首页 > 最新文献

Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Social Cognition 社会认知
Pub Date : 2020-08-27 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.234
K. G. Ratner
Contemporary models of how the mind operates and methods for testing them emerged from the cognitive revolution in the middle of the 20th century. Social psychology researchers of the 1970s and 1980s were inspired by these developments and launched the field of social cognition to understand how cognitive approaches could advance understanding of social processes. Decades later, core social psychology topics, such as impression formation, the self, attitudes, stereotyping and prejudice, and interpersonal relationships, are interpreted through the lens of cognitive psychology conceptualizations of attention, perception, categorization, memory, and reasoning. Social cognitive methods and theory have touched every area of modern social psychology. Twenty-first-century efforts are shoring up methodological practices and revisiting old theories, investigating a wider range of human experience, and tackling new avenues of social functioning.
心智如何运作的当代模型和测试这些模型的方法是从20世纪中期的认知革命中出现的。20世纪70年代和80年代的社会心理学研究人员受到这些发展的启发,开创了社会认知领域,以了解认知方法如何促进对社会过程的理解。几十年后,核心的社会心理学主题,如印象形成、自我、态度、刻板印象和偏见以及人际关系,通过认知心理学的注意、感知、分类、记忆和推理的概念化来解释。社会认知方法和理论已触及现代社会心理学的各个领域。21世纪的努力正在支持方法论实践,重新审视旧理论,调查更广泛的人类经验,并解决社会功能的新途径。
{"title":"Social Cognition","authors":"K. G. Ratner","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.234","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.234","url":null,"abstract":"Contemporary models of how the mind operates and methods for testing them emerged from the cognitive revolution in the middle of the 20th century. Social psychology researchers of the 1970s and 1980s were inspired by these developments and launched the field of social cognition to understand how cognitive approaches could advance understanding of social processes. Decades later, core social psychology topics, such as impression formation, the self, attitudes, stereotyping and prejudice, and interpersonal relationships, are interpreted through the lens of cognitive psychology conceptualizations of attention, perception, categorization, memory, and reasoning. Social cognitive methods and theory have touched every area of modern social psychology. Twenty-first-century efforts are shoring up methodological practices and revisiting old theories, investigating a wider range of human experience, and tackling new avenues of social functioning.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"175 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-08-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"126942638","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attitudes and Behavior 态度和行为
Pub Date : 2020-08-27 DOI: 10.1097/00006199-197505000-00023
G. Haddock, Sapphira R. Thorne, Lukas J. Wolf
Attitudes refer to overall evaluations of people, groups, ideas, and other objects, reflecting whether individuals like or dislike them. Attitudes have been found to be good predictors of behavior, with generally medium-sized effects. The role of attitudes in guiding behavior may be the primary reason why people’s social lives often revolve around expressing and discussing their attitudes, and why social psychology researchers have spent decades examining attitudes. Two central questions in the study of attitudes concern when and how attitudes predict behavior. The “when” question has been addressed over decades of research that has identified circumstances under which attitudes are more or less likely to predict behavior. That is, attitudes are stronger predictors of behaviors when both constructs are assessed in a corresponding or matching way, when attitudes are stronger, and among certain individuals and in certain situations and domains. The “how” question concerns influential models in the attitudes literature that provide a better understanding of the processes through which attitudes are linked with behaviors. For instance, these models indicate that other constructs need to be taken into account in understanding the attitude-behavior link, including intentions to perform a behavior, whether individuals perceive themselves to be in control of their behavior, and what they believe others around them think the individual should do (i.e., norms). The models also describe whether attitudes relate to behavior through relatively deliberative and controlled processes or relatively automatic and spontaneous processes. Overall, the long history of research on attitude-behavior links has provided a clearer prediction of when attitudes are linked with behaviors and a better understanding of the processes underlying this link.
