首页 > 最新文献

Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
The Incentive Sensitization Theory of Addiction 成瘾的激励敏感化理论
Pub Date : 2022-01-28 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.715
Mike J. F. Robinson, Alicia S. Zumbusch, Patrick Anselme
Many theoretical constructs have been formulated over the years to explain the phenomenon of addiction. While the incentive sensitization theory of addiction acknowledges the important contributions of many former theories, it postulates that addiction is a state of aberrant motivation. Through repeated drug use, individuals with addiction become hypersensitive to the effects of the drugs themselves and to the stimuli associated with these drugs, including a variety of drug paraphernalia. For all individuals consuming drugs, drug-related stimuli have an inherent predictive value that signals an impending dose of the drug. For people with addiction, these drug cues move beyond being merely predictors for the drug and are imbued with excessive motivational value (called incentive salience); they become powerful motivational magnets capable of instigating and enhancing cravings for the drug. This incentive sensitization occurs through a process of neuroadaptations in the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system that have been shown to be long-lasting. These brain changes yield increasingly intense, highly focused cravings for an addictive target and transform cues related to the target into incentive stimuli that promote compulsive reward-seeking and relapse. The incentive sensitization theory does not deny a role for pleasure, habits, and withdrawal in addiction, but posits that those individuals with addiction (a) continue to take drugs compulsively even while experiencing diminished pleasure, (b) demonstrate creative new ways to procure drugs when necessary, and (c) often relapse well beyond when withdrawal has subsided. The critical factor in the development and maintenance of addiction is the persistent neuroadaptation that sensitizes the attribution of incentive salience to drugs and their cues, which explains why recovering from addiction is a long and slow process. The incentive sensitization theory can account for drug-induced attentional bias as well as how addiction can develop toward nondrug reward sources such as food, sex, and gambling environments.
多年来,人们提出了许多理论来解释成瘾现象。虽然成瘾的激励敏感化理论承认许多先前理论的重要贡献,但它假设成瘾是一种异常动机的状态。通过反复使用药物,成瘾的个体对药物本身的影响以及与这些药物相关的刺激(包括各种药物用具)变得高度敏感。对于所有服用药物的人来说,与药物相关的刺激具有内在的预测价值,它预示着即将到来的药物剂量。对于成瘾的人来说,这些药物线索不仅仅是药物的预测因素,而且充满了过度的动机价值(称为激励显著性);它们变成了强大的激励磁铁,能够煽动和增强对毒品的渴望。这种激励敏化是通过中皮质边缘多巴胺系统的神经适应过程发生的,该过程已被证明是持久的。这些大脑变化产生了对成瘾目标越来越强烈、高度集中的渴望,并将与目标相关的线索转化为刺激刺激,促进强迫性的寻求奖励和复发。激励敏化理论并不否认快乐、习惯和戒断在成瘾中的作用,但假设那些成瘾的人(a)即使在体验到减少的快乐时仍会强迫性地继续服用药物,(b)在必要时展示创造性的新方法来获得药物,(c)在戒断消退后经常复发。成瘾发展和维持的关键因素是持续的神经适应,它使药物及其线索的激励显著性归因变得敏感,这解释了为什么从成瘾中恢复是一个漫长而缓慢的过程。激励敏化理论可以解释药物引起的注意偏差,以及成瘾如何向非药物奖励来源发展,如食物、性和赌博环境。
{"title":"The Incentive Sensitization Theory of Addiction","authors":"Mike J. F. Robinson, Alicia S. Zumbusch, Patrick Anselme","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.715","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.715","url":null,"abstract":"Many theoretical constructs have been formulated over the years to explain the phenomenon of addiction. While the incentive sensitization theory of addiction acknowledges the important contributions of many former theories, it postulates that addiction is a state of aberrant motivation. Through repeated drug use, individuals with addiction become hypersensitive to the effects of the drugs themselves and to the stimuli associated with these drugs, including a variety of drug paraphernalia. For all individuals consuming drugs, drug-related stimuli have an inherent predictive value that signals an impending dose of the drug. For people with addiction, these drug cues move beyond being merely predictors for the drug and are imbued with excessive motivational value (called incentive salience); they become powerful motivational magnets capable of instigating and enhancing cravings for the drug. This incentive sensitization occurs through a process of neuroadaptations in the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system that have been shown to be long-lasting. These brain changes yield increasingly intense, highly focused cravings for an addictive target and transform cues related to the target into incentive stimuli that promote compulsive reward-seeking and relapse. The incentive sensitization theory does not deny a role for pleasure, habits, and withdrawal in addiction, but posits that those individuals with addiction (a) continue to take drugs compulsively even while experiencing diminished pleasure, (b) demonstrate creative new ways to procure drugs when necessary, and (c) often relapse well beyond when withdrawal has subsided. The critical factor in the development and maintenance of addiction is the persistent neuroadaptation that sensitizes the attribution of incentive salience to drugs and their cues, which explains why recovering from addiction is a long and slow process. The incentive sensitization theory can account for drug-induced attentional bias as well as how addiction can develop toward nondrug reward sources such as food, sex, and gambling environments.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"12 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-01-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"126206091","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 24
Music Performance 音乐表演
Pub Date : 2021-12-22 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.492
Peter Q. Pfordresher
Music performance involves precise motor control that is coordinated with higher order planning to convey complex structural information. In addition, music performance usually involves motor tasks that are not learned spontaneously (as in the use of the vocal apparatus), the reproduction of preestablished sequences (notated or from memory), and synchronized joint performance with one or more other musicians. Music performance also relies on a rich repertoire of musical knowledge that can be used for purposes of expressive variation and improvisation. As such, the study of music performance provides a way to explore learning, motor control, memory, and interpersonal coordination in the context of a real-world behavior. Music performance skills vary considerably in the population and reflect interactions between genetic predispositions and the effect of intensive practice. At the same time, research suggests that most individuals have the capacity to perform music through singing or learning an instrument, and in this sense music performance taps into a universal human propensity for communication and coordination with conspecifics.
