首页 > 最新文献

Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Auditory Attention 听觉的注意力
Pub Date : 2021-06-28 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.778
C. Beaman
The modern world is noisy. Streets are cacophonies of traffic noise; homes and workplaces are replete with bleeping timers, announcements, and alarms. Everywhere there is the sound of human speech—from the casual chatter of strangers and the unwanted intrusion from electronic devices through to the conversations with friends and loved ones one may actually wish to hear. Unlike vision, it is not possible simply to “close our ears” and shut out the auditory world and nor, in many cases, is it desirable. On the one hand, soft background music or environmental sounds, such as birdsong or the noise of waves against the beach, is often comfortingly pleasurable or reassuring. On the other, alarms are usually auditory for a reason. Nevertheless, people somehow have to identify, from among the babble that surrounds them, the sounds and speech of interest and importance and to follow the thread of a chosen speaker in a crowded auditory environment. Additionally, irrelevant or unwanted chatter or other background noise should not hinder concentration on matters of greater interest or importance—students should ideally be able to study effectively despite noisy classrooms or university halls while still being open to the possibility of important interruptions from elsewhere. The scientific study of auditory attention has been driven by such practical problems: how people somehow manage to select the most interesting or most relevant speaker from the competing auditory demands made by the speech of others or isolate the music of the band from the chatter of the nightclub. In parallel, the causes of auditory distraction—and how to try to avoid it where necessary—have also been subject to scrutiny. A complete theory of auditory attention must account for the mechanisms by which selective attention is achieved, the causes of auditory distraction, and the reasons why individuals might differ in their ability in both cases.
现代世界是嘈杂的。街道上充斥着交通噪音;家庭和工作场所充斥着哔哔作响的计时器、公告和闹钟。到处都有人类说话的声音——从陌生人的闲聊和电子设备的干扰,到与朋友和亲人的交谈,人们实际上可能希望听到。与视觉不同,不可能简单地“闭上我们的耳朵”,把听觉世界拒之门外,而且在许多情况下,这也是不可取的。一方面,柔和的背景音乐或环境声音,如鸟鸣或海浪拍打海滩的声音,通常是令人愉快或安心的。另一方面,警报通常是听觉的,这是有原因的。尽管如此,人们还是不得不从周围的嘈杂声中识别出感兴趣和重要的声音和话语,并在拥挤的听觉环境中跟随选定的演讲者的线索。此外,不相关或不受欢迎的闲聊或其他背景噪音不应妨碍学生集中精力学习更感兴趣或更重要的事情——理想情况下,学生应该能够在嘈杂的教室或大学大厅中有效地学习,同时仍然对其他地方可能出现的重要干扰保持开放的态度。对听觉注意的科学研究一直受到这样一些实际问题的推动:人们如何设法从别人的演讲所提出的竞争性听觉要求中选择出最有趣或最相关的演讲者,或者将乐队的音乐与夜总会的嘈杂声区分开。与此同时,听觉分心的原因——以及如何在必要时避免它——也受到了仔细研究。一个完整的听觉注意理论必须解释选择性注意的实现机制,听觉分心的原因,以及个体在这两种情况下能力不同的原因。
{"title":"Auditory Attention","authors":"C. Beaman","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.778","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.778","url":null,"abstract":"The modern world is noisy. Streets are cacophonies of traffic noise; homes and workplaces are replete with bleeping timers, announcements, and alarms. Everywhere there is the sound of human speech—from the casual chatter of strangers and the unwanted intrusion from electronic devices through to the conversations with friends and loved ones one may actually wish to hear. Unlike vision, it is not possible simply to “close our ears” and shut out the auditory world and nor, in many cases, is it desirable. On the one hand, soft background music or environmental sounds, such as birdsong or the noise of waves against the beach, is often comfortingly pleasurable or reassuring. On the other, alarms are usually auditory for a reason. Nevertheless, people somehow have to identify, from among the babble that surrounds them, the sounds and speech of interest and importance and to follow the thread of a chosen speaker in a crowded auditory environment. Additionally, irrelevant or unwanted chatter or other background noise should not hinder concentration on matters of greater interest or importance—students should ideally be able to study effectively despite noisy classrooms or university halls while still being open to the possibility of important interruptions from elsewhere. The scientific study of auditory attention has been driven by such practical problems: how people somehow manage to select the most interesting or most relevant speaker from the competing auditory demands made by the speech of others or isolate the music of the band from the chatter of the nightclub. In parallel, the causes of auditory distraction—and how to try to avoid it where necessary—have also been subject to scrutiny. A complete theory of auditory attention must account for the mechanisms by which selective attention is achieved, the causes of auditory distraction, and the reasons why individuals might differ in their ability in both cases.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"72 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-06-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"128385750","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Self-Discrepancy and Regulatory Focus 自我矛盾与监管焦点
Pub Date : 2021-06-28 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.244
E. Higgins, E. Nakkawita
Self-discrepancy theory and regulatory focus theory are two related motivational theories. Self-discrepancy theory describes the associations between self and affect, positing that the relations among different sets of self-concepts influence a person’s emotional experience. A discrepancy between a person’s ideal self-guide (e.g., hopes and aspirations) and his or her actual self-concept produces dejection-related emotions (e.g., sadness), whereas a discrepancy between a person’s ought self-guide (e.g., duties and obligations) and his or her actual self-concept produces agitation-related emotions (e.g., anxiety). The intensity of these emotional experiences depends upon the magnitude and accessibility of the associated discrepancy. Regulatory focus theory builds on self-discrepancy theory, positing that distinct self-regulatory systems are reflected in the two types of self-guides proposed in self-discrepancy theory. The promotion system is motivated by ideal end-states, by pursuing hopes and aspirations; as a result, it is primarily concerned with the presence or absence of positive outcomes—with gains and non-gains. Given this focus on gains and non-gains, the promotion system is motivated by fundamental needs for nurturance and growth. In contrast, the prevention system is motivated by ought end-states, by fulfilling duties and obligations; as a result, it is primarily concerned with the presence or absence of negative outcomes—with losses and non-losses. Given this focus on losses and non-losses, the prevention system is motivated by fundamental needs for safety and security. The promotion and prevention systems predict a range of important variables relating to cognition, performance, and decision-making.