态度是指对人、群体、想法和其他事物的总体评价,反映了个人对它们的喜欢或不喜欢。人们发现,态度可以很好地预测行为,通常影响中等。态度在指导行为中的作用可能是人们的社会生活经常围绕着表达和讨论他们的态度的主要原因,也是社会心理学研究者花了几十年时间研究态度的主要原因。态度研究中的两个核心问题是态度何时以及如何预测行为。“什么时候”这个问题已经在几十年的研究中得到了解决,这些研究已经确定了态度或多或少可能预测行为的情况。也就是说,当两个构念以对应或匹配的方式被评估时,当态度更强时,在特定的个体中,在特定的情境和领域中,态度是更强的行为预测因子。“如何”问题涉及态度文献中有影响力的模型,这些模型提供了对态度与行为相联系的过程的更好理解。例如,这些模型表明,在理解态度-行为联系时,需要考虑其他构念,包括执行行为的意图,个人是否认为自己控制自己的行为,以及他们认为周围的人认为个人应该做什么(即规范)。这些模型还描述了态度与行为是通过相对审慎和受控的过程还是相对自动和自发的过程联系起来的。总的来说,对态度-行为联系的长期研究已经为态度何时与行为联系提供了更清晰的预测,并更好地理解了这种联系背后的过程。
{"title":"Attitudes and Behavior","authors":"G. Haddock, Sapphira R. Thorne, Lukas J. Wolf","doi":"10.1097/00006199-197505000-00023","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1097/00006199-197505000-00023","url":null,"abstract":"Attitudes refer to overall evaluations of people, groups, ideas, and other objects, reflecting whether individuals like or dislike them. Attitudes have been found to be good predictors of behavior, with generally medium-sized effects. The role of attitudes in guiding behavior may be the primary reason why people’s social lives often revolve around expressing and discussing their attitudes, and why social psychology researchers have spent decades examining attitudes.\u0000 Two central questions in the study of attitudes concern when and how attitudes predict behavior. The “when” question has been addressed over decades of research that has identified circumstances under which attitudes are more or less likely to predict behavior. That is, attitudes are stronger predictors of behaviors when both constructs are assessed in a corresponding or matching way, when attitudes are stronger, and among certain individuals and in certain situations and domains.\u0000 The “how” question concerns influential models in the attitudes literature that provide a better understanding of the processes through which attitudes are linked with behaviors. For instance, these models indicate that other constructs need to be taken into account in understanding the attitude-behavior link, including intentions to perform a behavior, whether individuals perceive themselves to be in control of their behavior, and what they believe others around them think the individual should do (i.e., norms). The models also describe whether attitudes relate to behavior through relatively deliberative and controlled processes or relatively automatic and spontaneous processes. Overall, the long history of research on attitude-behavior links has provided a clearer prediction of when attitudes are linked with behaviors and a better understanding of the processes underlying this link.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"123 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-08-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"122877876","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bystander Apathy 旁观者的冷漠
Pub Date : 2020-08-27 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.808
S. Nida
The brutal 1964 murder of Kitty Genovese sparked widespread public interest, primarily because it was reported to have taken place in view of some 38 witnesses, most of whom had seen the incident through the windows of their apartments in a high-rise building directly across the street. (Investigative work conducted some 50 years later suggests that there were not that many actual witnesses—more likely as few as seven or eight.) The ensuing analyses provided by newspaper columnists and others tended to focus on the callous indifference that had been demonstrated by those who had chosen not to intervene in the emergency, a state of affairs that came to be known, at least for a while, as “bystander apathy.” (It soon became clear, however, that bystanders in such events are rarely apathetic or indifferent.) Intrigued by the internal and interpersonal dynamics that might be involved, two social psychologists, Bibb Latané and John Darley, began a program of research that led to the conclusion that any notion of “safety in numbers” is illusory. In fact, it is the very presence of other people that may discourage helping in such circumstances. More specifically, other unresponsive bystanders may provide cues suggesting that the event is not serious and that inaction is the appropriate response. In addition, knowing that others are available to help allows the individual bystander to shift some of the responsibility for intervening to the others present, a process that Latané and Darley termed “diffusion of responsibility”; that is, the greater the number of others present, the easier it is for any one individual to assume that someone else will help. Subsequent research has demonstrated that this tendency for the individual to be less likely to help when part of a group than when alone—now known as the “bystander effect”—is a remarkably robust phenomenon. Even though social psychology has developed a thorough understanding of the mechanisms that drive this phenomenon, applying this knowledge is difficult, and significant incidents involving the bystander effect continue to occur.
1964年基蒂·吉诺维斯(Kitty Genovese)的残忍谋杀案引起了广泛的公众兴趣,主要是因为据报道,案发时约有38名目击者,其中大多数人是透过街对面一栋高层建筑公寓的窗户看到这一事件的。(大约50年后进行的调查工作表明,实际的目击者并没有那么多——更有可能只有七八个。)报纸专栏作家和其他人随后提供的分析倾向于关注那些选择不干预紧急情况的人所表现出的无情冷漠,这种事态至少在一段时间内被称为“旁观者冷漠”。(然而,很快就清楚了,这类事件中的旁观者很少是冷漠或漠不关心的。)两位社会心理学家Bibb latan和John Darley被可能涉及的内部和人际动态所吸引,他们开始了一个研究项目,得出了这样的结论:任何“人多安全”的概念都是虚幻的。事实上,在这种情况下,其他人的存在可能会阻碍帮助。更具体地说,其他没有反应的旁观者可能会提供暗示,表明事件并不严重,不作为是适当的反应。此外,知道其他人可以提供帮助,使得个体旁观者可以将干预的部分责任转移到在场的其他人身上,这一过程被latan和Darley称为“责任扩散”;也就是说,在场的人越多,任何一个人就越容易认为别人会帮忙。随后的研究表明,与独自一人相比,个人在群体中更不愿意提供帮助的倾向——现在被称为“旁观者效应”——是一种非常强大的现象。尽管社会心理学已经对导致这一现象的机制有了透彻的理解,但应用这些知识是困难的,涉及旁观者效应的重大事件仍在不断发生。