音乐表演需要精确的马达控制,并配合高阶规划来传达复杂的结构信息。此外,音乐表演通常涉及一些不是自发学习的运动任务(如使用发声器官),对预先建立的序列的再现(标记或记忆),以及与一个或多个其他音乐家的同步联合表演。音乐表演也依赖于丰富的音乐知识,可以用于表达变化和即兴创作的目的。因此,对音乐表演的研究提供了一种在现实世界行为背景下探索学习、运动控制、记忆和人际协调的方法。不同人群的音乐表演技能差异很大,这反映了遗传倾向和强化练习效果之间的相互作用。与此同时,研究表明,大多数人都有能力通过唱歌或学习一种乐器来表演音乐,从这个意义上说,音乐表演利用了人类与同类交流和协调的普遍倾向。
{"title":"Music Performance","authors":"Peter Q. Pfordresher","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.492","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.492","url":null,"abstract":"Music performance involves precise motor control that is coordinated with higher order planning to convey complex structural information. In addition, music performance usually involves motor tasks that are not learned spontaneously (as in the use of the vocal apparatus), the reproduction of preestablished sequences (notated or from memory), and synchronized joint performance with one or more other musicians. Music performance also relies on a rich repertoire of musical knowledge that can be used for purposes of expressive variation and improvisation. As such, the study of music performance provides a way to explore learning, motor control, memory, and interpersonal coordination in the context of a real-world behavior. Music performance skills vary considerably in the population and reflect interactions between genetic predispositions and the effect of intensive practice. At the same time, research suggests that most individuals have the capacity to perform music through singing or learning an instrument, and in this sense music performance taps into a universal human propensity for communication and coordination with conspecifics.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"28 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-12-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"128680258","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Multistable Perception 多稳态的感觉
Pub Date : 2021-11-29 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.893
A. Pastukhov
Multistable perception is produced by stimuli that are consistent with two or more different comparably likely perceptual interpretations. After the initial perception is resolved in favor of one of the interpretations, continued viewing leads to fluctuating subjective experience, as perception spontaneously switches between alternative states. Multistable perception occurs for different modalities, including visual, auditory, tactile, olfactory perception and proprioception, and various conflicting sensory representations, such as eye dominance, depth, motion, or meaning. Despite large differences, multistable stimuli produce quantitatively similar perceptual experience with stereotypical distribution of durations of dominance phases, similar dependence on the absolute and relative strength of competing perceptual interpretations, prior perceptual history, presentation method, attention, and volitional control, and so on. Taken together, this shows that multistable perception reflects the action of general canonical perceptual mechanisms whose purpose is to resolve the conflicting evidence and ensure a single dominant perception that can be used for action. Thus, it informs us about mechanisms of perceptual decision making, including the importance of feedback mechanisms in resolving perceptual ambiguity and the role of parietal and frontal regions in facilitating changes in perception. Multistable perception provides useful constraints for models, inspiring a plethora of models of perception that combine neurally plausible mechanisms, such as neural adaptation and inhibition, or are based on the idea of predictive coding. The sensitive nature of multistable perception makes a valuable experimental tool that can reveal even minor differences due to low- or high-level influences, including genetic or clinical cases. As such, it is an important tool in studying neural and behavioral correlates of consciousness as it dissociates perception from the stimulus.
