaluation of self and transgressor.
当人们了解到别人的错误行为时,他们很快就会谴责他们,并对他们的性格做出负面的推断。然而,当他们对冒犯者抱有强烈的积极情绪,或者认为这个人是他们的内部团体的一部分时,情况往往不是这样。我们调查了社交媒体名人洛根·保罗(Logan Paul)的粉丝在高度曝光的丑闻之后,在多大程度上仍然支持他,从而探索他们是否会在之前的承诺下保持忠诚,或者相反,感到特别被背叛,从而改变他们之前对他的积极评价。在丑闻前后评论的YouTube粉丝(N = 36,464)的大型数据集上使用分布式字典表示,我们发现一个人在丑闻前公开表达对主角的认可越多,他们在丑闻后的支持就越强。同样,先前的参与也与较少的消极道德情绪和更多的积极情绪以及为违法者辩护的尝试有关。此外,与非名人粉丝相比,丑闻发生后,粉丝们对他的支持程度要高得多。因此,即使在极端违反道德规范的情况下,高度忠诚的粉丝也无法更新现有的道德品质评价,这一模式与减少认知失调以保持对自我和违法者的积极评价的尝试是一致的。
{"title":"Followers forever: Prior commitment predicts post-scandal support of a social media celebrity","authors":"S. Karg, Michelle Lim, S. Schnall","doi":"10.32872/spb.8283","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.8283","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 When learning about wrongdoings of others, people are quick to condemn them and make negative inferences about their character. This tends to not be the case, however, when they hold strong positive feelings toward a transgressor, or consider this person to be part of their ingroup. We investigated the extent to which followers of a social media celebrity, Logan Paul, would still support him after a highly publicized scandal, thus exploring whether they would remain loyal given their prior commitment, or instead, feel especially betrayed and therefore revise their previously positive evaluation of him. Using Distributed Dictionary Representations on a large dataset of YouTube followers (N = 36,464) who commented both before and after the scandal, we found that the more often a person had publicly expressed their approval of the protagonist prior to the scandal, the stronger their post-scandal support was. Similarly, prior engagement was also associated with fewer negative moral emotions, and more positive emotions and attempts to defend the transgressor. Furthermore, compared to non-followers of the celebrity, followers were substantially more supportive of him after the scandal. Thus, highly committed fans failed to update existing moral character evaluations even in light of an extreme moral norm violation, a pattern that is consistent with attempts to reduce cognitive dissonance to maintain a positive evaluation of self and transgressor.","PeriodicalId":32922,"journal":{"name":"Social Psychological Bulletin","volume":"4 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-09-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85003178","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Stereotyping, prejudice, and the role of anxiety for compensatory control 刻板印象、偏见和焦虑在代偿控制中的作用
Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-25 DOI: 10.32872/spb.7875
Lisa J Schneider
This work examines the influence of personal control and anxiety on stereotyping and prejudice. In two experiments, personal control was manipulated in an autobiographical experience task. In Experiment 1, participants then completed measures of implicit and explicit gender stereotypes. In Experiment 2, implicit and explicit racial prejudice was assessed. Anxiety was tested as a possible mediator of the relationship between personal control and stereotyping and prejudice, respectively. Low personal control was associated with greater gender stereotyping and racial prejudice in explicit measures. Anxiety mediated the relationship between personal control and stereotyping but not between personal control and prejudice. Also, ingroup identification was found to moderate some of the relations between personal control, anxiety and stereotyping and prejudice. The results provide support for stereotyping and prejudice as compensatory control mechanisms, but evidence is mixed regarding the role of anxiety in mediating the processes.