{"title":"Bystander Apathy","authors":"S. Nida","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.808","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.808","url":null,"abstract":"The brutal 1964 murder of Kitty Genovese sparked widespread public interest, primarily because it was reported to have taken place in view of some 38 witnesses, most of whom had seen the incident through the windows of their apartments in a high-rise building directly across the street. (Investigative work conducted some 50 years later suggests that there were not that many actual witnesses—more likely as few as seven or eight.) The ensuing analyses provided by newspaper columnists and others tended to focus on the callous indifference that had been demonstrated by those who had chosen not to intervene in the emergency, a state of affairs that came to be known, at least for a while, as “bystander apathy.” (It soon became clear, however, that bystanders in such events are rarely apathetic or indifferent.) Intrigued by the internal and interpersonal dynamics that might be involved, two social psychologists, Bibb Latané and John Darley, began a program of research that led to the conclusion that any notion of “safety in numbers” is illusory. In fact, it is the very presence of other people that may discourage helping in such circumstances. More specifically, other unresponsive bystanders may provide cues suggesting that the event is not serious and that inaction is the appropriate response. In addition, knowing that others are available to help allows the individual bystander to shift some of the responsibility for intervening to the others present, a process that Latané and Darley termed “diffusion of responsibility”; that is, the greater the number of others present, the easier it is for any one individual to assume that someone else will help. Subsequent research has demonstrated that this tendency for the individual to be less likely to help when part of a group than when alone—now known as the “bystander effect”—is a remarkably robust phenomenon. Even though social psychology has developed a thorough understanding of the mechanisms that drive this phenomenon, applying this knowledge is difficult, and significant incidents involving the bystander effect continue to occur.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"29 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-08-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"115050529","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 67
Gordon Allport Gordon Allport
Pub Date : 2020-08-27 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.829
R. Fancher
Gordon W. Allport was a prominent Harvard University psychologist during the mid-20th century, notable both for his early and effective promotion of “personality” as an important psychological subdiscipline, and in his later career as a social psychologist for works on several issues of major social importance. In 1921 he and his older brother Floyd Allport jointly proposed the study and measurement of traits as the foundation of a new subdiscipline of personality psychology, with Gordon’s Harvard doctoral research a pilot study demonstrating the feasibility of the approach. On a subsequent postdoctoral fellowship in Germany Allport became impressed by William Stern’s “personalistic” psychology, which held that a person’s “individuality” could be defined in two ways: relational individuality, comprised of the particular combination of numerous measurable traits manifested by a subject in studies such as Allport’s thesis; and real individuality, a Gestalt-like conception of a personality that is more than just the sum of its parts, and discoverable only through a qualitative analysis of the traits’ role in an overall life history. These ideas inspired in Allport a conception of personality as a broad and independent psychological field that would incorporate both the “nomothetic,” experimental methods of the natural sciences in measuring and studying personality traits, and the non-experimental “idiographic” methods utilized in the historical and humanistic fields for providing conceptions of wholly integrated, unique personalities. Noting that Anglo-American psychology was heavily dominated by the former approach, he became an outspoken advocate of the latter as a necessary complement to it. Allport taught undergraduate seminars promoting this conception at Harvard and Dartmouth between 1924 and 1930, before returning permanently to Harvard in 1930. There, both independently and in collaborations with others, he conducted and promoted seminal personality research employing both nomothetic and idiographic methods. His comprehensive and authoritative 1937 textbook, Personality: A Psychological Interpretation, was a landmark in establishing personality as a major psychological discipline. With enhanced reputation, Allport became a leading institutional figure in American psychology. For the rest of his career he continued to advocate an inclusive, “eclectic” approach to personality psychology, while also turning attention to important social issues such as wartime morale and propaganda, the influence of radio as a mass medium, the role of religion in personality and society, and with particular impact the nature of prejudice.