多稳态知觉是由与两种或两种以上不同的比较可能的知觉解释相一致的刺激产生的。在最初的感知被一种解释所解决后,继续观看会导致主观体验的波动,因为感知会自发地在不同的状态之间切换。多稳态感知发生于不同的模式,包括视觉、听觉、触觉、嗅觉和本体感受,以及各种相互冲突的感觉表征,如眼睛主导、深度、运动或意义。尽管存在巨大差异,但多稳定刺激在数量上产生相似的感知体验,包括优势阶段持续时间的刻板分布、对竞争感知解释的绝对和相对强度的相似依赖、先前的感知历史、呈现方法、注意和意志控制等。综上所述,这表明多稳定感知反映了一般规范感知机制的作用,其目的是解决相互矛盾的证据,并确保可以用于行动的单一主导感知。因此,它告诉我们知觉决策的机制,包括反馈机制在解决知觉模糊中的重要性,以及顶叶和额叶区域在促进知觉变化中的作用。多稳定感知为模型提供了有用的约束,激发了大量的感知模型,这些模型结合了神经似是而非的机制,如神经适应和抑制,或者基于预测编码的想法。多稳态感知的敏感性使其成为一种有价值的实验工具,可以揭示由于低或高影响(包括遗传或临床病例)而产生的微小差异。因此,它是研究意识的神经和行为相关性的重要工具,因为它将感知与刺激分离开来。
{"title":"Multistable Perception","authors":"A. Pastukhov","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.893","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.893","url":null,"abstract":"Multistable perception is produced by stimuli that are consistent with two or more different comparably likely perceptual interpretations. After the initial perception is resolved in favor of one of the interpretations, continued viewing leads to fluctuating subjective experience, as perception spontaneously switches between alternative states. Multistable perception occurs for different modalities, including visual, auditory, tactile, olfactory perception and proprioception, and various conflicting sensory representations, such as eye dominance, depth, motion, or meaning. Despite large differences, multistable stimuli produce quantitatively similar perceptual experience with stereotypical distribution of durations of dominance phases, similar dependence on the absolute and relative strength of competing perceptual interpretations, prior perceptual history, presentation method, attention, and volitional control, and so on. Taken together, this shows that multistable perception reflects the action of general canonical perceptual mechanisms whose purpose is to resolve the conflicting evidence and ensure a single dominant perception that can be used for action. Thus, it informs us about mechanisms of perceptual decision making, including the importance of feedback mechanisms in resolving perceptual ambiguity and the role of parietal and frontal regions in facilitating changes in perception. Multistable perception provides useful constraints for models, inspiring a plethora of models of perception that combine neurally plausible mechanisms, such as neural adaptation and inhibition, or are based on the idea of predictive coding. The sensitive nature of multistable perception makes a valuable experimental tool that can reveal even minor differences due to low- or high-level influences, including genetic or clinical cases. As such, it is an important tool in studying neural and behavioral correlates of consciousness as it dissociates perception from the stimulus.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"200 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"115015676","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Inference in Social Cognition 社会认知推理
Pub Date : 2021-11-29 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.834
D. V. Becker, C. Unkelbach, K. Fiedler
Inferences are ubiquitous in social cognition, governing everything from first impressions to the communication of meaning itself. Social cognitive inferences are typically varieties of diagnostic reasoning or, more properly, “abductive” reasoning, in which people infer simple but plausible—although not deductively certain—underlying causes for observable social behaviors. Abductive inference and its relationship to inductive and deductive inference are first introduced. A description of how abductive inference operates on a continuum between those that arise rapidly and automatically (and appear like deductions) and those that inspire more deliberative efforts (and thus often recruit more inductive information gathering and testing) is then given. Next, many classic findings in social cognition, and social psychology more broadly, that reveal how widespread this type of inference is explored. Indeed, both judgements under uncertainty and dual-process theories can be illuminated by incorporating the abductive frame. What then follows is a discussion on the work in ecological and evolutionary approaches that suggest that, although these inferences often go beyond the information given and are prone to predictable errors, people are good enough at social inference to qualify as being “ecologically rational.” The conclusion explores emerging themes in social cognition that only heighten the need for this broader understanding of inference processes.
推理在社会认知中无处不在,控制着从第一印象到意义本身的交流的一切。社会认知推理是典型的诊断推理的变种,或者更恰当地说,是“溯因”推理,在这种推理中,人们为可观察到的社会行为推断出简单但合理的——尽管不是演绎的——潜在原因。首先介绍了溯因推理及其与归纳推理和演绎推理的关系。然后给出了溯因推理是如何在一个连续体上运行的描述,在那些快速和自动出现的推理(看起来像演绎)和那些激发更多深思熟虑的努力(因此通常需要更多的归纳信息收集和测试)之间。接下来,社会认知和更广泛的社会心理学领域的许多经典发现揭示了这种类型的推理被探索的广泛程度。事实上,不确定性和双过程理论下的判断都可以通过引入溯因框架来解释。接下来是对生态学和进化方法工作的讨论,这表明,尽管这些推断经常超出给定的信息,容易出现可预测的错误,但人们在社会推断方面足够优秀,可以被称为“生态理性”。结论探讨了社会认知中的新兴主题,这些主题只会提高对推理过程更广泛理解的需求。
{"title":"Inference in Social Cognition","authors":"D. V. Becker, C. Unkelbach, K. Fiedler","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.834","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.834","url":null,"abstract":"Inferences are ubiquitous in social cognition, governing everything from first impressions to the communication of meaning itself. Social cognitive inferences are typically varieties of diagnostic reasoning or, more properly, “abductive” reasoning, in which people infer simple but plausible—although not deductively certain—underlying causes for observable social behaviors. Abductive inference and its relationship to inductive and deductive inference are first introduced. A description of how abductive inference operates on a continuum between those that arise rapidly and automatically (and appear like deductions) and those that inspire more deliberative efforts (and thus often recruit more inductive information gathering and testing) is then given. Next, many classic findings in social cognition, and social psychology more broadly, that reveal how widespread this type of inference is explored. Indeed, both judgements under uncertainty and dual-process theories can be illuminated by incorporating the abductive frame. What then follows is a discussion on the work in ecological and evolutionary approaches that suggest that, although these inferences often go beyond the information given and are prone to predictable errors, people are good enough at social inference to qualify as being “ecologically rational.” The conclusion explores emerging themes in social cognition that only heighten the need for this broader understanding of inference processes.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"10 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"125358154","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Telework and Remote Work 远程工作和远程工作