这项工作考察了个人控制和焦虑对刻板印象和偏见的影响。在两个实验中,在自传体体验任务中操纵个人控制。在实验1中,参与者完成了内隐和外显性别刻板印象的测量。实验2评估内隐和外显种族偏见。焦虑分别作为个人控制与刻板印象和偏见之间关系的可能中介进行了测试。在明确的测量中,低个人控制与更大的性别刻板印象和种族偏见有关。焦虑在个人控制与刻板印象之间起中介作用,而在个人控制与偏见之间不起中介作用。此外,研究发现,群体内认同可以缓和个人控制、焦虑、刻板印象和偏见之间的一些关系。结果为刻板印象和偏见作为代偿性控制机制提供了支持,但关于焦虑在中介过程中的作用的证据是混合的。
{"title":"Stereotyping, prejudice, and the role of anxiety for compensatory control","authors":"Lisa J Schneider","doi":"10.32872/spb.7875","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.7875","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 This work examines the influence of personal control and anxiety on stereotyping and prejudice. In two experiments, personal control was manipulated in an autobiographical experience task. In Experiment 1, participants then completed measures of implicit and explicit gender stereotypes. In Experiment 2, implicit and explicit racial prejudice was assessed. Anxiety was tested as a possible mediator of the relationship between personal control and stereotyping and prejudice, respectively. Low personal control was associated with greater gender stereotyping and racial prejudice in explicit measures. Anxiety mediated the relationship between personal control and stereotyping but not between personal control and prejudice. Also, ingroup identification was found to moderate some of the relations between personal control, anxiety and stereotyping and prejudice. The results provide support for stereotyping and prejudice as compensatory control mechanisms, but evidence is mixed regarding the role of anxiety in mediating the processes.","PeriodicalId":32922,"journal":{"name":"Social Psychological Bulletin","volume":"10 1-2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72459713","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Determinants of adoption and rejection of protective measures during the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic: A longitudinal study in Germany’s second wave 在SARS-CoV-2大流行期间采取和拒绝保护措施的决定因素:德国第二波的纵向研究
Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-04 DOI: 10.32872/spb.7515
Pia Ochel, S. Eitze, Regina Siegers, C. Betsch, Anna Seufert
The adoption and acceptance of protective measures are crucial for containing the ongoing coronavirus disease (COVID-19) pandemic. In a recent article in this journal, Dohle et al. (https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.4315) investigated the influence of risk perceptions and trust in politics and science on those constructs in March/April 2020. Since then, the pandemic has undergone several dynamic changes. We analyzed longitudinal data (N = 800) to investigate whether trust and risk were relevant predictors for pandemic measures at a later stage (October 2020/January 2021). The concept of risk perception was supplemented by risk attitude and affective risk to produce a more comprehensive picture of the risk component. We found that greater trust in science at time point 1 predicted less rejection and more adoption of measures at time point 2. Moreover, trust in politics predicted less rejection of measures. From all aspects of cognitive risk perception, only higher severity predicted lower rejection. All other cognitive aspects were non-significant. However, affective risk was shown to be a major predictor: the more the coronavirus was perceived as frightening and worrisome, the lower the rejection and more frequent the adoption of measures. Also, the higher the risk attitude related to health topics, the less frequent the implementation of measures. We replicated the analysis with predictors from time point 2 and deviations are discussed. Our results indicate that affective risk and general attitude toward health risk are more predictive of taking up measures in the context of COVID-19 than cognitive risk.