自我差异理论和调节焦点理论是两种相关的动机理论。自我差异理论描述了自我和情感之间的联系,假设不同的自我概念之间的关系会影响一个人的情感体验。一个人的理想自我指导(如希望和抱负)和他或她的实际自我概念之间的差异会产生与沮丧相关的情绪(如悲伤),而一个人的应该自我指导(如责任和义务)和他或她的实际自我概念之间的差异会产生与激动相关的情绪(如焦虑)。这些情感体验的强度取决于相关差异的大小和可及性。调节焦点理论建立在自我差异理论的基础上,认为自我差异理论提出的两种类型的自我引导反映了不同的自我调节系统。晋升制度的动机是理想的最终状态,是对希望和抱负的追求;因此,它主要关注的是积极结果的存在与否——有收益还是无收益。鉴于这种对收益和非收益的关注,促进晋升制度的动机是对养育和成长的基本需求。相反,预防制度的动机是最终状态,是履行职责和义务;因此,它主要关注负面结果的存在与否——损失和非损失。鉴于这种对损失和非损失的关注,预防系统的动机是对安全和保障的基本需求。促进和预防系统预测了一系列与认知、表现和决策有关的重要变量。
{"title":"Self-Discrepancy and Regulatory Focus","authors":"E. Higgins, E. Nakkawita","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.244","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.244","url":null,"abstract":"Self-discrepancy theory and regulatory focus theory are two related motivational theories. Self-discrepancy theory describes the associations between self and affect, positing that the relations among different sets of self-concepts influence a person’s emotional experience. A discrepancy between a person’s ideal self-guide (e.g., hopes and aspirations) and his or her actual self-concept produces dejection-related emotions (e.g., sadness), whereas a discrepancy between a person’s ought self-guide (e.g., duties and obligations) and his or her actual self-concept produces agitation-related emotions (e.g., anxiety). The intensity of these emotional experiences depends upon the magnitude and accessibility of the associated discrepancy.\u0000 Regulatory focus theory builds on self-discrepancy theory, positing that distinct self-regulatory systems are reflected in the two types of self-guides proposed in self-discrepancy theory. The promotion system is motivated by ideal end-states, by pursuing hopes and aspirations; as a result, it is primarily concerned with the presence or absence of positive outcomes—with gains and non-gains. Given this focus on gains and non-gains, the promotion system is motivated by fundamental needs for nurturance and growth. In contrast, the prevention system is motivated by ought end-states, by fulfilling duties and obligations; as a result, it is primarily concerned with the presence or absence of negative outcomes—with losses and non-losses. Given this focus on losses and non-losses, the prevention system is motivated by fundamental needs for safety and security. The promotion and prevention systems predict a range of important variables relating to cognition, performance, and decision-making.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"9 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-06-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"114720981","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotions at Work 工作中的情绪
Pub Date : 2021-05-26 DOI: 10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.93
N. Ashkanasy, A. Bialkowski
Beginning in the 1980s, interest in studying emotions in organizational psychology has been on the rise. Prior to 2003, however, researchers in organizational psychology and organizational behavior tended to focus on only one or two levels of analysis. Ashkanasy argued that emotions are more appropriately conceived of as spanning all levels of organizational analysis, and introduced a theory of emotions in organizations that spans five levels of analysis. Level 1 of the model refers to within-person temporal variations in mood and emotion, which employees experience in their everyday working lives. Level 2 refers to individual differences in emotional intelligence and trait affectivity (i.e., between-person emotional variables). Level 3 relates to the perception of emotions in dyadic interactions. Level 4 relates to the emotional states and process that take place between leaders and group members. Level 5 involves organization-wide variables. The article concludes with a discussion of how, via the concept of emotional intelligence, emotions at each level of the model form an integrated picture of emotions in organizational settings.
