首页 > 最新文献

Autism and Developmental Language Impairments最新文献

英文 中文
The effectiveness of semantic intervention for word-finding difficulties in college-aged students (16–19 years) with persistent Language Disorder 语义干预对16-19岁持续性语言障碍大学生找字困难的效果
Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2019-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/2396941519870784
L. Campbell, Hilary Nicoll, S. Ebbels
Background and aims Little evidence exists for the effectiveness of intervention for older adolescents and young adults with language disorders, particularly for those over 16 years. This study involves college-aged students aged 16–19 years with Language Disorder and Word-Finding Difficulties and investigates whether progress in word finding following 1:1 semantic intervention is greater than progress during a baseline period as measured by a standardised test. Methods Twenty-five college-aged students (20 males and 5 females) with Language Disorder and Word-Finding Difficulties (aged 16;4–18;4 years) participated in a single baseline design study comparing progress on the Test of Adolescent and Adult Word Finding during an intervention period with progress during a baseline period of equal length. Intervention was focused on semantics of different words from those in the Test of Adolescent and Adult Word Finding and was delivered 1:1 by the participants’ usual Speech and Language Therapist, 30 minutes per week, for eight weeks. Results The participants made significant progress in raw score on the Test of Adolescent and Adult Word Finding during both the baseline (d = 1.4) and intervention (d = 2.5) periods, but progress during the intervention period was significantly greater than during the baseline period (d = 1.16). Individual data showed reliable change for five participants during the baseline period and for 20 participants during the intervention period. At the start of the study, all participants had standard scores below 85, but after intervention, 10/25 participants scored above 85. Conclusions Four hours of semantic intervention led to significantly greater gains on a standardised test of word finding than during a baseline period of equal length in 16–19 year olds with Language Disorder and Word-Finding Difficulties. The words in the standardised test had not been included in the intervention, indicating generalised gains. Implications This study shows that intervention (at least for Word-Finding Difficulties) can be effective for this older age group of college-aged students with Language Disorder and therefore the effectiveness of Speech and Language Therapy services for this age group in a wider range of areas of language should also be investigated.
背景和目的很少有证据表明对患有语言障碍的老年青少年和年轻人进行干预的有效性,尤其是对16岁以上的人。这项研究涉及16-19岁患有语言障碍和单词查找困难的大学生,并调查了1:1语义干预后单词查找的进展是否大于标准化测试测量的基线期的进展。方法25名患有语言障碍和查词困难的大学生(男20名,女5名)(年龄16岁;4-18岁;4岁)参加了一项单一基线设计研究,比较干预期内青少年和成人查词测试的进展与同等长度的基线期内的进展。干预的重点是青少年和成人单词发现测试中不同单词的语义,并由参与者的普通言语和语言治疗师进行1:1的干预,每周30分钟,持续8周。结果参与者在青少年和成人单词发现测试中的原始分数在基线(d = 1.4)和干预(d = 2.5)期,但干预期的进展明显大于基线期(d = 1.16)。个体数据显示,5名参与者在基线期间和20名参与者在干预期间发生了可靠的变化。在研究开始时,所有参与者的标准得分都低于85分,但在干预后,10/25的参与者得分高于85分。结论在16-19岁的语言障碍和查词困难患者中,4小时的语义干预使标准化查词测试的成绩显著高于同等长度的基线期。标准化测试中的单词没有被包括在干预中,这表明了普遍的收获。含义这项研究表明,干预(至少针对单词查找困难)对这一年龄段的语言障碍大学生是有效的,因此,还应该调查这一年龄组在更广泛的语言领域的言语和语言治疗服务的有效性。
{"title":"The effectiveness of semantic intervention for word-finding difficulties in college-aged students (16–19 years) with persistent Language Disorder","authors":"L. Campbell, Hilary Nicoll, S. Ebbels","doi":"10.1177/2396941519870784","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941519870784","url":null,"abstract":"Background and aims Little evidence exists for the effectiveness of intervention for older adolescents and young adults with language disorders, particularly for those over 16 years. This study involves college-aged students aged 16–19 years with Language Disorder and Word-Finding Difficulties and investigates whether progress in word finding following 1:1 semantic intervention is greater than progress during a baseline period as measured by a standardised test. Methods Twenty-five college-aged students (20 males and 5 females) with Language Disorder and Word-Finding Difficulties (aged 16;4–18;4 years) participated in a single baseline design study comparing progress on the Test of Adolescent and Adult Word Finding during an intervention period with progress during a baseline period of equal length. Intervention was focused on semantics of different words from those in the Test of Adolescent and Adult Word Finding and was delivered 1:1 by the participants’ usual Speech and Language Therapist, 30 minutes per week, for eight weeks. Results The participants made significant progress in raw score on the Test of Adolescent and Adult Word Finding during both the baseline (d = 1.4) and intervention (d = 2.5) periods, but progress during the intervention period was significantly greater than during the baseline period (d = 1.16). Individual data showed reliable change for five participants during the baseline period and for 20 participants during the intervention period. At the start of the study, all participants had standard scores below 85, but after intervention, 10/25 participants scored above 85. Conclusions Four hours of semantic intervention led to significantly greater gains on a standardised test of word finding than during a baseline period of equal length in 16–19 year olds with Language Disorder and Word-Finding Difficulties. The words in the standardised test had not been included in the intervention, indicating generalised gains. Implications This study shows that intervention (at least for Word-Finding Difficulties) can be effective for this older age group of college-aged students with Language Disorder and therefore the effectiveness of Speech and Language Therapy services for this age group in a wider range of areas of language should also be investigated.","PeriodicalId":36716,"journal":{"name":"Autism and Developmental Language Impairments","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/2396941519870784","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47451358","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Reciprocal relationships between lexical and syntactic skills of children with Developmental Language Disorder and the role of executive functions 发展性语言障碍儿童词汇和句法技能与执行功能的相互关系
Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2019-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/2396941519863984
E. Blom, T. Boerma
Background and aims Recent research indicates that children with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) often also score lower than their peers with typical development (TD) on tasks testing nonverbal executive functioning (EF). This study investigated whether there is evidence that children with DLD use linguistic and EF resources to support their lexical and syntactic development. Three questions were addressed: (1) How do children with DLD develop in the domains of lexicon and syntax, and how does their development compare to TD controls? (2) To what extent do children with DLD show reciprocal relations between lexical and syntactic skills, and how does this compare to TD controls? (3) Is EF ability related to DLD children’s lexical and syntactic skills, and how does this compare to TD controls? Methods Data from 117 children (NDLD = 78; NTD = 39) were collected three times with yearly intervals. At time 1, the children were 5 or 6 years old. Standardized receptive vocabulary and sentence repetition tests measured lexicon and syntax, respectively. Nonverbal EF tasks tested selective attention, interference control and working memory. Cross-lagged analyses were conducted to determine the direction of relationships. Results Both groups showed stable lexical and syntactic growth. In children with DLD, but not in TD controls, syntactic skills predicted lexical skills. In the DLD group, EF predicted lexical skills. Reversely, in the TD group, lexical skills predicted EF. Conclusions The results of this study are consistent with the hypothesis that the lexical development of children with DLD is supported by both their verbal abilities in the domain of syntax and their nonverbal EF abilities. Implications Interventions that improve the syntactic and EF abilities of children with DLD may have spreading effects and positively impact on word learning by children with DLD.