采取和接受保护措施对于遏制正在进行的冠状病毒病(COVID-19)大流行至关重要。在本杂志最近的一篇文章中,Dohle等人(https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.4315)在2020年3月/ 4月调查了风险感知和对政治和科学的信任对这些结构的影响。自那时以来,这一流行病经历了若干动态变化。我们分析了纵向数据(N = 800),以调查信任和风险是否是后期(2020年10月/ 2021年1月)大流行措施的相关预测因素。风险感知的概念由风险态度和情感风险补充,以产生更全面的风险组成部分。我们发现,在时间点1对科学的更大信任预示着在时间点2更少的拒绝和更多的采取措施。此外,对政治的信任预示着对措施的抵制会减少。从认知风险感知的各个方面来看,只有严重程度越高,排异反应越低。所有其他认知方面都不显著。然而,情感风险被证明是一个主要的预测因素:冠状病毒越被认为是可怕和令人担忧的,拒绝率就越低,采取措施的频率也越高。此外,与健康主题有关的风险态度越高,实施措施的频率就越低。我们用时间点2的预测器重复了分析,并讨论了偏差。我们的研究结果表明,情感风险和对健康风险的总体态度比认知风险更能预测在COVID-19背景下采取措施。
{"title":"Determinants of adoption and rejection of protective measures during the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic: A longitudinal study in Germany’s second wave","authors":"Pia Ochel, S. Eitze, Regina Siegers, C. Betsch, Anna Seufert","doi":"10.32872/spb.7515","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.7515","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 The adoption and acceptance of protective measures are crucial for containing the ongoing coronavirus disease (COVID-19) pandemic. In a recent article in this journal, Dohle et al. (https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.4315) investigated the influence of risk perceptions and trust in politics and science on those constructs in March/April 2020. Since then, the pandemic has undergone several dynamic changes. We analyzed longitudinal data (N = 800) to investigate whether trust and risk were relevant predictors for pandemic measures at a later stage (October 2020/January 2021). The concept of risk perception was supplemented by risk attitude and affective risk to produce a more comprehensive picture of the risk component. We found that greater trust in science at time point 1 predicted less rejection and more adoption of measures at time point 2. Moreover, trust in politics predicted less rejection of measures. From all aspects of cognitive risk perception, only higher severity predicted lower rejection. All other cognitive aspects were non-significant. However, affective risk was shown to be a major predictor: the more the coronavirus was perceived as frightening and worrisome, the lower the rejection and more frequent the adoption of measures. Also, the higher the risk attitude related to health topics, the less frequent the implementation of measures. We replicated the analysis with predictors from time point 2 and deviations are discussed. Our results indicate that affective risk and general attitude toward health risk are more predictive of taking up measures in the context of COVID-19 than cognitive risk.","PeriodicalId":32922,"journal":{"name":"Social Psychological Bulletin","volume":"107 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-07-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81469529","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
National identification, social dominance orientation, and attitudes towards Black Pete in the Netherlands: Person- and variable-centered analyses 荷兰人的国家认同、社会支配取向与对黑皮特的态度:以人为中心和以变量为中心的分析
Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-04 DOI: 10.32872/spb.7853
Daudi van Veen, Rosanneke A. G. Emmen, J. Mesman
Black Pete, a blackface figure, is a popular but controversial part of the Dutch Sinterklaas festivity. Many ethnically Dutch people do not consider the figure to be a racist phenomenon and prefer not to change the figure, although many Black people in the Netherlands consider the figure to be racist. Prior research and public discourse suggest that national identity and wanting to maintain group dominance may explain why many ethnically Dutch people do not want to change the figure. Using a person-centered approach, we investigated if subgroups could be identified whose positive attitudes towards Black Pete and Sinterklaas clustered with high national identification or social dominance orientation (SDO) among Dutch university students (N = 174). Three subgroups were identified. The high national identification group scored high on positive attitudes towards Sinterklaas, Black Pete, and national identity but low on SDO. The high SDO group scored high on positive attitudes towards Sinterklaas, Black Pete, and SDO but low on national identity. The neutral-indifferent group scored low on these variables. Additionally, using a variable-centered approach, we investigated if higher national identification and SDO would be associated with stronger positive towards Sinterklaas and Black Pete. Higher national identification, but not SDO, was associated with stronger positive attitudes towards Sinterklaas. However, national identification and SDO were both associated with stronger positive attitudes towards Black Pete. These findings suggest that many people who prefer not to change Black Pete also have identity concerns or a preference for cultural dominance.