从20世纪80年代开始,在组织心理学中研究情绪的兴趣一直在上升。然而,在2003年之前,组织心理学和组织行为学的研究者往往只关注一两个层面的分析。Ashkanasy认为,将情绪理解为跨越组织分析的所有层次更为恰当,并引入了一个跨越五个分析层次的组织情绪理论。模型的第一级是指员工在日常工作生活中经历的情绪和情绪的个人内部时间变化。第二级是指情绪智力和特质情感的个体差异(即人与人之间的情绪变量)。第3级涉及在二元互动中对情绪的感知。第四级涉及领导者和团队成员之间的情绪状态和过程。第5级涉及组织范围的变量。文章最后讨论了如何通过情商的概念,在模型的每一个层次上的情绪形成了组织环境中情绪的综合图景。
{"title":"Emotions at Work","authors":"N. Ashkanasy, A. Bialkowski","doi":"10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.93","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.93","url":null,"abstract":"Beginning in the 1980s, interest in studying emotions in organizational psychology has been on the rise. Prior to 2003, however, researchers in organizational psychology and organizational behavior tended to focus on only one or two levels of analysis. Ashkanasy argued that emotions are more appropriately conceived of as spanning all levels of organizational analysis, and introduced a theory of emotions in organizations that spans five levels of analysis. Level 1 of the model refers to within-person temporal variations in mood and emotion, which employees experience in their everyday working lives. Level 2 refers to individual differences in emotional intelligence and trait affectivity (i.e., between-person emotional variables). Level 3 relates to the perception of emotions in dyadic interactions. Level 4 relates to the emotional states and process that take place between leaders and group members. Level 5 involves organization-wide variables. The article concludes with a discussion of how, via the concept of emotional intelligence, emotions at each level of the model form an integrated picture of emotions in organizational settings.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"140 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-05-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"126931160","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Projective Psychodiagnostics: Inkblots, Stories, and Drawings as Clinical Measures 投射性精神诊断:墨迹、故事和图画作为临床测量
Pub Date : 2021-05-26 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.637
M. Eby
Projective psychodiagnostics refers to the use of psychological instruments through which the subject is asked to respond to a set of ambiguous (though often suggestive) stimuli, thereby “projecting” aspects of their personality into these responses. The most prominent of these instruments includes the Rorschach Inkblot Technique, in which the subject is confronted with ten inkblots and is asked what these stimuli look like, and then what perceptual features make them look that way. Another common projective technique is the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), a storytelling exercise in which the subject responds with a narrative to a series of ambiguous but sometimes highly charged black and white pictures depicting human interactions. Over time, new pictures have been developed for similar storytelling instruments targeted to children (the Children’s Apperception Test) or different ethnic populations. Both of these tests emerged under the influence of psychodynamic theories, and of the work of Carl Jung, whose Word Association Test served as a projective measure of psychological conflicts. Finally, there is a series of drawing tests which, while less commonly used, have had a projective history, including human figure drawings, the Bender–Gestalt Test, and the Wartegg Drawing Completion Test. Projective instruments have been used in a variety of psychiatric settings and have been criticized for being insufficiently grounded in either quantitative measures or scientific validity. The Rorschach has emerged with increasingly statistically based scoring systems addressing perceptual features, language, and content in the assessment of risk and diagnosis. The TAT is essentially a structured interview (since most scoring systems are not used by clinicians), but it nonetheless appears to be useful in gleaning information about a subject’s relationships with other people. Drawing tasks and sentence completion tests (derived from word association tests) are less commonly used, though more prevalent with children whose verbal abilities may be more limited. In general, projective tests appear to have some limited ability to define diagnosis and risk (and can be especially helpful in defining thought disorder and prognosis), but they may be most useful in helping clinicians obtain a deeper picture of conflicts and resources within the person tested.
投射性精神诊断指的是使用心理工具,通过这些工具,受试者被要求对一系列模糊的(尽管通常是暗示性的)刺激做出反应,从而将他们个性的各个方面“投射”到这些反应中。这些工具中最著名的包括罗夏墨迹技术(Rorschach Inkblot Technique),在这项技术中,受试者面对10个墨迹,并被问及这些刺激是什么样子的,然后是什么感知特征使它们看起来是那样的。另一种常见的投射技巧是主题统觉测试(Thematic Apperception Test, TAT),这是一种讲故事的练习,在这个练习中,受试者对一系列描述人类互动的模糊但有时高度紧张的黑白图片进行叙述。随着时间的推移,针对儿童(儿童统觉测试)或不同种族人群的类似讲故事工具开发了新的图片。这两种测试都是在心理动力学理论和卡尔·荣格(Carl Jung)的工作的影响下出现的,荣格的单词联想测试是一种心理冲突的投射测量方法。最后,还有一系列的绘图测试,虽然不太常用,但有一个投影的历史,包括人体绘图,弯曲格式塔测试和华特格绘图完成测试。投射工具已经在各种精神病学设置中使用,并被批评为在定量测量或科学有效性方面缺乏充分的基础。罗夏墨迹已经出现了越来越多的基于统计的评分系统,解决了风险和诊断评估中的感知特征、语言和内容。TAT本质上是一种结构化的访谈(因为大多数评分系统不被临床医生使用),但它似乎在收集受试者与其他人关系的信息方面很有用。绘画任务和句子完成测试(来源于单词联想测试)不太常用,尽管在语言能力可能更有限的孩子中更常见。一般来说,投射性测试在定义诊断和风险方面的能力有限(在定义思维障碍和预后方面尤其有用),但它们可能在帮助临床医生更深入地了解被测试者的冲突和资源方面最有用。
{"title":"Projective Psychodiagnostics: Inkblots, Stories, and Drawings as Clinical Measures","authors":"M. Eby","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.637","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.637","url":null,"abstract":"Projective psychodiagnostics refers to the use of psychological instruments through which the subject is asked to respond to a set of ambiguous (though often suggestive) stimuli, thereby “projecting” aspects of their personality into these responses. The most prominent of these instruments includes the Rorschach Inkblot Technique, in which the subject is confronted with ten inkblots and is asked what these stimuli look like, and then what perceptual features make them look that way. Another common projective technique is the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), a storytelling exercise in which the subject responds with a narrative to a series of ambiguous but sometimes highly charged black and white pictures depicting human interactions. Over time, new pictures have been developed for similar storytelling instruments targeted to children (the Children’s Apperception Test) or different ethnic populations. Both of these tests emerged under the influence of psychodynamic theories, and of the work of Carl Jung, whose Word Association Test served as a projective measure of psychological conflicts. Finally, there is a series of drawing tests which, while less commonly used, have had a projective history, including human figure drawings, the Bender–Gestalt Test, and the Wartegg Drawing Completion Test.