Pub Date : 2021-10-29 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.850
M. Vartiainen
“Telework” and “remote work” have both increased sharply in recent years during and after the pandemic. The basic difference between telework and remote work is that a teleworker uses personal electronic devices in addition to working physically remotely from a place other than an office or company premises, whereas remote work does not require visits to the main workplace or the use of electronic personal devices. “Mobile tele- and remote workers” use several other places in addition to home for working. “Digital online telework” is a global form of employment that uses online platforms to enable individuals, teams, and organizations to access other individuals or organizations to solve problems or to provide services in exchange for payment. Often tele- and remote workers cowork in virtual teams and projects. The prevalence of various types of tele- and remote working vary. Although there are conceptual challenges to operationalizing the concept, it is estimated that hundreds of millions—and possibly more—people today earn their living working at and from their home or other places using digital tools and platforms. In the future, it is expected that new hybrid modes of working will emerge enabled by digital technologies. These changes in working increase the complexity of job demands because of the increased variety of contextual job characteristics. The main benefits of these new ways of working are organizational flexibility and individual autonomy; at the same time, unclear social relations may increase feelings of isolation and challenge the work-life balance.
近年来,在大流行期间和之后,"远程工作"和"远程工作"都急剧增加。远程工作和远程工作的基本区别在于,远程工作者除了在办公室或公司场所以外的地方进行物理远程工作外,还使用个人电子设备,而远程工作不需要访问主要工作场所或使用电子个人设备。“移动远程和远程工作者”除了在家工作外,还使用其他几个地方。“数字在线远程工作”是一种全球性的就业形式,它使用在线平台使个人、团队和组织能够访问其他个人或组织来解决问题或提供服务以换取报酬。远程工作者经常在虚拟团队和项目中合作。各种类型的远程和远程工作的流行程度各不相同。尽管在实施这一概念方面存在概念上的挑战,但据估计,今天有数亿甚至更多的人使用数字工具和平台在家中或其他地方工作谋生。在未来,预计数字技术将催生新的混合工作模式。工作中的这些变化增加了工作需求的复杂性,因为上下文工作特征的多样性增加了。这些新的工作方式的主要好处是组织的灵活性和个人的自主权;与此同时,不明确的社会关系可能会增加孤独感,挑战工作与生活的平衡。
{"title":"Telework and Remote Work","authors":"M. Vartiainen","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.850","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.850","url":null,"abstract":"“Telework” and “remote work” have both increased sharply in recent years during and after the pandemic. The basic difference between telework and remote work is that a teleworker uses personal electronic devices in addition to working physically remotely from a place other than an office or company premises, whereas remote work does not require visits to the main workplace or the use of electronic personal devices. “Mobile tele- and remote workers” use several other places in addition to home for working. “Digital online telework” is a global form of employment that uses online platforms to enable individuals, teams, and organizations to access other individuals or organizations to solve problems or to provide services in exchange for payment. Often tele- and remote workers cowork in virtual teams and projects. The prevalence of various types of tele- and remote working vary. Although there are conceptual challenges to operationalizing the concept, it is estimated that hundreds of millions—and possibly more—people today earn their living working at and from their home or other places using digital tools and platforms. In the future, it is expected that new hybrid modes of working will emerge enabled by digital technologies. These changes in working increase the complexity of job demands because of the increased variety of contextual job characteristics. The main benefits of these new ways of working are organizational flexibility and individual autonomy; at the same time, unclear social relations may increase feelings of isolation and challenge the work-life balance.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"22 2 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"128640222","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Attention in Early Development 早期发展中的注意力
Pub Date : 2021-10-29 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.52
S. Conte, J. Richards
Attention is a complex construct that shows development throughout the life span and undergoes significant changes over the first years of life. The complexity of attentional processes is described by the different systems and brain network theorized to describe the construct (i.e., alerting, orienting, executive attention, and sustained attention). Evidence of the development of attention in infancy comes from several behavioral paradigms—primarily focused on the analysis of infants’ eye gaze—physiological measures, and neuroimaging techniques. Many of the changes in attention rely upon the structural and functional development of brain areas involved in attention processes. Behavioral and physiological signs mark the development of attention and are identifiable very early in life. The investigation of the typical development of attention is pivotal for the understanding of atypical trajectories that characterize many neurodevelopmental disorders. The individuation of alterations in early visual attention processes may be utilized to guide intervention programs aimed at improving attention and other cognitive domains.