黑皮特,一个黑脸人物,是荷兰圣诞庆祝活动中一个受欢迎但有争议的部分。许多荷兰人不认为这个数字是种族主义现象,也不愿意改变这个数字,尽管荷兰的许多黑人认为这个数字是种族主义的。先前的研究和公共话语表明,国家认同和想要保持群体优势可能解释了为什么许多荷兰人不想改变这个数字。采用以人为本的方法,我们调查了荷兰大学生中对黑皮特和圣诞老人的积极态度是否与高国家认同或社会优势取向(SDO)聚集在一起的亚群体(N = 174)。确定了三个亚组。国家认同感高的群体在对圣诞老人、黑皮特和国家认同感的积极态度上得分较高,但在SDO方面得分较低。高SDO组在对圣诞老人、黑皮特和SDO的积极态度上得分较高,但在国家认同上得分较低。中立组在这些变量上得分较低。此外,采用变量中心的方法,我们调查了较高的国家认同和SDO是否与对Sinterklaas和Black Pete的强烈积极态度有关。较高的国家认同,而不是SDO,与对Sinterklaas的更强的积极态度相关。然而,国家认同和SDO都与对黑皮特的更强的积极态度有关。这些发现表明,许多不愿意改变黑皮特的人也有身份问题或对文化主导地位的偏好。
{"title":"National identification, social dominance orientation, and attitudes towards Black Pete in the Netherlands: Person- and variable-centered analyses","authors":"Daudi van Veen, Rosanneke A. G. Emmen, J. Mesman","doi":"10.32872/spb.7853","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.7853","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Black Pete, a blackface figure, is a popular but controversial part of the Dutch Sinterklaas festivity. Many ethnically Dutch people do not consider the figure to be a racist phenomenon and prefer not to change the figure, although many Black people in the Netherlands consider the figure to be racist. Prior research and public discourse suggest that national identity and wanting to maintain group dominance may explain why many ethnically Dutch people do not want to change the figure. Using a person-centered approach, we investigated if subgroups could be identified whose positive attitudes towards Black Pete and Sinterklaas clustered with high national identification or social dominance orientation (SDO) among Dutch university students (N = 174). Three subgroups were identified. The high national identification group scored high on positive attitudes towards Sinterklaas, Black Pete, and national identity but low on SDO. The high SDO group scored high on positive attitudes towards Sinterklaas, Black Pete, and SDO but low on national identity. The neutral-indifferent group scored low on these variables. Additionally, using a variable-centered approach, we investigated if higher national identification and SDO would be associated with stronger positive towards Sinterklaas and Black Pete. Higher national identification, but not SDO, was associated with stronger positive attitudes towards Sinterklaas. However, national identification and SDO were both associated with stronger positive attitudes towards Black Pete. These findings suggest that many people who prefer not to change Black Pete also have identity concerns or a preference for cultural dominance.","PeriodicalId":32922,"journal":{"name":"Social Psychological Bulletin","volume":"41 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-07-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87800778","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Political trust by individuals of low socioeconomic status: The key role of anomie 低社会经济地位个体的政治信任:失范的关键作用
Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-05-05 DOI: 10.32872/spb.6897
Thierry Bornand, O. Klein
The socioeconomic status (SES) of individuals is related to their political trust. The higher their status, the more they trust the political system. This well-known relation is generally explained in terms of socialisation. The higher the SES, the more people are exposed to democratic values or interact with trustworthy institutions. This increases political interest, which increases political trust. In this study, we propose a complementary explanation: lower SES enhances the perception that the social fabric is breaking down (anomie), and this reduces political trust. We test this hypothesis by using structural equation modeling (SEM) on a representative survey (n = 1203) conducted in the Wallonia region of Belgium. That region appeared suited to explore our hypothesis because of its long-term economic difficulties. The results reveal that those of low SES have less political trust because they perceive more anomie in society. These results are consistent even when the alternative explanation is taken into account (the socialisation hypothesis). Moreover, the results also showed that a higher level of anomie reduced interpersonal trust which reduced political trust (serial mediation). These results highlight the key role of anomie when considering the relation of SES with political trust.