\u0000 Projective instruments have been used in a variety of psychiatric settings and have been criticized for being insufficiently grounded in either quantitative measures or scientific validity. The Rorschach has emerged with increasingly statistically based scoring systems addressing perceptual features, language, and content in the assessment of risk and diagnosis. The TAT is essentially a structured interview (since most scoring systems are not used by clinicians), but it nonetheless appears to be useful in gleaning information about a subject’s relationships with other people. Drawing tasks and sentence completion tests (derived from word association tests) are less commonly used, though more prevalent with children whose verbal abilities may be more limited. In general, projective tests appear to have some limited ability to define diagnosis and risk (and can be especially helpful in defining thought disorder and prognosis), but they may be most useful in helping clinicians obtain a deeper picture of conflicts and resources within the person tested.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"57 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-05-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"121487918","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Judgment and Decision Making 判断和决策
Pub Date : 2021-04-26 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.536
P. Brust-Renck, Rebecca B Weldon, V. Reyna
Everyday life is comprised of a series of decisions, from choosing what to wear to deciding what major to declare in college and whom to share a life with. Modern era economic theories were first brought into psychology in the 1950s and 1960s by Ward Edwards and Herbert Simon. Simon suggested that individuals do not always choose the best alternative among the options because they are bounded by cognitive limitations (e.g., memory). People who choose the good-enough option “satisfice” rather than optimize, because they are bounded by their limited time, knowledge, and computational capacity. Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky were among those who took the next step by demonstrating that individuals are not only limited but are inconsistent in their preferences, and hence irrational. Describing a series of biases and fallacies, they elaborated intuitive strategies (i.e., heuristics) that people tend to use when faced with difficult questions (e.g., “What proportion of long-distance relationships break up within a year?”) by answering based on simpler, similar questions (e.g., “Do instances of swift breakups of long-distance relationships come readily to mind?”). More recently, the emotion-versus-reason debate has been incorporated into the field as an approach to how judgments can be governed by two fundamentally different processes, such as intuition (or affect) and reasoning (or deliberation). A series of dual-process approaches by Seymour Epstein, George Lowenstein, Elke Weber, Paul Slovic, and Ellen Peters, among others, attempt to explain how a decision based on emotional and/or impulsive judgments (i.e., system 1) should be distinguished from those that are based on a slow process that is governed by rules of reasoning (i.e., system 2). Valerie Reyna and Charles Brainerd and other scholars take a different approach to dual processes and propose a theory—fuzzy-trace theory—that incorporates many of the prior theoretical elements but also introduces the novel concept of gist mental representations of information (i.e., essential meaning) shaped by culture and experience. Adding to processes of emotion or reward sensitivity and reasoning or deliberation, fuzzy-trace theory characterizes gist as insightful intuition (as opposed to crude system 1 intuition) and contrasts it with verbatim or precise processing that does not consist of meaningful interpretation. Some of these new perspectives explain classic paradoxes and predict new effects that allow us to better understand human judgment and decision making. More recent contributions to the field include research in neuroscience, in particular from neuroeconomics.
每天的生活都是由一系列的决定组成的,从选择穿什么到决定在大学里选什么专业,再到决定和谁一起生活。在20世纪50年代和60年代,沃德·爱德华兹(Ward Edwards)和赫伯特·西蒙(Herbert Simon)首次将现代经济理论引入心理学。西蒙认为,个体并不总是在选项中选择最佳选项,因为他们受到认知限制(例如,记忆)的限制。选择“足够好”选项的人“满足”而不是优化,因为他们受限于有限的时间、知识和计算能力。丹尼尔·卡尼曼(Daniel Kahneman)和阿莫斯·特沃斯基(Amos Tversky)等人进一步证明,个人的偏好不仅有限,而且不一致,因此是非理性的。他们描述了一系列的偏见和谬论,详细阐述了人们在面对难题时倾向于使用的直觉策略(即启发式)(例如,“一年内远距离恋情破裂的比例是多少?”),通过回答更简单、类似的问题(例如,“远距离恋情迅速分手的例子是否很容易浮现在脑海中?”)。最近,情感与理性的争论已经被纳入该领域,作为一种方法来研究判断如何被两个根本不同的过程所支配,比如直觉(或情感)和推理(或审议)。西摩·爱泼斯坦、乔治·洛温斯坦、埃尔克·韦伯、保罗·斯洛维奇和艾伦·彼得斯等人提出了一系列双过程方法,试图解释如何区分基于情绪和/或冲动判断(即系统1)的决策,以及基于受推理规则支配的缓慢过程的决策(即:瓦莱丽·雷纳和查尔斯·布雷纳德等学者对双重过程采取了不同的方法,提出了一种理论——模糊痕迹理论——它融合了许多先前的理论元素,但也引入了文化和经验形成的信息(即本质意义)的主要心理表征的新概念。除了情感、奖励、敏感、推理或思考的过程之外,模糊追踪理论将主旨描述为有洞察力的直觉(与粗糙的系统1直觉相反),并将其与逐字逐句或不包含有意义的解释的精确处理进行对比。其中一些新观点解释了经典的悖论,并预测了新的影响,使我们能够更好地理解人类的判断和决策。该领域最近的贡献包括神经科学的研究,特别是神经经济学的研究。
{"title":"Judgment and Decision Making","authors":"P. Brust-Renck, Rebecca B Weldon, V. Reyna","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.536","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.536","url":null,"abstract":"Everyday life is comprised of a series of decisions, from choosing what to wear to deciding what major to declare in college and whom to share a life with. Modern era economic theories were first brought into psychology in the 1950s and 1960s by Ward Edwards and Herbert Simon. Simon suggested that individuals do not always choose the best alternative among the options because they are bounded by cognitive limitations (e.g., memory). People who choose the good-enough option “satisfice” rather than optimize, because they are bounded by their limited time, knowledge, and computational capacity. Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky were among those who took the next step by demonstrating that individuals are not only limited but are inconsistent in their preferences, and hence irrational. Describing a series of biases and fallacies, they elaborated intuitive strategies (i.e., heuristics) that people tend to use when faced with difficult questions (e.g., “What proportion of long-distance relationships break up within a year?”) by answering based on simpler, similar questions (e.g., “Do instances of swift breakups of long-distance relationships come readily to mind?”).