背景和目的最近的研究表明,患有发展性语言障碍(DLD)的儿童在测试非语言执行功能(EF)的任务中的得分通常也低于具有典型发展(TD)的同龄人。本研究调查了是否有证据表明DLD儿童使用语言和EF资源来支持他们的词汇和句法发展。研究人员提出了三个问题:(1)DLD儿童在词汇和句法领域的发展情况如何,与TD对照组相比,他们的发展情况怎么样?(2) DLD儿童在多大程度上表现出词汇和句法技能之间的相互关系,与TD对照组相比如何?(3) EF能力与DLD儿童的词汇和句法技能有关吗?与TD对照相比如何?方法117例儿童(NDLD)的临床资料 = 78;NTD = 39)每年收集三次。在第1次实验中,孩子们都是5或6岁。标准化的接受性词汇和句子重复测试分别测量词汇和句法。非言语EF任务测试了选择性注意力、干扰控制和工作记忆。进行交叉滞后分析以确定关系的方向。结果两组在词汇和句法方面均表现出稳定的增长。在DLD儿童中,但在TD对照组中,句法技能预测词汇技能。在DLD组中,EF预测词汇技能。相反,在TD组中,词汇技能预测EF。结论本研究的结果与以下假设一致:DLD儿童的词汇发展受到其句法领域的言语能力和非言语EF能力的支持。含义改善DLD儿童句法和EF能力的干预措施可能具有传播效应,并对DLD儿童的单词学习产生积极影响。
{"title":"Reciprocal relationships between lexical and syntactic skills of children with Developmental Language Disorder and the role of executive functions","authors":"E. Blom, T. Boerma","doi":"10.1177/2396941519863984","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941519863984","url":null,"abstract":"Background and aims Recent research indicates that children with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) often also score lower than their peers with typical development (TD) on tasks testing nonverbal executive functioning (EF). This study investigated whether there is evidence that children with DLD use linguistic and EF resources to support their lexical and syntactic development. Three questions were addressed: (1) How do children with DLD develop in the domains of lexicon and syntax, and how does their development compare to TD controls? (2) To what extent do children with DLD show reciprocal relations between lexical and syntactic skills, and how does this compare to TD controls? (3) Is EF ability related to DLD children’s lexical and syntactic skills, and how does this compare to TD controls? Methods Data from 117 children (NDLD = 78; NTD = 39) were collected three times with yearly intervals. At time 1, the children were 5 or 6 years old. Standardized receptive vocabulary and sentence repetition tests measured lexicon and syntax, respectively. Nonverbal EF tasks tested selective attention, interference control and working memory. Cross-lagged analyses were conducted to determine the direction of relationships. Results Both groups showed stable lexical and syntactic growth. In children with DLD, but not in TD controls, syntactic skills predicted lexical skills. In the DLD group, EF predicted lexical skills. Reversely, in the TD group, lexical skills predicted EF. Conclusions The results of this study are consistent with the hypothesis that the lexical development of children with DLD is supported by both their verbal abilities in the domain of syntax and their nonverbal EF abilities. Implications Interventions that improve the syntactic and EF abilities of children with DLD may have spreading effects and positively impact on word learning by children with DLD.","PeriodicalId":36716,"journal":{"name":"Autism and Developmental Language Impairments","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/2396941519863984","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44886261","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
Comprehension of novel metaphor in young children with Developmental Language Disorder 幼儿发展性语言障碍对小说隐喻的理解
Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2018-12-01 DOI: 10.1177/2396941518817229
Daniela Bühler, A. Perovic, N. Pouscoulous
Background and aims Difficulties with aspects of morphosyntax, phonology and/or vocabulary are the hallmark of Development Language Disorder (DLD). Yet, little is known about the linguistic-pragmatic abilities of young children with DLD. Previous studies suggest that children with DLD are experiencing difficulties with idioms, sayings and slang expressions, often interpreting them in a literal or unconventional fashion. However, it is unclear whether this is caused by difficulties to make pragmatic inferences in general or whether it stems from their semantic abilities. We therefore investigated novel metaphor understanding in young children with and without DLD. Methods We assessed novel metaphor comprehension using a reference assignment task with 15 children with DLD diagnoses (ages 42–49 months) as well as typically developing peers matched on chronological age (n = 15) and on language (n = 15). Results Children with DLD performed worse than their age-matched peers but in a comparable manner to the (younger) language-matched typically developing children. Performance was not related to non-verbal intelligence in the children with DLD. Conclusion The findings indicate that young children with DLD have difficulties with metaphor comprehension but also suggest that these difficulties are in line with their general language difficulties and linked to their overall linguistic competence rather than reflecting additional specific issues with deriving pragmatic inferences. Implications Our study adds to a growing body of literature showing that children with low language abilities are also likely to display more difficulties in understanding figurative language independently of any other symptomatology of their clinical diagnosis. It also supports the argument that deficits in the pragmatic domain are a secondary impairment rather than a core deficit in children with DLD. Nonetheless, children with DLD do show difficulties in understanding metaphors. Understanding figurative language is necessary for everyday communication and should therefore be targeted alongside traditional treatments by clinicians treating children with DLD.