注意力是一个复杂的结构,在整个生命周期中表现出发展,并在生命的最初几年经历了重大变化。注意过程的复杂性是由不同的系统和大脑网络来描述的(即,警报,定向,执行注意和持续注意)。婴儿期注意力发展的证据来自几个行为范式——主要集中在婴儿凝视的分析、生理测量和神经成像技术。注意力的许多变化依赖于与注意力过程有关的大脑区域的结构和功能发展。行为和生理迹象标志着注意力的发展,在生命的早期就可以识别出来。对典型注意力发展的研究对于理解许多神经发育障碍的非典型发展轨迹至关重要。早期视觉注意过程的个性化改变可以用来指导旨在提高注意力和其他认知领域的干预计划。
{"title":"Attention in Early Development","authors":"S. Conte, J. Richards","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.52","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.52","url":null,"abstract":"Attention is a complex construct that shows development throughout the life span and undergoes significant changes over the first years of life. The complexity of attentional processes is described by the different systems and brain network theorized to describe the construct (i.e., alerting, orienting, executive attention, and sustained attention).\u0000 Evidence of the development of attention in infancy comes from several behavioral paradigms—primarily focused on the analysis of infants’ eye gaze—physiological measures, and neuroimaging techniques. Many of the changes in attention rely upon the structural and functional development of brain areas involved in attention processes. Behavioral and physiological signs mark the development of attention and are identifiable very early in life.\u0000 The investigation of the typical development of attention is pivotal for the understanding of atypical trajectories that characterize many neurodevelopmental disorders. The individuation of alterations in early visual attention processes may be utilized to guide intervention programs aimed at improving attention and other cognitive domains.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"22 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"114173500","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Gender, Race, and Leadership 性别、种族和领导力
Pub Date : 2021-09-29 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.450
C. Begeny, Cye Wong, Teri A. Kirby, F. Rink
Leaders exist in myriad types of groups. Yet in many of them—including in organizational, political, and educational domains—leadership roles are disproportionately occupied by individuals of certain social categories (e.g., men, white individuals). Speaking to this imbalance in representation, there is a wealth of theory and research indicating that gender and race are key to understanding: (a) who tends to get placed in leadership roles, and (b) what an individual’s experience will be like while in that role or on the path to it. In part, this is because there are commonly held stereotypes that make certain individuals—often those of socially dominant racial and gender groups—seem better suited for leadership. By comparison, individuals of other genders and races are often perceived and evaluated as less suitable and treated as such (e.g., deprived of opportunities to become leaders or develop leadership skills). These stereotypes can also elicit disparate internal states (e.g., stereotype threat, internalized negative self-perceptions) that affect individuals’ likelihood of pursuing or obtaining such roles (e.g., by affecting their motivation or performance). In this way, leadership dynamics are intimately connected to the study of gender and race. Overall, these dynamics involve several psychological processes. This includes myriad forms of gender and racial bias—discrimination in evaluations, pay, hiring, promotions, and in access to role models, mentorship, and support; backlash effects, queen bee effects (self-group distancing), glass cliff effects, motherhood penalties, and fatherhood bonuses. It also involves multiple lines of theorizing—role congruity theory, lack of fit, masculine defaults and ambient belonging, modern sexism, aversive racism, social identity threat, and others. Looking ahead, there are several critical directions for advancing research on gender, race, and leadership. This includes examining leadership processes from a more precise, intersectional lens rather than studying the implications of one’s gender or race in isolation (e.g., by integrating work on intersectionality theory, gendered races, and intersectional invisibility). Future study of these processes will also need to consider other relevant social identities (e.g., reflecting class, religion, age, sexuality, ability and neurodiversity, nationality, and immigration status), along with a more thorough consideration of gender—going beyond the study of (cisgender) men and women to consider how transgender and gender-nonconforming individuals are perceived and treated in leadership roles or on the path to such roles. Additionally, and ultimately, it will be critical to develop effective strategies for addressing the underrepresentation of women, racial and ethnic minorities, and other social groups in leadership. In part this will mean carefully evaluating strategies now being employed (e.g., organizational diversity messages, quotas and affirmative action, me