\u0000 More recently, the emotion-versus-reason debate has been incorporated into the field as an approach to how judgments can be governed by two fundamentally different processes, such as intuition (or affect) and reasoning (or deliberation). A series of dual-process approaches by Seymour Epstein, George Lowenstein, Elke Weber, Paul Slovic, and Ellen Peters, among others, attempt to explain how a decision based on emotional and/or impulsive judgments (i.e., system 1) should be distinguished from those that are based on a slow process that is governed by rules of reasoning (i.e., system 2). Valerie Reyna and Charles Brainerd and other scholars take a different approach to dual processes and propose a theory—fuzzy-trace theory—that incorporates many of the prior theoretical elements but also introduces the novel concept of gist mental representations of information (i.e., essential meaning) shaped by culture and experience. Adding to processes of emotion or reward sensitivity and reasoning or deliberation, fuzzy-trace theory characterizes gist as insightful intuition (as opposed to crude system 1 intuition) and contrasts it with verbatim or precise processing that does not consist of meaningful interpretation. Some of these new perspectives explain classic paradoxes and predict new effects that allow us to better understand human judgment and decision making. More recent contributions to the field include research in neuroscience, in particular from neuroeconomics.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"14 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-04-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"122065543","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The Social Psychology of Sex and Gender 性与社会性别的社会心理学
Pub Date : 2021-03-25 DOI: 10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.513
P. Hegarty, Emma Sarter
Between the late 1960s and early 1980s, gender became an important topic in U.S. social psychology, raising questions about the conceptual relationship between “sex” and “gender.” A second-wave feminist project to describe differences between women and men as previously exaggerated and currently changeable was aligned with social psychology’s emphasis on the distorting power of stereotypes and the strong influence of immediate situations on human behavior. Feminism and social psychology both suggested psychology could foment social transformation, and the authors and participants of psychological research have undoubtedly become far less “womanless” in the past half-century. By the late 1980s several incommensurate social psychologies of gender existed, creating debates about the meaning of emphasizing gender differences and similarities and the gendered social psychology of psychological science itself. However, psychology remained largely a “white space” in the 1970s and 1980s, which were also “difficult decades” in transgender history. The increasing recognition of intersectional feminism and trans-affirmative perspectives in the 2010s set the context for regarding this history from different contemporary standpoints.
在20世纪60年代末至80年代初,性别成为美国社会心理学的一个重要话题,提出了关于“性”和“性别”之间概念关系的问题。第二波女权主义项目将男女之间的差异描述为以前被夸大的,现在是可变的,这与社会心理学强调的刻板印象的扭曲力量和即时情况对人类行为的强烈影响是一致的。女权主义和社会心理学都认为心理学可以促进社会转型,在过去的半个世纪里,心理学研究的作者和参与者无疑变得不再那么“无女人”了。到20世纪80年代末,出现了几种不相称的性别社会心理学,引起了关于强调性别差异和相似性的意义以及心理科学本身的性别社会心理学的争论。然而,在20世纪70年代和80年代,心理学在很大程度上仍然是一个“空白”,这也是跨性别历史上“困难的几十年”。2010年代对交叉女权主义和跨性别平权观点的日益认可,为从不同的当代立场看待这段历史奠定了背景。
{"title":"The Social Psychology of Sex and Gender","authors":"P. Hegarty, Emma Sarter","doi":"10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.513","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.513","url":null,"abstract":"Between the late 1960s and early 1980s, gender became an important topic in U.S. social psychology, raising questions about the conceptual relationship between “sex” and “gender.” A second-wave feminist project to describe differences between women and men as previously exaggerated and currently changeable was aligned with social psychology’s emphasis on the distorting power of stereotypes and the strong influence of immediate situations on human behavior. Feminism and social psychology both suggested psychology could foment social transformation, and the authors and participants of psychological research have undoubtedly become far less “womanless” in the past half-century. By the late 1980s several incommensurate social psychologies of gender existed, creating debates about the meaning of emphasizing gender differences and similarities and the gendered social psychology of psychological science itself. However, psychology remained largely a “white space” in the 1970s and 1980s, which were also “difficult decades” in transgender history. The increasing recognition of intersectional feminism and trans-affirmative perspectives in the 2010s set the context for regarding this history from different contemporary standpoints.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"9 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-03-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"130361666","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Depression 抑郁症的认知行为疗法
Pub Date : 2021-02-23 DOI: 10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.837
S. Moorey, S. Hollon
Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) has the strongest evidence base of all the psychological treatments for depression. It has been shown to be effective in reducing symptoms of depression and preventing relapse. All models of CBT share in common an assumption that emotional states are created and maintained through learned patterns of thoughts and behaviors and that new and more helpful patterns can be learned through psychological interventions. They also share a commitment to empirical testing of the theory and clinical practice. Beck’s Cognitive Therapy sees negative distorted thinking as central to depression and is the most established form of CBT for depression. Behavioral approaches, such as Behavioral Activation, which emphasize behavioral rather than cognitive change, also has a growing evidence base. Promising results are emerging from therapies such as Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) and rumination-focused therapy that focus on the process of managing thoughts rather than their content. Its efficacy-established CBT now faces the challenge of cost-effective dissemination to depressed people in the community.