戈登·w·奥尔波特(Gordon W. Allport)是20世纪中期哈佛大学一位杰出的心理学家,他早期有效地将“人格”作为一个重要的心理学分支学科,以及在他后来作为社会心理学家的职业生涯中,对几个重要的社会问题进行了研究。1921年,他和他的哥哥弗洛伊德·奥尔波特(Floyd Allport)共同提出了对性格特征的研究和测量,作为人格心理学新分支学科的基础,戈登在哈佛大学的博士研究是一项初步研究,证明了这种方法的可行性。在随后的德国博士后研究中,奥尔波特对威廉·斯特恩(William Stern)的“个人主义”心理学印象深刻,该心理学认为一个人的“个性”可以用两种方式定义:关系个性,由奥尔波特的论文等研究对象所表现出的许多可测量特征的特定组合组成;真正的个性是一种格式塔式的人格概念,它不仅仅是各部分的总和,而且只有通过对整个生活史中特征角色的定性分析才能发现。这些想法启发了奥尔波特将人格作为一个广泛而独立的心理学领域的概念,该领域将结合自然科学中测量和研究人格特征的“本体”实验方法,以及历史和人文领域中用于提供完全整合的、独特的人格概念的非实验“具体”方法。注意到英美心理学在很大程度上被前一种方法所主导,他成为了后一种方法的直言不讳的倡导者,作为它的必要补充。1924年至1930年间,奥尔波特在哈佛和达特茅斯教授本科生研讨会,推广这一概念,1930年永久返回哈佛。在那里,无论是独立还是与他人合作,他都采用命名法和具体方法进行并促进了开创性的人格研究。他1937年出版的综合性、权威性的教科书《人格:一种心理学解释》是将人格确立为一门主要心理学学科的里程碑。随着声誉的提高,奥尔波特成为美国心理学学界的领军人物。在他职业生涯的剩余时间里,他继续倡导一种包容的、“折衷的”人格心理学方法,同时也将注意力转向重要的社会问题,如战时士气和宣传、广播作为大众媒介的影响、宗教在人格和社会中的作用,以及对偏见的本质产生特别影响。
{"title":"Gordon Allport","authors":"R. Fancher","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.829","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.829","url":null,"abstract":"Gordon W. Allport was a prominent Harvard University psychologist during the mid-20th century, notable both for his early and effective promotion of “personality” as an important psychological subdiscipline, and in his later career as a social psychologist for works on several issues of major social importance. In 1921 he and his older brother Floyd Allport jointly proposed the study and measurement of traits as the foundation of a new subdiscipline of personality psychology, with Gordon’s Harvard doctoral research a pilot study demonstrating the feasibility of the approach. On a subsequent postdoctoral fellowship in Germany Allport became impressed by William Stern’s “personalistic” psychology, which held that a person’s “individuality” could be defined in two ways: relational individuality, comprised of the particular combination of numerous measurable traits manifested by a subject in studies such as Allport’s thesis; and real individuality, a Gestalt-like conception of a personality that is more than just the sum of its parts, and discoverable only through a qualitative analysis of the traits’ role in an overall life history. These ideas inspired in Allport a conception of personality as a broad and independent psychological field that would incorporate both the “nomothetic,” experimental methods of the natural sciences in measuring and studying personality traits, and the non-experimental “idiographic” methods utilized in the historical and humanistic fields for providing conceptions of wholly integrated, unique personalities. Noting that Anglo-American psychology was heavily dominated by the former approach, he became an outspoken advocate of the latter as a necessary complement to it.\u0000 Allport taught undergraduate seminars promoting this conception at Harvard and Dartmouth between 1924 and 1930, before returning permanently to Harvard in 1930. There, both independently and in collaborations with others, he conducted and promoted seminal personality research employing both nomothetic and idiographic methods. His comprehensive and authoritative 1937 textbook, Personality: A Psychological Interpretation, was a landmark in establishing personality as a major psychological discipline. With enhanced reputation, Allport became a leading institutional figure in American psychology. For the rest of his career he continued to advocate an inclusive, “eclectic” approach to personality psychology, while also turning attention to important social issues such as wartime morale and propaganda, the influence of radio as a mass medium, the role of religion in personality and society, and with particular impact the nature of prejudice.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"23 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-08-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"127700286","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Qualitative Inquiry 定性调查
Pub Date : 2020-07-30 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.485
J. Marecek, E. Magnusson
Qualitative inquiry is a form of psychological research that seeks in-depth understanding of people and their social worlds. Qualitative researchers typically study the experiences of people as meaning-making agents, relying on verbal material. Qualitative inquiry has a long history in psychology, beginning in the 19th century with founders of psychology like William James and Wilhelm Wundt. However, for much of the 20th century, qualitative inquiry has occupied a marginal position in the discipline. This marginalization is best understood in relation to the discipline’s early struggle to be regarded as legitimate. Adopting the methods of the natural sciences—notably quantification and measurement—was a means to that end. Qualitative approaches, though suppressed for much of the 20th century, were not entirely eliminated from the field. Personality theorists, for example, continued to make use of them. The 1970s marked the advent of new forms of qualitative inquiry in psychology, which drew from a variety of intellectual and philosophical movements. These developments continued to gain acceptance and adherents. Since the turn of the 20th century, national and international organizations of qualitative researchers in psychology have been established. Venues for publishing qualitative research in psychology have increased. Nonetheless, qualitative inquiry is still marginalized in many academic psychology departments, and training in qualitative methods is seldom part of the methods curriculum.