个体的社会经济地位与其政治信任相关。他们的地位越高,就越信任政治体制。这种众所周知的关系通常可以从社会化的角度来解释。社会经济地位越高,人们接触到民主价值观或与值得信赖的机构互动的人就越多。这增加了政治兴趣,从而增加了政治信任。在本研究中,我们提出了一个补充解释:较低的社会经济地位增强了社会结构崩溃(失范)的感觉,这降低了政治信任。我们通过在比利时瓦隆地区进行的一项代表性调查(n = 1203)上使用结构方程模型(SEM)来检验这一假设。由于该地区长期的经济困难,它似乎适合探索我们的假设。结果表明,低社会经济地位的人对政治的信任程度较低,因为他们认为社会中存在更多的失范现象。即使考虑到另一种解释(社会化假设),这些结果也是一致的。此外,研究结果还显示,较高的失范水平会降低人际信任,进而降低政治信任(序列中介)。这些结果表明,在考虑社会经济地位与政治信任的关系时,社会失范是关键因素。
{"title":"Political trust by individuals of low socioeconomic status: The key role of anomie","authors":"Thierry Bornand, O. Klein","doi":"10.32872/spb.6897","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.6897","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 The socioeconomic status (SES) of individuals is related to their political trust. The higher their status, the more they trust the political system. This well-known relation is generally explained in terms of socialisation. The higher the SES, the more people are exposed to democratic values or interact with trustworthy institutions. This increases political interest, which increases political trust. In this study, we propose a complementary explanation: lower SES enhances the perception that the social fabric is breaking down (anomie), and this reduces political trust. We test this hypothesis by using structural equation modeling (SEM) on a representative survey (n = 1203) conducted in the Wallonia region of Belgium. That region appeared suited to explore our hypothesis because of its long-term economic difficulties. The results reveal that those of low SES have less political trust because they perceive more anomie in society. These results are consistent even when the alternative explanation is taken into account (the socialisation hypothesis). Moreover, the results also showed that a higher level of anomie reduced interpersonal trust which reduced political trust (serial mediation). These results highlight the key role of anomie when considering the relation of SES with political trust.","PeriodicalId":32922,"journal":{"name":"Social Psychological Bulletin","volume":"8 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-05-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84537858","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Biological sex and psychological gender differences in the experience and expression of romantic jealousy 生理性别和心理性别在浪漫嫉妒的体验和表达上的差异
Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-04-07 DOI: 10.32872/spb.4161
P. Banaszkiewicz
Romantic jealousy is a multidimensional response to a perceived threat to one’s relationship or self-esteem and the specific emotions experienced in the process are complex and interrelated, affecting one another. Many researchers focus on jealousy-related sex differences, however there are few studies exploring gender-specific jealousy. The current study investigated whether individuals representing various types of biological sex and psychological gender differ in their experience and expression of romantic jealousy. The study involved 367 subjects (213 women, 154 men) ranging in age from 18 to 40 years. The assessments were carried out using the Psychological Gender Inventory based on gender schema theory, proposed by Bem, and the author’s own Questionnaire on the Emotion of Romantic Jealousy. The results of MANOVA showed associations between romantic jealousy and both biological sex and psychological gender, however efforts to save the relationship appear to be the only gender-differentiated response to jealousy. Those with a high level of feminine traits are more likely to take action to preserve their relationships. Overall negative emotions elicited by a partner’s infidelity are stronger in women and in feminine individuals. The results confirm it is necessary to take psychological gender into account in research focusing on jealousy. The findings, however, do not support claims suggesting that men and masculine individuals tend to respond with stronger aggression to a partner’s infidelity, as proposed in the literature.