认知行为疗法(CBT)在所有治疗抑郁症的心理疗法中具有最有力的证据基础。它已被证明对减轻抑郁症状和防止复发有效。所有的CBT模式都有一个共同的假设,即情绪状态是通过习得的思想和行为模式来创造和维持的,而新的、更有益的模式可以通过心理干预来习得。他们还共同致力于理论和临床实践的实证检验。贝克的认知疗法认为消极扭曲的思维是抑郁症的核心,也是治疗抑郁症的最成熟的认知行为疗法。行为方法,如强调行为而不是认知变化的行为激活,也有越来越多的证据基础。基于正念的认知疗法(MBCT)和以反刍为重点的疗法正在产生有希望的结果,这些疗法侧重于管理思想的过程,而不是其内容。它的有效性建立CBT现在面临的挑战是具有成本效益的传播到社区的抑郁症患者。
{"title":"Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Depression","authors":"S. Moorey, S. Hollon","doi":"10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.837","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.837","url":null,"abstract":"Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) has the strongest evidence base of all the psychological treatments for depression. It has been shown to be effective in reducing symptoms of depression and preventing relapse. All models of CBT share in common an assumption that emotional states are created and maintained through learned patterns of thoughts and behaviors and that new and more helpful patterns can be learned through psychological interventions. They also share a commitment to empirical testing of the theory and clinical practice. Beck’s Cognitive Therapy sees negative distorted thinking as central to depression and is the most established form of CBT for depression. Behavioral approaches, such as Behavioral Activation, which emphasize behavioral rather than cognitive change, also has a growing evidence base. Promising results are emerging from therapies such as Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) and rumination-focused therapy that focus on the process of managing thoughts rather than their content. Its efficacy-established CBT now faces the challenge of cost-effective dissemination to depressed people in the community.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"109 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-02-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"123123706","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Common Factors in Psychotherapy 心理治疗的常见因素
Pub Date : 2021-02-23 DOI: 10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.79
Julia Browne, C. Cather, K. Mueser
Common factors, or characteristics that are present across psychotherapies, have long been considered important to fostering positive psychotherapy outcomes. The contextual model offers an overarching theoretical framework for how common factors facilitate therapeutic change. Specifically, this model posits that improvements occur through three primary pathways: (a) the real relationship, (b) expectations, and (c) specific ingredients. The most-well-studied common factors, which also are described within the contextual model, include the therapeutic alliance, therapist empathy, positive regard, genuineness, and client expectations. Empirical studies have demonstrated that a strong therapeutic alliance, higher ratings of therapist empathy, positive regard, genuineness, and more favorable outcome expectations are related to improved treatment outcomes. Yet, the long-standing debate continues regarding whether psychotherapy outcomes are most heavily determined by these common factors or by factors specific to the type of therapy used. There have been calls for an integration of the two perspectives and a shift toward evaluating mechanisms as a way to move the field forward. Nonetheless, the common factors are valuable in treatment delivery and should be a focus in delivering psychotherapy.