领导者存在于无数类型的群体中。然而,在其中的许多领域——包括组织、政治和教育领域——领导角色被某些社会类别的个人(例如,男性、白人)不成比例地占据。谈到这种代表性的不平衡,有大量的理论和研究表明,性别和种族是理解的关键:(a)谁倾向于担任领导角色,(b)个人在担任该角色或通往该角色的道路上的经历是什么样的。在某种程度上,这是因为人们普遍持有成见,认为某些人——通常是社会上占主导地位的种族和性别群体——似乎更适合担任领导职务。相比之下,其他性别和种族的人往往被认为和评价为不太合适,并被这样对待(例如,被剥夺了成为领导者或发展领导技能的机会)。这些刻板印象还可以引发不同的内部状态(例如,刻板印象威胁,内化的负面自我认知),影响个人追求或获得这些角色的可能性(例如,通过影响他们的动机或表现)。这样,领导力动态与性别和种族的研究密切相关。总的来说,这些动态涉及几个心理过程。这包括在评估、薪酬、招聘、晋升以及获得榜样、指导和支持方面的各种形式的性别和种族偏见歧视;反冲效应,蜂王效应(自我群体距离),玻璃悬崖效应,母亲惩罚和父亲奖励。它还涉及多种理论——角色一致性理论、缺乏契合、男性默认值和环境归属感、现代性别歧视、厌恶种族主义、社会身份威胁等等。展望未来,有几个关键的方向来推进性别、种族和领导力的研究。这包括从一个更精确的、交叉的角度来审视领导过程,而不是孤立地研究一个人的性别或种族的影响(例如,通过整合交叉理论、性别种族和交叉隐形的工作)。未来对这些过程的研究还需要考虑其他相关的社会身份(例如,反映阶级、宗教、年龄、性别、能力和神经多样性、国籍和移民身份),以及对性别的更彻底的考虑——超越对(顺性)男性和女性的研究,考虑跨性别者和性别不一致的个人在领导角色或通往领导角色的道路上是如何被感知和对待的。此外,最终至关重要的是,必须制定有效战略,解决妇女、种族和少数民族以及其他社会群体在领导层中的代表性不足问题。在某种程度上,这将意味着仔细评估目前正在采用的策略(例如,组织多样性信息、配额和平权行动、指导计划)——包括那些即使不会造成伤害,也可能在很大程度上无效的策略(例如,隐性偏见培训、鼓励女性“向前一步”的运动)。解决领导层缺乏多样性的问题,将是解决更广泛的社会不平等问题的关键一步。
{"title":"Gender, Race, and Leadership","authors":"C. Begeny, Cye Wong, Teri A. Kirby, F. Rink","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.450","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.450","url":null,"abstract":"Leaders exist in myriad types of groups. Yet in many of them—including in organizational, political, and educational domains—leadership roles are disproportionately occupied by individuals of certain social categories (e.g., men, white individuals). Speaking to this imbalance in representation, there is a wealth of theory and research indicating that gender and race are key to understanding: (a) who tends to get placed in leadership roles, and (b) what an individual’s experience will be like while in that role or on the path to it. In part, this is because there are commonly held stereotypes that make certain individuals—often those of socially dominant racial and gender groups—seem better suited for leadership. By comparison, individuals of other genders and races are often perceived and evaluated as less suitable and treated as such (e.g., deprived of opportunities to become leaders or develop leadership skills). These stereotypes can also elicit disparate internal states (e.g., stereotype threat, internalized negative self-perceptions) that affect individuals’ likelihood of pursuing or obtaining such roles (e.g., by affecting their motivation or performance). In this way, leadership dynamics are intimately connected to the study of gender and race. Overall, these dynamics involve several psychological processes. This includes myriad forms of gender and racial bias—discrimination in evaluations, pay, hiring, promotions, and in access to role models, mentorship, and support; backlash effects, queen bee effects (self-group distancing), glass cliff effects, motherhood penalties, and fatherhood bonuses. It also involves multiple lines of theorizing—role congruity theory, lack of fit, masculine defaults and ambient belonging, modern sexism, aversive racism, social identity threat, and others.\u0000 Looking ahead, there are several critical directions for advancing research on gender, race, and leadership. This includes examining leadership processes from a more precise, intersectional lens rather than studying the implications of one’s gender or race in isolation (e.g., by integrating work on intersectionality theory, gendered races, and intersectional invisibility). Future study of these processes will also need to consider other relevant social identities (e.g., reflecting class, religion, age, sexuality, ability and neurodiversity, nationality, and immigration status), along with a more thorough consideration of gender—going beyond the study of (cisgender) men and women to consider how transgender and gender-nonconforming individuals are perceived and treated in leadership roles or on the path to such roles. Additionally, and ultimately, it will be critical to develop effective strategies for addressing the underrepresentation of women, racial and ethnic minorities, and other social groups in leadership. In part this will mean carefully evaluating strategies now being employed (e.g., organizational diversity messages, quotas and affirmative action, me","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"125862180","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Motor Development in Context 情境中的运动发展
Pub Date : 2021-09-29 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.60
Sarah E. Berger, Melissa N. Horger, Aaron DeMasi, Lana B. Karasik
The study of motor development has traditionally focused on the timing and sequence of the acquisition of motor skills, such as sitting, crawling, or walking, over the first years of life. Because motor skills are directly observable, motor development serves as a useful exemplar for general principles of development. Current frameworks emphasize motor development in and as a context, such as how change in motor skill interacts with simultaneous change in other developmental domains, how the acquisition of new motor skills creates new opportunities for learning, and how the context in which motor development occurs shapes the course of development. For example, the onset of new motor skills changes the allocation of attentional resources, the quality of infants’ sleep, and available perceptual information. Reciprocally, contexts such as culturally specific parenting practices and individual differences in everyday experiences impact the timing and trajectory of new motor skills.