背景和目的形态语法、音系和/或词汇方面的困难是发展性语言障碍(DLD)的标志。然而,人们对DLD幼儿的语言语用能力知之甚少。先前的研究表明,患有DLD的儿童在理解习语、谚语和俚语方面存在困难,他们经常按照字面意思或非传统的方式来解释它们。然而,目前尚不清楚这是由于普遍的语用推理困难造成的,还是源于他们的语义能力。因此,我们研究了有和没有DLD的幼儿的新隐喻理解。方法对15名被诊断为DLD的儿童(42-49个月)以及在实足年龄(n = 15)和语言(n = 15)上匹配的正常发展的同龄人进行参考分配任务,评估他们的新隐喻理解能力。结果DLD儿童的表现比同龄儿童差,但与(更年轻的)语言匹配的典型发育儿童的表现相当。DLD患儿的表现与非语言智力无关。结论研究结果表明,DLD幼儿在隐喻理解方面存在困难,但这些困难与他们的一般语言困难一致,并与他们的整体语言能力有关,而不是反映出他们在语用推理方面存在额外的具体问题。我们的研究增加了越来越多的文献表明,低语言能力的儿童在理解比喻语言方面也可能表现出更多的困难,而独立于他们的临床诊断的任何其他症状。它还支持了这样一种观点,即语用领域的缺陷是DLD儿童的次要缺陷,而不是核心缺陷。尽管如此,患有DLD的儿童在理解隐喻方面确实表现出困难。理解比喻性语言对于日常交流是必要的,因此临床医生在治疗DLD儿童时应将其作为传统治疗的目标。
{"title":"Comprehension of novel metaphor in young children with Developmental Language Disorder","authors":"Daniela Bühler, A. Perovic, N. Pouscoulous","doi":"10.1177/2396941518817229","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941518817229","url":null,"abstract":"Background and aims Difficulties with aspects of morphosyntax, phonology and/or vocabulary are the hallmark of Development Language Disorder (DLD). Yet, little is known about the linguistic-pragmatic abilities of young children with DLD. Previous studies suggest that children with DLD are experiencing difficulties with idioms, sayings and slang expressions, often interpreting them in a literal or unconventional fashion. However, it is unclear whether this is caused by difficulties to make pragmatic inferences in general or whether it stems from their semantic abilities. We therefore investigated novel metaphor understanding in young children with and without DLD. Methods We assessed novel metaphor comprehension using a reference assignment task with 15 children with DLD diagnoses (ages 42–49 months) as well as typically developing peers matched on chronological age (n = 15) and on language (n = 15). Results Children with DLD performed worse than their age-matched peers but in a comparable manner to the (younger) language-matched typically developing children. Performance was not related to non-verbal intelligence in the children with DLD. Conclusion The findings indicate that young children with DLD have difficulties with metaphor comprehension but also suggest that these difficulties are in line with their general language difficulties and linked to their overall linguistic competence rather than reflecting additional specific issues with deriving pragmatic inferences. Implications Our study adds to a growing body of literature showing that children with low language abilities are also likely to display more difficulties in understanding figurative language independently of any other symptomatology of their clinical diagnosis. It also supports the argument that deficits in the pragmatic domain are a secondary impairment rather than a core deficit in children with DLD. Nonetheless, children with DLD do show difficulties in understanding metaphors. Understanding figurative language is necessary for everyday communication and should therefore be targeted alongside traditional treatments by clinicians treating children with DLD.","PeriodicalId":36716,"journal":{"name":"Autism and Developmental Language Impairments","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/2396941518817229","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43976798","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Systematic review of facilitated communication 2014–2018 finds no new evidence that messages delivered using facilitated communication are authored by the person with disability 对2014-2018年促进沟通的系统审查发现,没有新的证据表明使用促进沟通传递的信息是由残疾人撰写的
Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2018-12-01 DOI: 10.1177/2396941518821570
B. Hemsley, Lucy Bryant, R. Schlosser, H. Shane, R. Lang, D. Paul, M. Banajee, M. Ireland