长期以来,人们一直认为心理治疗中存在的共同因素或特征对促进积极的心理治疗结果很重要。情境模型为共同因素如何促进治疗改变提供了一个总体的理论框架。具体来说,该模型假设改善通过三个主要途径发生:(a)真实关系,(b)期望,(c)特定成分。研究得最充分的共同因素,也在情境模型中被描述,包括治疗联盟、治疗师共情、积极关注、真诚和客户期望。实证研究表明,强大的治疗联盟、更高的治疗师共情评分、积极关注、真诚度和更有利的结果预期与改善的治疗结果有关。然而,长期以来的争论仍在继续,即心理治疗的结果是由这些共同因素还是由所使用的治疗类型的特定因素决定的。有人呼吁将这两种观点结合起来,并转向评估机制,作为推动该领域向前发展的一种方式。尽管如此,这些共同因素在治疗过程中是有价值的,应该成为提供心理治疗的重点。
{"title":"Common Factors in Psychotherapy","authors":"Julia Browne, C. Cather, K. Mueser","doi":"10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.79","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.79","url":null,"abstract":"Common factors, or characteristics that are present across psychotherapies, have long been considered important to fostering positive psychotherapy outcomes. The contextual model offers an overarching theoretical framework for how common factors facilitate therapeutic change. Specifically, this model posits that improvements occur through three primary pathways: (a) the real relationship, (b) expectations, and (c) specific ingredients. The most-well-studied common factors, which also are described within the contextual model, include the therapeutic alliance, therapist empathy, positive regard, genuineness, and client expectations. Empirical studies have demonstrated that a strong therapeutic alliance, higher ratings of therapist empathy, positive regard, genuineness, and more favorable outcome expectations are related to improved treatment outcomes. Yet, the long-standing debate continues regarding whether psychotherapy outcomes are most heavily determined by these common factors or by factors specific to the type of therapy used. There have been calls for an integration of the two perspectives and a shift toward evaluating mechanisms as a way to move the field forward. Nonetheless, the common factors are valuable in treatment delivery and should be a focus in delivering psychotherapy.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"43 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-02-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"123232344","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Play
Pub Date : 2021-02-23 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.791
S. Pellis, V. Pellis
Play behavior is relatively rare in the animal kingdom, but is widespread, and in some lineages is very common not only in childhood but also in adulthood. It can take many forms, as playful actions can be directed to a social partner (social play), to an inanimate object (object play), or self-directed, as the animal, jumps, runs, and turns (locomotor-rotational play). Considerable progress has been made in understanding the neural, emotional, and cognitive mechanisms mammals use in regulating social play, but whether comparable mechanisms are used to regulate other forms of play, or apply to non-mammalian animals, remains to be resolved. Similarly, social play in some mammals has been demonstrated to benefit the development of sociocognitive skills and emotional resilience, while locomotor-rotational play can benefit the development of motor skills. The factors that allow some species to gain these benefits also remain to be resolved. Statistical approaches that take the relatedness of species into account are increasingly being applied to analyze a growing comparative database that includes species from many different lineages. In addition, mathematical and computational models are being used to test the explanatory power of various factors to account for the evolution of play. Coupled with new methods in neuroscience that provide a deeper understanding of the brain during play, these approaches will enable extraordinary progress in understanding play over the next few decades.
玩耍行为在动物王国中相对罕见,但却很普遍,在某些谱系中,不仅在儿童时期,而且在成年期都很常见。它可以有多种形式,如游戏行为可以指向社会伙伴(社交游戏),指向无生命的物体(物体游戏),或自我导向,如动物跳跃、奔跑和转身(运动旋转游戏)。在理解哺乳动物调节社交游戏的神经、情感和认知机制方面已经取得了相当大的进展,但类似的机制是否用于调节其他形式的游戏,或适用于非哺乳动物,仍有待解决。同样,一些哺乳动物的社交游戏已被证明有利于社会认知技能和情绪恢复能力的发展,而运动旋转游戏则有利于运动技能的发展。使某些物种获得这些好处的因素也有待解决。考虑物种亲缘关系的统计方法越来越多地被应用于分析日益增长的比较数据库,其中包括来自许多不同谱系的物种。此外,数学和计算模型被用来测试各种因素的解释能力,以解释游戏的演变。再加上神经科学的新方法可以更深入地了解游戏过程中的大脑,这些方法将在未来几十年里在理解游戏方面取得非凡的进展。
{"title":"Play","authors":"S. Pellis, V. Pellis","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.791","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.791","url":null,"abstract":"Play behavior is relatively rare in the animal kingdom, but is widespread, and in some lineages is very common not only in childhood but also in adulthood. It can take many forms, as playful actions can be directed to a social partner (social play), to an inanimate object (object play), or self-directed, as the animal, jumps, runs, and turns (locomotor-rotational play). Considerable progress has been made in understanding the neural, emotional, and cognitive mechanisms mammals use in regulating social play, but whether comparable mechanisms are used to regulate other forms of play, or apply to non-mammalian animals, remains to be resolved. Similarly, social play in some mammals has been demonstrated to benefit the development of sociocognitive skills and emotional resilience, while locomotor-rotational play can benefit the development of motor skills. The factors that allow some species to gain these benefits also remain to be resolved. Statistical approaches that take the relatedness of species into account are increasingly being applied to analyze a growing comparative database that includes species from many different lineages. In addition, mathematical and computational models are being used to test the explanatory power of various factors to account for the evolution of play. Coupled with new methods in neuroscience that provide a deeper understanding of the brain during play, these approaches will enable extraordinary progress in understanding play over the next few decades.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"208 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-02-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"121197147","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychoanalysis in Argentina 阿根廷的精神分析
Pub Date : 2021-01-22 DOI: 10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.689
H. Klappenbach, A. Gentile, F. Ferrari, H. Scholten
Psychoanalysis in Argentina has been established as a profession since the foundation of the Argentine Psychoanalytic Association (1942), and the perspective of Melanie Klein initially predominated. Before that institutional event, Freud’s theories were considered in a more far-reaching and less homogeneous intellectual and medical field. At the beginning of the 20th century, Freud’s first readers in Argentina were strongly influenced by French culture and science. Although the initial mention of Freud’s work was by a Chilean doctor, Germán Greve, intellectuals such as José Ingenieros or Enrique Mouchet also read him from a critical perspective. In the 1950s and 1960s, consolidated psychoanalytic institutional spaces had been developed in Buenos Aires, while in the other provinces there was still the gestation of the institutional field that would allow the specific training of psychoanalysts. In two of the most important cities, Córdoba and Rosario, psychoanalysis was adopted by a group of intellectuals, physicians, and judges linked to the University Reform movement. Deodoro Roca, Jorge Orgaz, Saúl Taborda, Juan Filloy, and Gregorio Bermann adopted the Viennese theories, albeit from different perspectives. In Rosario, the figure of Pizarro Crespo not only integrated Freud’s ideas into a psychosomatic perspective, but, in an unsuspected way, constitutes the first reference to Jacques Lacan’s work in Argentina. Toward the 1960s, the creation of undergraduate psychology programs was marked by the presence of notable teachers linked to psychoanalysis. Around the same time, a new paradigm was introduced into psychoanalysis: Lacanianism. Within the framework of the reception of structuralism, the theories of Louis Althusser and the first discussions of Lacan’s teaching began to spread. This new paradigm had a decisive impact on different professional fields and varying social sciences in the country. While Oscar Masotta became one of the main disseminators of Lacan in Buenos Aires, Raúl Sciarretta and Rafael Paz were more relevant in other provinces of the country, particularly in the cities of Córdoba, Rosario, and Tucumán, cities where the institutionalization of psychoanalysis was strengthened from the 1970s onwards.