运动发展的研究传统上集中在运动技能获得的时间和顺序上,如坐、爬或走,在生命的最初几年。因为运动技能是可以直接观察到的,所以运动发展可以作为发展的一般原则的有用范例。目前的框架强调运动发展是在一个环境中进行的,例如运动技能的变化如何与其他发展领域的同步变化相互作用,新运动技能的习得如何创造新的学习机会,以及运动发展发生的环境如何影响发展过程。例如,新的运动技能的开始改变了注意力资源的分配,婴儿的睡眠质量和可用的感知信息。反过来,特定文化背景下的养育方式和日常经历中的个体差异也会影响新运动技能的形成时间和轨迹。
{"title":"Motor Development in Context","authors":"Sarah E. Berger, Melissa N. Horger, Aaron DeMasi, Lana B. Karasik","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.60","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.60","url":null,"abstract":"The study of motor development has traditionally focused on the timing and sequence of the acquisition of motor skills, such as sitting, crawling, or walking, over the first years of life. Because motor skills are directly observable, motor development serves as a useful exemplar for general principles of development. Current frameworks emphasize motor development in and as a context, such as how change in motor skill interacts with simultaneous change in other developmental domains, how the acquisition of new motor skills creates new opportunities for learning, and how the context in which motor development occurs shapes the course of development. For example, the onset of new motor skills changes the allocation of attentional resources, the quality of infants’ sleep, and available perceptual information. Reciprocally, contexts such as culturally specific parenting practices and individual differences in everyday experiences impact the timing and trajectory of new motor skills.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"224 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"131764316","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Voice Production and Perception 声音制作与感知
Pub Date : 2021-09-29 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.766
Roza G. Kamiloğlu, D. Sauter
The voice is a prime channel of communication in humans and other animals. Voices convey many kinds of information, including physical characteristics like body size and sex, as well as providing cues to the vocalizing individual’s identity and emotional state. Vocalizations are produced by dynamic modifications of the physiological vocal production system. The source-filter theory explains how vocalizations are produced in two stages: (a) the production of a sound source in the larynx, and (b) the filtering of that sound by the vocal tract. This two-stage process largely applies to all primate vocalizations. However, there are some differences between the vocal production apparatus of humans as compared to nonhuman primates, such as the lower position of the larynx and lack of air sacs in humans. Thanks to our flexible vocal apparatus, humans can produce a range of different types of vocalizations, including spoken language, nonverbal vocalizations, whispering, and singing. A comprehensive understanding of vocal communication takes both production and perception of vocalizations into account. Internal processes are expressed in the form of specific acoustic patterns in the producer’s voice. In order to communicate information in vocalizations, those acoustic patterns must be acoustically registered by listeners via auditory perception mechanisms. Both production and perception of vocalizations are affected by psychobiological mechanisms as well as sociocultural factors. Furthermore, vocal production and perception can be impaired by a range of different disorders. Vocal production and hearing disorders, as well as mental disorders including autism spectrum disorder, depression, and schizophrenia, affect vocal communication.
声音是人类和其他动物交流的主要渠道。声音传达了多种信息,包括身体特征,如体型和性别,以及为发声个体的身份和情绪状态提供线索。发声是由生理发声系统的动态变化产生的。声源过滤理论解释了发声是如何分两个阶段产生的:(a)喉部产生声源,(b)声道对声音进行过滤。这两个阶段的过程在很大程度上适用于所有灵长类动物的发声。然而,与非人类灵长类动物相比,人类的发声器官存在一些差异,比如喉部的位置较低,人类没有气囊。由于我们灵活的发声器官,人类可以产生一系列不同类型的发声,包括口语、非语言发声、耳语和唱歌。对发声交流的全面理解需要考虑发声的产生和感知。内部过程以制作人声音中特定的声学模式的形式表达出来。为了在发声中交流信息,这些声音模式必须通过听觉感知机制被听者在声学上注册。发声的产生和感知不仅受到心理生物学机制的影响,还受到社会文化因素的影响。此外,声音的产生和感知可能会受到一系列不同疾病的损害。声音产生和听力障碍,以及包括自闭症谱系障碍、抑郁症和精神分裂症在内的精神障碍,都会影响声音交流。
{"title":"Voice Production and Perception","authors":"Roza G. Kamiloğlu, D. Sauter","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.766","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.766","url":null,"abstract":"The voice is a prime channel of communication in humans and other animals. Voices convey many kinds of information, including physical characteristics like body size and sex, as well as providing cues to the vocalizing individual’s identity and emotional state. Vocalizations are produced by dynamic modifications of the physiological vocal production system. The source-filter theory explains how vocalizations are produced in two stages: (a) the production of a sound source in the larynx, and (b) the filtering of that sound by the vocal tract. This two-stage process largely applies to all primate vocalizations. However, there are some differences between the vocal production apparatus of humans as compared to nonhuman primates, such as the lower position of the larynx and lack of air sacs in humans. Thanks to our flexible vocal apparatus, humans can produce a range of different types of vocalizations, including spoken language, nonverbal vocalizations, whispering, and singing.\u0000 A comprehensive understanding of vocal communication takes both production and perception of vocalizations into account. Internal processes are expressed in the form of specific acoustic patterns in the producer’s voice. In order to communicate information in vocalizations, those acoustic patterns must be acoustically registered by listeners via auditory perception mechanisms. Both production and perception of vocalizations are affected by psychobiological mechanisms as well as sociocultural factors. Furthermore, vocal production and perception can be impaired by a range of different disorders. Vocal production and hearing disorders, as well as mental disorders including autism spectrum disorder, depression, and schizophrenia, affect vocal communication.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"14 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"127854047","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attributing Inferred Causes and Explanations to Behavior 将推断的原因和解释归因于行为