定性调查是一种寻求深入了解人和他们的社会世界的心理研究形式。定性研究人员通常将人们的经历作为意义制造主体进行研究,依靠语言材料。定性调查在心理学中有着悠久的历史,从19世纪威廉·詹姆斯和威廉·冯特等心理学创始人开始。然而,在20世纪的大部分时间里,定性研究在这门学科中占据了边缘地位。这种边缘化最好的理解是与该学科早期被视为合法的斗争有关。采用自然科学的方法——尤其是量化和测量——是实现这一目标的一种手段。定性方法虽然在20世纪的大部分时间里受到压制,但并没有完全从这个领域中消失。例如,人格理论家继续使用它们。20世纪70年代标志着心理学新形式的定性研究的出现,这些研究来自于各种各样的智力和哲学运动。这些发展继续获得认可和追随者。自20世纪初以来,国内和国际的心理学定性研究组织相继成立。发表心理学定性研究的场所增加了。尽管如此,定性研究在许多学术心理部门仍然被边缘化,并且定性方法的培训很少是方法课程的一部分。
{"title":"Qualitative Inquiry","authors":"J. Marecek, E. Magnusson","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.485","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.485","url":null,"abstract":"Qualitative inquiry is a form of psychological research that seeks in-depth understanding of people and their social worlds. Qualitative researchers typically study the experiences of people as meaning-making agents, relying on verbal material. Qualitative inquiry has a long history in psychology, beginning in the 19th century with founders of psychology like William James and Wilhelm Wundt. However, for much of the 20th century, qualitative inquiry has occupied a marginal position in the discipline. This marginalization is best understood in relation to the discipline’s early struggle to be regarded as legitimate. Adopting the methods of the natural sciences—notably quantification and measurement—was a means to that end. Qualitative approaches, though suppressed for much of the 20th century, were not entirely eliminated from the field. Personality theorists, for example, continued to make use of them.\u0000 The 1970s marked the advent of new forms of qualitative inquiry in psychology, which drew from a variety of intellectual and philosophical movements. These developments continued to gain acceptance and adherents. Since the turn of the 20th century, national and international organizations of qualitative researchers in psychology have been established. Venues for publishing qualitative research in psychology have increased. Nonetheless, qualitative inquiry is still marginalized in many academic psychology departments, and training in qualitative methods is seldom part of the methods curriculum.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"131105830","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Culture and Social Development 文化与社会发展
Pub Date : 2020-07-30 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.592
H. Keller
Humans need other people to survive and thrive. Therefore, relatedness is a basic human need. However, relatedness can be conceived of very differently in different cultural environments, depending on the affordances and constraints of the particular context. Specifically, the level of formal education and, relatedly, the age of the mother at first birth, the number of children, and the household composition have proven to be contextual dimensions that are informative for norms and values, including the conception of relatedness. Higher formal education, late parenthood, few children, and a nuclear family drive relationships as emotional constructs between independent and self-contained individuals as adaptive in Western middle-class families. The perspective of the individual is primary and is organized by psychological autonomy. Lower formal education, early parenthood, with many children, and large multigenerational households, drive the conception of relationships as role-based networks of obligations that are adapted to non-Western rural farm life. The perspective of the social system is primary and organized by hierarchical relatedness. Social development as developmental science in general, represented in textbooks and handbooks, is based on the Western middle-class view of the independent individual. Accordingly, developmental milestones are rooted in the separation of the individual from the social environment. The traditional rural farmer child’s development is grounded in cultural emphases of communality which stress other developmental priorities than the Western view. Cross-cultural research is mainly interpreted against the Western standard as the normal case, but serious ethical challenges are involved in this practice. The consequence is that textbooks need to be rewritten to include multiple cultural perspectives with multiple developmental pathways.
人类需要他人来生存和发展。因此,亲缘关系是人类的基本需求。然而,在不同的文化环境中,亲缘关系可以有非常不同的理解,这取决于特定语境的支持和约束。具体地说,正规教育的水平以及与之相关的母亲初次生育的年龄、子女的数目和家庭组成已被证明是对规范和价值观,包括关系概念提供信息的背景方面。高等的正规教育,晚育,少生孩子,以及核心家庭将关系作为独立和自给自足的个体之间的情感结构来适应西方中产阶级家庭。个体的视角是首要的,是由心理自主性组织起来的。较低的正规教育水平、较早为人父母、子女众多以及多代同堂的大家庭,推动了以角色为基础的义务网络关系的概念,这种关系适应了非西方农村农场生活。社会系统的视角是初级的,由等级关系组织。在教科书和手册中,社会发展作为一般的发展科学,是以西方中产阶级对独立个体的看法为基础的。因此,发展的里程碑植根于个人与社会环境的分离。传统农村农民儿童的发展是建立在强调共同体的文化基础上的,这种文化强调的是与西方观点不同的其他发展优先事项。跨文化研究主要以西方标准为标准进行解释,但这种做法涉及到严重的伦理挑战。其结果是,教科书需要重写,以包括多种文化视角和多种发展途径。
{"title":"Culture and Social Development","authors":"H. Keller","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.592","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.592","url":null,"abstract":"Humans need other people to survive and thrive. Therefore, relatedness is a basic human need. However, relatedness can be conceived of very differently in different cultural environments, depending on the affordances and constraints of the particular context. Specifically, the level of formal education and, relatedly, the age of the mother at first birth, the number of children, and the household composition have proven to be contextual dimensions that are informative for norms and values, including the conception of relatedness. Higher formal education, late parenthood, few children, and a nuclear family drive relationships as emotional constructs between independent and self-contained individuals as adaptive in Western middle-class families. The perspective of the individual is primary and is organized by psychological autonomy. Lower formal education, early parenthood, with many children, and large multigenerational households, drive the conception of relationships as role-based networks of obligations that are adapted to non-Western rural farm life. The perspective of the social system is primary and organized by hierarchical relatedness.\u0000 Social development as developmental science in general, represented in textbooks and handbooks, is based on the Western middle-class view of the independent individual. Accordingly, developmental milestones are rooted in the separation of the individual from the social environment. The traditional rural farmer child’s development is grounded in cultural emphases of communality which stress other developmental priorities than the Western view. Cross-cultural research is mainly interpreted against the Western standard as the normal case, but serious ethical challenges are involved in this practice. The consequence is that textbooks need to be rewritten to include multiple cultural perspectives with multiple developmental pathways.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"133234458","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