Background and aims Facilitated Communication (FC) is a technique that involves a person with a disability pointing to letters, pictures, or objects on a keyboard or on a communication board, typically with physical support from a “facilitator”. Proponents claim that FC reveals previously undetected literacy and communication skills in people with communication disability. However, systematic reviews conducted up to 2014 reveal no evidence that the messages generated using FC are authored by the person with a disability. This study aimed to conduct a systematic review of the literature on FC published between 2014 and 2018 to inform the 2018 update of the 1995 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Position Statement on FC. Method A systematic search was undertaken to locate articles about FC in English published in the peer reviewed literature since 2014; and to classify these according to the study design for analysis. Studies meeting the inclusion criteria were classified according to four categories of evidence: (a) quantitative experimental data pertaining to authorship, (b) quantitative descriptive data on messages produced using FC, (c) qualitative data, or (d) commentary material on FC. Main contribution In total, 18 studies met the inclusion criteria. There were no new empirical studies and no new descriptive quantitative studies addressing the authorship of messages delivered using FC. Three new qualitative studies qualified for inclusion; these did not first establish authorship. Of the 15 new commentary papers on FC located, 14 were critical and one was non-critical. The results could be used to inform the development or update of current position statements on FC held locally, nationally, and globally. Conclusion There are no new studies on authorship and there remains no evidence that FC is a valid form of communication for individuals with severe communication disabilities. There continue to be no studies available demonstrating that individuals with communication disabilities are the authors of the messages generated using FC. Furthermore, there is substantial peer-reviewed literature that is critical of FC and warns against its use. Implications FC continues to be contested in high profile court cases and its use promoted in school settings and workshops at university campuses in the US. Our empty systematic review will influence both clinical practice and future clinical guidance; most immediately the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Position Statement on FC and any future guidance issued by the 19 associations worldwide with positions against FC.
背景和目的促进沟通(FC)是一种技术,涉及残疾人指着键盘或交流板上的字母、图片或物体,通常需要“促进者”的物理支持。支持者声称,FC揭示了以前未被发现的沟通障碍患者的读写能力和沟通技能。然而,截至2014年进行的系统审查显示,没有证据表明使用FC生成的信息是由残疾人撰写的。本研究旨在对2014年至2018年间发表的关于FC的文献进行系统回顾,为1995年美国语言听力协会关于FC的立场声明的2018年更新提供信息。方法系统检索2014年以来同行评议文献中有关FC的英文文章;并根据研究设计对其进行分类分析。符合纳入标准的研究根据四类证据进行分类:(a)与作者有关的定量实验数据,(b)使用FC产生的信息的定量描述性数据,(c)定性数据,或(d) FC评论材料。总共有18项研究符合纳入标准。没有新的实证研究,也没有新的描述性定量研究来解决使用FC传递的信息的作者问题。三个新的定性研究符合纳入条件;这些并没有首先确立作者身份。在15篇关于FC的新评论论文中,14篇是批判性的,1篇是非批判性的。研究结果可用于制定或更新当前在地方、国家和全球举行的FC立场声明。结论没有关于作者身份的新研究,也没有证据表明FC是严重沟通障碍患者有效的沟通方式。目前还没有研究表明有交流障碍的人是使用FC生成的信息的作者。此外,有大量的同行评议文献对FC持批评态度,并警告不要使用它。影响FC继续在备受瞩目的法庭案件中受到争议,并在美国的学校环境和大学校园的研讨会中推广使用。我们的空白系统评价将影响临床实践和未来的临床指导;最直接的是美国语言听力协会对FC的立场声明,以及全球19个反对FC的协会未来发布的任何指导意见。
{"title":"Systematic review of facilitated communication 2014–2018 finds no new evidence that messages delivered using facilitated communication are authored by the person with disability","authors":"B. Hemsley, Lucy Bryant, R. Schlosser, H. Shane, R. Lang, D. Paul, M. Banajee, M. Ireland","doi":"10.1177/2396941518821570","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941518821570","url":null,"abstract":"Background and aims Facilitated Communication (FC) is a technique that involves a person with a disability pointing to letters, pictures, or objects on a keyboard or on a communication board, typically with physical support from a “facilitator”. Proponents claim that FC reveals previously undetected literacy and communication skills in people with communication disability. However, systematic reviews conducted up to 2014 reveal no evidence that the messages generated using FC are authored by the person with a disability. This study aimed to conduct a systematic review of the literature on FC published between 2014 and 2018 to inform the 2018 update of the 1995 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Position Statement on FC. Method A systematic search was undertaken to locate articles about FC in English published in the peer reviewed literature since 2014; and to