浪漫的嫉妒是对一个人的关系或自尊的感知威胁的多维反应,在这个过程中所经历的特定情绪是复杂的,相互关联的,相互影响的。许多研究人员关注与嫉妒有关的性别差异,但很少有研究探讨性别嫉妒。目前的研究调查了不同生理性别和心理性别的个体在浪漫嫉妒的经历和表达上是否存在差异。该研究涉及367名受试者(213名女性,154名男性),年龄从18岁到40岁不等。本研究采用基于性别图式理论的心理性别量表(Psychological Gender Inventory)和作者自编的《浪漫嫉妒情绪问卷》进行评估。方差分析的结果显示了浪漫嫉妒与生理性别和心理性别之间的联系,然而,挽救关系的努力似乎是对嫉妒的唯一性别差异反应。那些女性化程度高的人更有可能采取行动来维护他们的关系。伴侣不忠引发的整体负面情绪在女性和女性个体中更为强烈。研究结果证实,在关注嫉妒的研究中,有必要考虑心理性别因素。然而,研究结果并不支持文献中提出的男性和有男子气概的人对伴侣的不忠倾向于做出更强烈的攻击反应的说法。
{"title":"Biological sex and psychological gender differences in the experience and expression of romantic jealousy","authors":"P. Banaszkiewicz","doi":"10.32872/spb.4161","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.4161","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Romantic jealousy is a multidimensional response to a perceived threat to one’s relationship or self-esteem and the specific emotions experienced in the process are complex and interrelated, affecting one another. Many researchers focus on jealousy-related sex differences, however there are few studies exploring gender-specific jealousy. The current study investigated whether individuals representing various types of biological sex and psychological gender differ in their experience and expression of romantic jealousy. The study involved 367 subjects (213 women, 154 men) ranging in age from 18 to 40 years. The assessments were carried out using the Psychological Gender Inventory based on gender schema theory, proposed by Bem, and the author’s own Questionnaire on the Emotion of Romantic Jealousy. The results of MANOVA showed associations between romantic jealousy and both biological sex and psychological gender, however efforts to save the relationship appear to be the only gender-differentiated response to jealousy. Those with a high level of feminine traits are more likely to take action to preserve their relationships. Overall negative emotions elicited by a partner’s infidelity are stronger in women and in feminine individuals. The results confirm it is necessary to take psychological gender into account in research focusing on jealousy. The findings, however, do not support claims suggesting that men and masculine individuals tend to respond with stronger aggression to a partner’s infidelity, as proposed in the literature.","PeriodicalId":32922,"journal":{"name":"Social Psychological Bulletin","volume":"7 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-04-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87540382","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Welcome letter by the new editors 新编辑的欢迎信
Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-04-06 DOI: 10.32872/spb.8359
Katarzyna Jaśko, K. Cantarero, Monika Wróbel
Starting from 2022, we are taking over the role of Co-Editors-in-Chief of Social Psycho­ logical Bulletin. We are honored and delighted to serve the journal for the next five years, after the outstanding leadership of Michał Parzuchowski and Marcin Bukowski, outgoing Editors-in-Chief (2017–2021). Inspired by the vision of Maria Lewicka, who was the founding Editor-in-Chief (2006–2016), Michał and Marcin introduced far-reaching changes that have already produced visible results. In short, the previous team managed to successfully transform SPB from a solid but local Polish journal in social psychology to a truly international journal making it relevant to a global audience. As a direct result of Marcin and Michał’s impressive efforts, fifteen issues that have been published since the transformation to an international journal included more than a hundred articles authored by 202 researchers from 24 countries. Thanks to the collab­ oration with Psychopen.eu and the help of generous sponsors (above all, the Polish Social Psychological Society, the founder and owner of the title), the journal is now fully open access and without any costs for readers and authors. As such, it satisfies the highest – diamond gold – Open Access model. Importantly, that was not the only change initiated by the previous team toward more open and transparent journal practi­ ces. Social Psychological Bulletin has been strongly engaged in promoting open science practices (e.g., pre-registration, open data, open materials) with the aim of implementing the highest standards from the Transparency and Openness Promotion (TOP) guidelines (https://topfactor.org/). We would like to thank Michał and Marcin and all past Editors of SPB for all the work they put in to turning the journal into a significant international outlet for important Editorial