LGBTQ Psychology 同性恋群体心理学
Pub Date : 2020-07-30 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.666
P. Hegarty
LGBTQ (Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, Transgender, and Queer) psychology is a loosely organized subfield of psychology. The field emerged, principally in the United States, in the late 1960s in concert with the de-pathologization of adult homosexuality in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Over the decade of the 1970s, psychologists stopped researching adult lesbians and gay men as a psychiatric category and initiated new research on relationships, parenting, and the prejudice experienced by this stigmatized group. The HIV/AIDS epidemic lead this subfield to grow rapidly, to focus on men, to gain far wider engagement from mainstream psychologists, and to make health outcomes central to LGBTQ psychology’s raison d’etre. The 1990s were described as a period of “coming of age” as the field began to address bisexuality more directly, to internationalize, and to become more central to strategies in the United States to use psychological evidence to support the civil rights of minorities in court cases. The development of transgender-affirmative psychologies, a literature on the particular psychological issues of LGBTQ people of color in the United States, and an emphasis on the rights of same-gender couples to legal recognition of their relationships were new and prominent themes in the 21st-century literature. This subfield of psychology has been characterized by its historical emergence in the United States, a relative lack of attention to children, an urge to affirm under-represented groups by researching them, and a frustration that descriptive research does not always bring about the desired social transformations that motivate it.
LGBTQ(女同性恋、男同性恋、双性恋、变性人和酷儿)心理学是心理学的一个松散分支。20世纪60年代末,随着《精神疾病诊断与统计手册》(Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders)对成年同性恋的去病理性化,这一领域主要出现在美国。在20世纪70年代的十年里,心理学家们不再把成年男女同性恋者作为一个精神病学的范畴来研究,而是开始对这一被污名化的群体所经历的关系、养育和偏见进行新的研究。艾滋病毒/艾滋病的流行导致这一分支领域迅速发展,关注男性,获得主流心理学家更广泛的参与,并使健康结果成为LGBTQ心理学存在的核心理由。20世纪90年代被描述为一个“成熟”的时期,因为该领域开始更直接地处理双性恋问题,国际化,并成为美国使用心理学证据在法庭案件中支持少数民族公民权利的战略的核心。跨性别平权心理学的发展,关于美国有色人种LGBTQ群体特殊心理问题的文献,以及对同性伴侣关系获得法律承认的权利的强调,是21世纪文学中新的突出主题。这一心理学分支的特点是,它在美国历史上出现,相对缺乏对儿童的关注,急于通过研究来肯定代表性不足的群体,以及对描述性研究并不总能带来预期的社会变革的沮丧。
{"title":"LGBTQ Psychology","authors":"P. Hegarty","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.666","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.666","url":null,"abstract":"LGBTQ (Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, Transgender, and Queer) psychology is a loosely organized subfield of psychology. The field emerged, principally in the United States, in the late 1960s in concert with the de-pathologization of adult homosexuality in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Over the decade of the 1970s, psychologists stopped researching adult lesbians and gay men as a psychiatric category and initiated new research on relationships, parenting, and the prejudice experienced by this stigmatized group. The HIV/AIDS epidemic lead this subfield to grow rapidly, to focus on men, to gain far wider engagement from mainstream psychologists, and to make health outcomes central to LGBTQ psychology’s raison d’etre. The 1990s were described as a period of “coming of age” as the field began to address bisexuality more directly, to internationalize, and to become more central to strategies in the United States to use psychological evidence to support the civil rights of minorities in court cases. The development of transgender-affirmative psychologies, a literature on the particular psychological issues of LGBTQ people of color in the United States, and an emphasis on the rights of same-gender couples to legal recognition of their relationships were new and prominent themes in the 21st-century literature. This subfield of psychology has been characterized by its historical emergence in the United States, a relative lack of attention to children, an urge to affirm under-represented groups by researching them, and a frustration that descriptive research does not always bring about the desired social transformations that motivate it.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"74 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"117321054","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Psychoanalysis and Critical Theory 精神分析与批判理论
Pub Date : 2020-07-30 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.682
G. Jovanović
The relationship between psychoanalysis and Critical Theory (the Frankfurt School), contrary to dominant interpretations, is examined from a sociocultural perspective. Psychoanalysis addressed the sociopolitical issues of its time, including cultural shifts, war, and the cultural conditio humana in general. Beyond that, and more importantly, it is argued that the core psychoanalytic concepts, including drive itself, can be understood as a structure open to social co-construction. Such an interpretation of psychoanalysis can provide a link to Critical Theory of society. First, both sociopolitical and theoretical conditions in the 1920s and 1930s merit analysis under which members of the Frankfurt Institute for Social Research referred to Freud’s psychoanalysis. A theory was needed that would examine a missing point in Marxist interpretations, which the Institute adopted as its political and theoretical framework. What was missing was a place for subjective mediating factors, especially important among which were those generated by drives and those that operated unconsciously. The views on psychoanalysis and its role in the first generation of Critical Theory are analyzed, particularly the views of Horkheimer, Adorno, Fromm, and, most extensively, Marcuse, given the fact that Freud’s psychoanalysis had a central role in his thought. Finally, questions regarding the contemporary relevance of psychoanalysis and Critical Theory under new sociocultural conditions in the 21st century are raised.