classify these according to the study design for analysis. Studies meeting the inclusion criteria were classified according to four categories of evidence: (a) quantitative experimental data pertaining to authorship, (b) quantitative descriptive data on messages produced using FC, (c) qualitative data, or (d) commentary material on FC. Main contribution In total, 18 studies met the inclusion criteria. There were no new empirical studies and no new descriptive quantitative studies addressing the authorship of messages delivered using FC. Three new qualitative studies qualified for inclusion; these did not first establish authorship. Of the 15 new commentary papers on FC located, 14 were critical and one was non-critical. The results could be used to inform the development or update of current position statements on FC held locally, nationally, and globally. Conclusion There are no new studies on authorship and there remains no evidence that FC is a valid form of communication for individuals with severe communication disabilities. There continue to be no studies available demonstrating that individuals with communication disabilities are the authors of the messages generated using FC. Furthermore, there is substantial peer-reviewed literature that is critical of FC and warns against its use. Implications FC continues to be contested in high profile court cases and its use promoted in school settings and workshops at university campuses in the US. Our empty systematic review will influence both clinical practice and future clinical guidance; most immediately the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Position Statement on FC and any future guidance issued by the 19 associations worldwide with positions against FC.","PeriodicalId":36716,"journal":{"name":"Autism and Developmental Language Impairments","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/2396941518821570","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42962665","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 22
Subskills associated with spelling ability in children with and without autism spectrum disorders 患有和不患有自闭症谱系障碍儿童的亚技能与拼写能力
Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2018-10-01 DOI: 10.1177/2396941518803807
B. Bailey, J. Arciuli
Background and aims Effective literacy instruction demands a clear understanding of the subskills that underpin children’s reading and writing abilities. Some previous research on reading has questioned whether the same subskills support literacy acquisition for typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorders. This study examined the subskills associated with spelling ability in a group of 20 children with ASD aged 5–12 years (ASD group). A group of 20 typically developing children matched for age and word spelling accuracy (TD group) provided comparative data. Methods Participants completed standardised assessments of vocabulary, phonological awareness, letter knowledge and word spelling. Errors produced in response to the word spelling assessment were analysed for evidence of phonological awareness. In addition, all spelling attempts were analysed for evidence of phonological, orthographic, and morphological awareness, ‘linguistic awareness’, using the Computerised Spelling Sensitivity System. Results Correlation and regression analyses showed statistically significant relationships between phonological awareness and word spelling accuracy for children in the ASD and TD groups. Spelling errors produced by both groups contained evidence of phonological awareness. Analysis of all spelling attempts showed that the overall level of linguistic awareness encoded by children in the ASD and TD groups was not significantly different. Conclusions These findings provide evidence that phonological awareness and other subskills support spelling in children with autism spectrum disorders as they do in typically developing children. Implications The similar spelling profiles exhibited by children with autism spectrum disorders and their typically developing peers suggest that these populations may benefit from literacy instruction that targets the same underpinning subskills.