从2022年开始,我们将担任《社会心理学通报》的联合主编。在即将离任的主编michaowparzuchowski和Marcin Bukowski的杰出领导下(2017-2021),我们很荣幸也很高兴在未来五年内为该杂志服务。受2006-2016年创刊总编辑玛丽亚·乐维卡(Maria Lewicka)愿景的启发,米夏沃夫和马尔辛引入了影响深远的变革,这些变革已经产生了明显的效果。简而言之,之前的团队成功地将SPB从一个坚实的波兰本地社会心理学期刊转变为一个真正的国际期刊,使其与全球受众相关。由于Marcin和michakov令人印象深刻的努力,自转变为国际期刊以来,已经出版的15期包括来自24个国家的202名研究人员撰写的100多篇文章。感谢与Psychopen的合作。在慷慨赞助者的帮助下(最重要的是,波兰社会心理学会,该杂志的创始人和所有者),该杂志现在完全开放获取,对读者和作者不收取任何费用。因此,它满足了最高的钻石金——开放存取模型。重要的是,这并不是前一个团队向更开放和透明的期刊实践发起的唯一变化。《社会心理学公报》一直致力于促进开放科学实践(例如,预注册、开放数据、开放材料),旨在实施透明度和开放性促进(TOP)指南(https://topfactor.org/)的最高标准。我们要感谢michaowand Marcin和所有SPB的前任编辑,感谢他们所做的一切,使该杂志成为重要社论的重要国际出口
{"title":"Welcome letter by the new editors","authors":"Katarzyna Jaśko, K. Cantarero, Monika Wróbel","doi":"10.32872/spb.8359","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.8359","url":null,"abstract":"Starting from 2022, we are taking over the role of Co-Editors-in-Chief of Social Psycho­ logical Bulletin. We are honored and delighted to serve the journal for the next five years, after the outstanding leadership of Michał Parzuchowski and Marcin Bukowski, outgoing Editors-in-Chief (2017–2021). Inspired by the vision of Maria Lewicka, who was the founding Editor-in-Chief (2006–2016), Michał and Marcin introduced far-reaching changes that have already produced visible results. In short, the previous team managed to successfully transform SPB from a solid but local Polish journal in social psychology to a truly international journal making it relevant to a global audience. As a direct result of Marcin and Michał’s impressive efforts, fifteen issues that have been published since the transformation to an international journal included more than a hundred articles authored by 202 researchers from 24 countries. Thanks to the collab­ oration with Psychopen.eu and the help of generous sponsors (above all, the Polish Social Psychological Society, the founder and owner of the title), the journal is now fully open access and without any costs for readers and authors. As such, it satisfies the highest – diamond gold – Open Access model. Importantly, that was not the only change initiated by the previous team toward more open and transparent journal practi­ ces. Social Psychological Bulletin has been strongly engaged in promoting open science practices (e.g., pre-registration, open data, open materials) with the aim of implementing the highest standards from the Transparency and Openness Promotion (TOP) guidelines (https://topfactor.org/). We would like to thank Michał and Marcin and all past Editors of SPB for all the work they put in to turning the journal into a significant international outlet for important Editorial","PeriodicalId":32922,"journal":{"name":"Social Psychological Bulletin","volume":"166 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-04-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73283131","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rape myth acceptance is lower and predicts harsher evaluations of rape among impacted people 强奸神话的接受度较低,在受影响的人群中预示着对强奸的更严厉的评价
Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-03-14 DOI: 10.32872/spb.3897
Boglárka Nyúl, Hadi Sam Nariman, M. Szabó, Dávid Ferenczy, Anna Kende
Public reactions to rape are often distorted by the acceptance of so-called rape myths. The goal of our research was to examine how rape myth acceptance (RMA) is connected to the evaluation of rape cases among survivors, unimpacted people, and those impacted by rape through a close relation, who can potentially be important allies of survivors in bringing about social change. We tested these connections in three online survey studies. In Study 1 (N = 758) we found that those impacted by rape personally or through a close relation accepted rape myths less. In Study 2, using a nationally representative sample in Hungary (N = 1007), we tested whether RMA predicted uncertain rape cases more strongly than certain (i.e., stereotypical) ones, considering that a stereotypical rape scenario is condemned by most members of society, but not all rape is labeled as such. We found that RMA predicted the evaluation of both rape scenarios, but the prediction was stronger when rape was uncertain. In Study 3 (N = 384), in a pre-registered study we examined how RMA predicted the evaluation of rape cases amongst people with different previous experiences (impacted/unimpacted). We found that unimpacted people accepted rape myths more, blamed the victim more and labeled the case less as rape when the case was uncertain. These findings suggest that rape myth acceptance functions as cognitive schema and that rape impacted people could have a key role not only in the life of survivors but as allies for social change as well.