自阿根廷精神分析协会(1942)成立以来,精神分析在阿根廷已经成为一种专业,Melanie Klein的观点最初占主导地位。在这一制度化事件之前,弗洛伊德的理论被认为是一个更深远的、不那么同质化的智力和医学领域。20世纪初,弗洛伊德在阿根廷的第一批读者深受法国文化和科学的影响。虽然最初提到弗洛伊德作品的是智利医生Germán Greve,但像joss Ingenieros和Enrique Mouchet这样的知识分子也从批判的角度阅读了他的作品。在20世纪50年代和60年代,在布宜诺斯艾利斯建立了统一的精神分析机构空间,而在其他省份,仍有机构领域的孕育,这将允许精神分析学家的具体培训。在两个最重要的城市Córdoba和罗萨里奥(Rosario),精神分析被一群与大学改革运动有关的知识分子、医生和法官所采用。Deodoro Roca, Jorge Orgaz, Saúl Taborda, Juan Filloy和Gregorio Bermann从不同的角度采纳了维也纳的理论。在《罗萨里奥》中,皮萨罗·克雷斯波的形象不仅将弗洛伊德的思想融入到心身视角中,而且以一种意想不到的方式,构成了雅克·拉康在阿根廷工作的第一个参考。在20世纪60年代,本科心理学课程的创建以与精神分析相关的著名教师的出现为标志。大约在同一时间,一种新的范式被引入精神分析:拉康主义。在接受结构主义的框架内,阿尔都塞的理论和对拉康教学的初步讨论开始传播。这种新范式对我国不同专业领域和不同社会科学产生了决定性的影响。当Oscar Masotta成为拉康在布宜诺斯艾利斯的主要传播者之一时,Raúl Sciarretta和Rafael Paz在该国的其他省份更具相关性,特别是在Córdoba, Rosario和Tucumán等城市,这些城市的精神分析制度化从20世纪70年代开始得到加强。
{"title":"Psychoanalysis in Argentina","authors":"H. Klappenbach, A. Gentile, F. Ferrari, H. Scholten","doi":"10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.689","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/ACREFORE/9780190236557.013.689","url":null,"abstract":"Psychoanalysis in Argentina has been established as a profession since the foundation of the Argentine Psychoanalytic Association (1942), and the perspective of Melanie Klein initially predominated. Before that institutional event, Freud’s theories were considered in a more far-reaching and less homogeneous intellectual and medical field. At the beginning of the 20th century, Freud’s first readers in Argentina were strongly influenced by French culture and science. Although the initial mention of Freud’s work was by a Chilean doctor, Germán Greve, intellectuals such as José Ingenieros or Enrique Mouchet also read him from a critical perspective. In the 1950s and 1960s, consolidated psychoanalytic institutional spaces had been developed in Buenos Aires, while in the other provinces there was still the gestation of the institutional field that would allow the specific training of psychoanalysts. In two of the most important cities, Córdoba and Rosario, psychoanalysis was adopted by a group of intellectuals, physicians, and judges linked to the University Reform movement. Deodoro Roca, Jorge Orgaz, Saúl Taborda, Juan Filloy, and Gregorio Bermann adopted the Viennese theories, albeit from different perspectives. In Rosario, the figure of Pizarro Crespo not only integrated Freud’s ideas into a psychosomatic perspective, but, in an unsuspected way, constitutes the first reference to Jacques Lacan’s work in Argentina. Toward the 1960s, the creation of undergraduate psychology programs was marked by the presence of notable teachers linked to psychoanalysis. Around the same time, a new paradigm was introduced into psychoanalysis: Lacanianism. Within the framework of the reception of structuralism, the theories of Louis Althusser and the first discussions of Lacan’s teaching began to spread. This new paradigm had a decisive impact on different professional fields and varying social sciences in the country. While Oscar Masotta became one of the main disseminators of Lacan in Buenos Aires, Raúl Sciarretta and Rafael Paz were more relevant in other provinces of the country, particularly in the cities of Córdoba, Rosario, and Tucumán, cities where the institutionalization of psychoanalysis was strengthened from the 1970s onwards.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"70 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-01-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"127966776","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1