背景和目的有效的识字教学需要清楚地了解支撑儿童阅读和写作能力的辅助技能。之前一些关于阅读的研究质疑,同样的亚技能是否支持典型发育中的儿童和自闭症谱系障碍儿童的识字能力。这项研究检测了20名5-12岁ASD儿童(ASD组)与拼写能力相关的亚技能。一个由20名年龄和单词拼写准确性匹配的典型发育中儿童组成的小组(TD组)提供了比较数据。方法参与者完成词汇、语音意识、字母知识和单词拼写的标准化评估。对单词拼写评估中产生的错误进行了分析,以寻找语音意识的证据。此外,使用计算机拼写敏感系统分析了所有拼写尝试的语音、拼写和形态意识,即“语言意识”的证据。结果相关和回归分析显示,ASD和TD组儿童的语音意识与单词拼写准确性之间存在统计学显著关系。两组人的拼写错误都包含了语音意识的证据。对所有拼写尝试的分析表明,ASD和TD组儿童的语言意识总体水平没有显著差异。结论这些发现提供了证据,证明语音意识和其他亚技能支持自闭症谱系障碍儿童的拼写,就像他们在典型发育中的儿童一样。含义自闭症谱系障碍儿童及其典型发育中的同龄人表现出的类似拼写特征表明,这些人群可能会从针对相同基础亚技能的识字教学中受益。
{"title":"Subskills associated with spelling ability in children with and without autism spectrum disorders","authors":"B. Bailey, J. Arciuli","doi":"10.1177/2396941518803807","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941518803807","url":null,"abstract":"Background and aims Effective literacy instruction demands a clear understanding of the subskills that underpin children’s reading and writing abilities. Some previous research on reading has questioned whether the same subskills support literacy acquisition for typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorders. This study examined the subskills associated with spelling ability in a group of 20 children with ASD aged 5–12 years (ASD group). A group of 20 typically developing children matched for age and word spelling accuracy (TD group) provided comparative data. Methods Participants completed standardised assessments of vocabulary, phonological awareness, letter knowledge and word spelling. Errors produced in response to the word spelling assessment were analysed for evidence of phonological awareness. In addition, all spelling attempts were analysed for evidence of phonological, orthographic, and morphological awareness, ‘linguistic awareness’, using the Computerised Spelling Sensitivity System. Results Correlation and regression analyses showed statistically significant relationships between phonological awareness and word spelling accuracy for children in the ASD and TD groups. Spelling errors produced by both groups contained evidence of phonological awareness. Analysis of all spelling attempts showed that the overall level of linguistic awareness encoded by children in the ASD and TD groups was not significantly different. Conclusions These findings provide evidence that phonological awareness and other subskills support spelling in children with autism spectrum disorders as they do in typically developing children. Implications The similar spelling profiles exhibited by children with autism spectrum disorders and their typically developing peers suggest that these populations may benefit from literacy instruction that targets the same underpinning subskills.","PeriodicalId":36716,"journal":{"name":"Autism and Developmental Language Impairments","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/2396941518803807","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48237064","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
An unconventional path to greater social-communication skills and independence for an adolescent on the autism spectrum 对于自闭症谱系的青少年来说,这是一条提高社交技能和独立性的非常规途径
Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2018-10-01 DOI: 10.1177/2396941518809611
D. Trembath, R. J. Bala, Joanne Tamblyn, Marleen F. Westerveld
Background In 2015, a father took his 14-year-old son who is on the autism spectrum on a six-month journey aimed to develop his son’s social-communication and independent living skills. The duo travelled across 10 countries, meeting people and practising these skills. This study examined their goals, motivations for, and outcomes of the journey. Method We used intrinsic case study methodology with mixed methods, including interviews with parents and professionals; analyses of filmed interactions between the son, his father and strangers during the journey; and descriptive analysis of parent-reported changes in their son’s participation at home, school and in the community using the Participation and Environment Measure – Children and Youth. Results Qualitative analysis of the interviews with parents and professionals revealed a set of insightful goals and motivations, focusing on creating an optimal environment for the son’s development. Parents reported increases in their son’s social-communication and independent living skills, but also unexpected changes in his perspective and self-belief. The former findings were consistent with those arising from video analysis, whereby social-pragmatic skills critical to good conversations (staying on topic, body position, eye contact) all increased over the course of the journey, while abrupt topic changes and conversational prompts reduced. Participation and inclusion across home, school and community settings all increased over the same period. Conclusion While this study makes no claims regarding causation, the findings indicate that the journey was associated with positive changes for the son and his parents, leading to greater expectations for, and progress towards, independence following the journey. Implications of the findings for supporting young people on the autism spectrum in regular community settings are discussed.