公众对强奸的反应往往被所谓的强奸神话所扭曲。我们的研究目的是研究强奸神话接受(RMA)如何与幸存者、未受影响的人以及那些通过亲密关系受到强奸影响的人对强奸案件的评估联系起来,这些人可能是幸存者带来社会变革的重要盟友。我们在三个在线调查研究中测试了这些联系。在研究1 (N = 758)中,我们发现那些亲身或通过亲密关系受到强奸影响的人更少接受强奸神话。在研究2中,我们在匈牙利使用了一个具有全国代表性的样本(N = 1007),我们测试了RMA对不确定强奸案件的预测是否比特定(即刻板印象)案件更强,因为刻板印象的强奸场景受到大多数社会成员的谴责,但并非所有强奸都被贴上了这样的标签。研究发现,RMA对两种强奸情景的评价均有预测作用,但在强奸具有不确定性时预测作用更强。在研究3 (N = 384)中,在一项预先登记的研究中,我们研究了RMA如何预测具有不同先前经历(受影响/未受影响)的人对强奸案件的评估。我们发现,未受影响的人更容易接受强奸神话,更多地指责受害者,在案件不确定时更少地将案件贴上强奸的标签。这些发现表明,对强奸神话的接受是一种认知模式,受到强奸影响的人不仅在幸存者的生活中发挥着关键作用,而且还可能成为社会变革的盟友。
{"title":"Rape myth acceptance is lower and predicts harsher evaluations of rape among impacted people","authors":"Boglárka Nyúl, Hadi Sam Nariman, M. Szabó, Dávid Ferenczy, Anna Kende","doi":"10.32872/spb.3897","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.3897","url":null,"abstract":"Public reactions to rape are often distorted by the acceptance of so-called rape myths. The goal of our research was to examine how rape myth acceptance (RMA) is connected to the evaluation of rape cases among survivors, unimpacted people, and those impacted by rape through a close relation, who can potentially be important allies of survivors in bringing about social change. We tested these connections in three online survey studies. In Study 1 (N = 758) we found that those impacted by rape personally or through a close relation accepted rape myths less. In Study 2, using a nationally representative sample in Hungary (N = 1007), we tested whether RMA predicted uncertain rape cases more strongly than certain (i.e., stereotypical) ones, considering that a stereotypical rape scenario is condemned by most members of society, but not all rape is labeled as such. We found that RMA predicted the evaluation of both rape scenarios, but the prediction was stronger when rape was uncertain. In Study 3 (N = 384), in a pre-registered study we examined how RMA predicted the evaluation of rape cases amongst people with different previous experiences (impacted/unimpacted). We found that unimpacted people accepted rape myths more, blamed the victim more and labeled the case less as rape when the case was uncertain. These findings suggest that rape myth acceptance functions as cognitive schema and that rape impacted people could have a key role not only in the life of survivors but as allies for social change as well.","PeriodicalId":32922,"journal":{"name":"Social Psychological Bulletin","volume":"12 4 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-03-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80404408","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Social Psychological Bulletin
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1