精神分析和批判理论(法兰克福学派)之间的关系,与主流解释相反,是从社会文化的角度来考察的。精神分析解决了当时的社会政治问题,包括文化转变、战争和一般的人类文化状况。除此之外,更重要的是,作者认为精神分析的核心概念,包括驱动力本身,可以被理解为一种对社会共同构建开放的结构。这种对精神分析的解释可以提供与社会批判理论的联系。首先,20世纪20年代和30年代的社会政治和理论条件都值得分析,法兰克福社会研究所的成员将其称为弗洛伊德的精神分析。需要一种理论来检查马克思主义解释中缺失的一点,研究所将其作为其政治和理论框架。缺少的是主观调节因素的位置,其中特别重要的是那些由驱动产生的和无意识操作的因素。本文分析了关于精神分析的观点及其在第一代批判理论中的作用,特别是霍克海默、阿多诺、弗洛姆的观点,以及最广泛的马尔库塞的观点,因为弗洛伊德的精神分析在他的思想中起着核心作用。最后,提出了在21世纪新社会文化条件下精神分析和批判理论的当代相关性问题。
{"title":"Psychoanalysis and Critical Theory","authors":"G. Jovanović","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.682","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.682","url":null,"abstract":"The relationship between psychoanalysis and Critical Theory (the Frankfurt School), contrary to dominant interpretations, is examined from a sociocultural perspective. Psychoanalysis addressed the sociopolitical issues of its time, including cultural shifts, war, and the cultural conditio humana in general. Beyond that, and more importantly, it is argued that the core psychoanalytic concepts, including drive itself, can be understood as a structure open to social co-construction. Such an interpretation of psychoanalysis can provide a link to Critical Theory of society. First, both sociopolitical and theoretical conditions in the 1920s and 1930s merit analysis under which members of the Frankfurt Institute for Social Research referred to Freud’s psychoanalysis. A theory was needed that would examine a missing point in Marxist interpretations, which the Institute adopted as its political and theoretical framework. What was missing was a place for subjective mediating factors, especially important among which were those generated by drives and those that operated unconsciously. The views on psychoanalysis and its role in the first generation of Critical Theory are analyzed, particularly the views of Horkheimer, Adorno, Fromm, and, most extensively, Marcuse, given the fact that Freud’s psychoanalysis had a central role in his thought. Finally, questions regarding the contemporary relevance of psychoanalysis and Critical Theory under new sociocultural conditions in the 21st century are raised.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"37 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"114913349","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Organizational Justice 组织公平
Pub Date : 2020-07-30 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.555
D. D. Steiner
Organizational justice refers to people’s perceptions of the fairness or unfairness of the treatment they receive in the organizations where they work. The ways authorities, such as supervisors and managers, make decisions and implement them are evaluated by employees in terms of their fairness. Other agents, such as coworkers and customers who interact with employees, also can generate judgments of fairness or unfairness at work. These fairness perceptions can be conceived according to four dimensions of organizational justice as well as in general terms. The four dimensions are distributive, procedural, interpersonal, and informational. Typically, distributive justice evaluates the equity of treatment, where people expect outcomes proportionate to their contributions. Workers also evaluate the fairness of procedures used to make decisions and the quality of their interpersonal relations with the various actors of the organization, including the information the actors communicate regarding decisions and the procedures followed to make them. When people perceive that they are treated fairly, positive consequences result for them and for their organizations. Thus, they tend to be more satisfied, evaluate their management more favorably, engage in more prosocial behaviors within their organizations, perform at higher levels, and remain in their employing organizations for longer periods. When people experience unfair treatment, negative consequences include stress and health-related concerns for employees, negative attitudes toward the organization, and counterproductive behaviors, such as theft, vandalism, or absenteeism. People react strongly to fair or unfair treatment for different reasons. They may believe that fair treatment will allow them to receive the rewards that they deserve, it may communicate that they are valued in a group, or fair treatment may be valued as an important and basic principle of human functioning. Research on organizational justice in 2020 focuses on understanding the mechanisms producing fairness judgments and their consequences and on the boundary conditions limiting the observed relations with their antecedents and outcomes.