Pub Date : 2021-09-29 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.317
G. Moskowitz, Irmak Olcaysoy Okten, Alexandra Sackett
Behavior is a reflection of the intentions, attitudes, goals, beliefs, and desires of a person. These intra-individual factors are coordinated with what opportunities the situation affords and the perceived constraints placed on the person by their context and the norms of the culture they are in. Further, the intentions, attitudes, goals, beliefs, and desires of a person are often not known to them in any given moment, and because they reside within the mind of that person they are almost always not known to the people who are perceiving that person. To know anything about other people we must observe and identify/classify their behavior and then attribute to the observed behavior inferences and judgments about the internal states of that person serving as the motivating force behind their behavior. This entry explores this process of attribution. Heider described attribution as the process that determines “how one person thinks and feels about another person, how he perceives him and what he does to him, what he expects him to do or think, how he reacts to the actions of the other.” The entry explores the rules that people follow in order to make sense of behavior, and the rational versus non-rational nature of the procedure. Even when highly motivated to think rationally, this process can be biased, and flaws can appear in the attribution process, such as from chronic differences among perceivers due to culture, experience, or personality. How the process would unfold if accurate and purely rational is contrasted with how it unfolds when biased. How we feel, and how we choose to act, are derived from how we make sense of the world. Thus, attribution processes are foundational for understanding how we feel, for establishing expectations, and planning how to act in turn.
行为是一个人的意图、态度、目标、信仰和欲望的反映。这些个人内部因素与环境提供的机会以及他们所处的环境和文化规范对个人施加的感知限制相协调。此外,一个人的意图、态度、目标、信仰和欲望在任何时候都不为人所知,因为它们存在于那个人的思想中,所以几乎总是不为感知那个人的人所知。为了了解别人,我们必须观察并识别/分类他们的行为,然后根据观察到的行为,推断和判断那个人的内部状态,作为他们行为背后的动力。这篇文章探讨了归因的过程。Heider将归因描述为一个过程,这个过程决定了“一个人对另一个人的想法和感觉,他如何看待他,他对他做了什么,他期望他做什么或想什么,他对另一个人的行为有什么反应。”该条目探讨了人们为了理解行为而遵循的规则,以及程序的理性与非理性本质。即使在高度激励理性思考的情况下,这个过程也可能存在偏见,并且在归因过程中可能出现缺陷,例如由于文化,经验或个性导致的感知者之间的长期差异。如果这个过程是准确和纯粹理性的,那么它将如何展开,这与它在有偏见的情况下如何展开形成对比。我们如何感受,我们如何选择行动,都来源于我们如何理解这个世界。因此,归因过程是理解我们的感受、建立期望和计划如何行动的基础。
{"title":"Attributing Inferred Causes and Explanations to Behavior","authors":"G. Moskowitz, Irmak Olcaysoy Okten, Alexandra Sackett","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.317","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.317","url":null,"abstract":"Behavior is a reflection of the intentions, attitudes, goals, beliefs, and desires of a person. These intra-individual factors are coordinated with what opportunities the situation affords and the perceived constraints placed on the person by their context and the norms of the culture they are in. Further, the intentions, attitudes, goals, beliefs, and desires of a person are often not known to them in any given moment, and because they reside within the mind of that person they are almost always not known to the people who are perceiving that person. To know anything about other people we must observe and identify/classify their behavior and then attribute to the observed behavior inferences and judgments about the internal states of that person serving as the motivating force behind their behavior. This entry explores this process of attribution. Heider described attribution as the process that determines “how one person thinks and feels about another person, how he perceives him and what he does to him, what he expects him to do or think, how he reacts to the actions of the other.” The entry explores the rules that people follow in order to make sense of behavior, and the rational versus non-rational nature of the procedure. Even when highly motivated to think rationally, this process can be biased, and flaws can appear in the attribution process, such as from chronic differences among perceivers due to culture, experience, or personality. How the process would unfold if accurate and purely rational is contrasted with how it unfolds when biased. How we feel, and how we choose to act, are derived from how we make sense of the world. Thus, attribution processes are foundational for understanding how we feel, for establishing expectations, and planning how to act in turn.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"128020765","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1