组织公正是指人们对自己在工作的组织中所受到的待遇的公平或不公平的看法。主管和经理等权威人士制定决策和执行决策的方式由员工根据其公平性进行评估。其他代理人,如与员工互动的同事和客户,也会对工作中的公平或不公平做出判断。这些公平感知可以根据组织公正的四个维度以及一般术语来构想。这四个维度分别是分配性、程序性、人际性和信息性。通常,分配正义评估的是待遇的公平性,即人们期望的结果与他们的贡献成比例。员工还会评估决策程序的公平性,以及他们与组织中各个参与者之间的人际关系质量,包括参与者就决策传达的信息以及制定决策所遵循的程序。当人们意识到自己受到公平对待时,对他们自己和他们的组织都会产生积极的影响。因此,他们倾向于更满意,更有利地评价他们的管理,在他们的组织中参与更多的亲社会行为,在更高的层次上表现,并在他们的雇佣组织中呆更长的时间。当人们遭受不公平待遇时,负面后果包括员工的压力和健康问题,对组织的消极态度,以及适得其反的行为,如盗窃,破坏公物或旷工。由于不同的原因,人们对公平或不公平的待遇反应强烈。他们可能认为公平待遇会让他们得到应得的奖励,这可能表明他们在群体中受到重视,或者公平待遇可能被视为人类功能的重要和基本原则。2020年组织公正研究的重点是理解公平判断及其后果的产生机制,以及限制观察到的与其前件和结果的关系的边界条件。
{"title":"Organizational Justice","authors":"D. D. Steiner","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.555","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.555","url":null,"abstract":"Organizational justice refers to people’s perceptions of the fairness or unfairness of the treatment they receive in the organizations where they work. The ways authorities, such as supervisors and managers, make decisions and implement them are evaluated by employees in terms of their fairness. Other agents, such as coworkers and customers who interact with employees, also can generate judgments of fairness or unfairness at work. These fairness perceptions can be conceived according to four dimensions of organizational justice as well as in general terms. The four dimensions are distributive, procedural, interpersonal, and informational. Typically, distributive justice evaluates the equity of treatment, where people expect outcomes proportionate to their contributions. Workers also evaluate the fairness of procedures used to make decisions and the quality of their interpersonal relations with the various actors of the organization, including the information the actors communicate regarding decisions and the procedures followed to make them. When people perceive that they are treated fairly, positive consequences result for them and for their organizations. Thus, they tend to be more satisfied, evaluate their management more favorably, engage in more prosocial behaviors within their organizations, perform at higher levels, and remain in their employing organizations for longer periods. When people experience unfair treatment, negative consequences include stress and health-related concerns for employees, negative attitudes toward the organization, and counterproductive behaviors, such as theft, vandalism, or absenteeism. People react strongly to fair or unfair treatment for different reasons. They may believe that fair treatment will allow them to receive the rewards that they deserve, it may communicate that they are valued in a group, or fair treatment may be valued as an important and basic principle of human functioning. Research on organizational justice in 2020 focuses on understanding the mechanisms producing fairness judgments and their consequences and on the boundary conditions limiting the observed relations with their antecedents and outcomes.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"12 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"128609025","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Thirst
Pub Date : 2020-07-30 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.723
N. Rowland
Thirst is a specific and compelling sensation, often arising from internal signals of dehydration but modulated by many environmental variables. There are several historical landmarks in the study of thirst and drinking behavior. The basic physiology of body fluid balance is important, in particular the mechanisms that conserve fluid loss. The transduction of fluid deficits can be discussed in relation to osmotic pressure (osmoreceptors) and volume (baroreceptors). Other relevant issues include the neurobiological mechanisms by which these signals are transformed to intracellular and extracellular dehydration thirsts, respectively, including the prominent role of structures along the lamina terminalis. Other considerations are the integration of signals from natural dehydration conditions, including water deprivation, thermoregulatory fluid loss, and thirst associated with eating dry food. These mechanisms should also be considered within a broader theoretical framework of organization of motivated behavior based on incentive salience.
口渴是一种特殊的、令人信服的感觉,通常由脱水的内部信号引起,但受许多环境变量的调节。在口渴和饮水行为的研究中有几个历史性的里程碑。体液平衡的基本生理学是重要的,特别是保存体液流失的机制。流体缺陷的转导可以与渗透压(渗透感受器)和体积(压力感受器)有关。其他相关问题包括这些信号分别转化为细胞内和细胞外脱水口渴的神经生物学机制,包括沿末梢层结构的突出作用。其他考虑因素包括自然脱水条件下的信号整合,包括缺水、体温调节液体损失和与吃干食物相关的口渴。这些机制也应该在基于激励显著性的动机行为组织的更广泛的理论框架中加以考虑。
{"title":"Thirst","authors":"N. Rowland","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.723","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.723","url":null,"abstract":"Thirst is a specific and compelling sensation, often arising from internal signals of dehydration but modulated by many environmental variables. There are several historical landmarks in the study of thirst and drinking behavior. The basic physiology of body fluid balance is important, in particular the mechanisms that conserve fluid loss. The transduction of fluid deficits can be discussed in relation to osmotic pressure (osmoreceptors) and volume (baroreceptors). Other relevant issues include the neurobiological mechanisms by which these signals are transformed to intracellular and extracellular dehydration thirsts, respectively, including the prominent role of structures along the lamina terminalis. Other considerations are the integration of signals from natural dehydration conditions, including water deprivation, thermoregulatory fluid loss, and thirst associated with eating dry food. These mechanisms should also be considered within a broader theoretical framework of organization of motivated behavior based on incentive salience.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"45 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"117284513","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1