2015年,一位父亲带着他14岁的自闭症儿子进行了为期六个月的旅行,目的是培养儿子的社交和独立生活技能。两人走遍了10个国家,与人们见面并练习这些技巧。这项研究调查了他们的目标、动机和旅行的结果。方法本研究采用内在案例研究方法,包括对家长和专业人士的访谈;在旅途中,儿子、父亲和陌生人之间的互动分析;以及使用“参与和环境测量-儿童和青少年”对父母报告的儿子在家、学校和社区参与情况的变化进行描述性分析。结果对父母和专业人士的访谈进行定性分析,揭示了一系列深刻的目标和动机,重点是为儿子的发展创造最佳环境。父母们报告说,他们儿子的社交能力和独立生活能力有所提高,但他的观点和自信也发生了意想不到的变化。前一项研究结果与视频分析的结果一致,即社交实用技能(保持话题、身体姿势、眼神交流)在旅途中都有所提高,而突然的话题变化和对话提示则减少了。在同一时期,家庭、学校和社区环境中的参与和包容都有所增加。虽然这项研究没有提出因果关系,但研究结果表明,旅行与儿子和他的父母的积极变化有关,导致儿子对旅行后的独立有更大的期望,并朝着独立的方向发展。研究结果对在常规社区环境中支持自闭症谱系的年轻人的意义进行了讨论。
{"title":"An unconventional path to greater social-communication skills and independence for an adolescent on the autism spectrum","authors":"D. Trembath, R. J. Bala, Joanne Tamblyn, Marleen F. Westerveld","doi":"10.1177/2396941518809611","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941518809611","url":null,"abstract":"Background In 2015, a father took his 14-year-old son who is on the autism spectrum on a six-month journey aimed to develop his son’s social-communication and independent living skills. The duo travelled across 10 countries, meeting people and practising these skills. This study examined their goals, motivations for, and outcomes of the journey. Method We used intrinsic case study methodology with mixed methods, including interviews with parents and professionals; analyses of filmed interactions between the son, his father and strangers during the journey; and descriptive analysis of parent-reported changes in their son’s participation at home, school and in the community using the Participation and Environment Measure – Children and Youth. Results Qualitative analysis of the interviews with parents and professionals revealed a set of insightful goals and motivations, focusing on creating an optimal environment for the son’s development. Parents reported increases in their son’s social-communication and independent living skills, but also unexpected changes in his perspective and self-belief. The former findings were consistent with those arising from video analysis, whereby social-pragmatic skills critical to good conversations (staying on topic, body position, eye contact) all increased over the course of the journey, while abrupt topic changes and conversational prompts reduced. Participation and inclusion across home, school and community settings all increased over the same period. Conclusion While this study makes no claims regarding causation, the findings indicate that the journey was associated with positive changes for the son and his parents, leading to greater expectations for, and progress towards, independence following the journey. Implications of the findings for supporting young people on the autism spectrum in regular community settings are discussed.","PeriodicalId":36716,"journal":{"name":"Autism and Developmental Language Impairments","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/2396941518809611","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42474355","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pragmatic versus structural difficulties in the production of pronominal clitics in French-speaking children with autism spectrum disorder 自闭症谱系障碍法语儿童代词群产生的语用困难与结构困难
Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2018-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/2396941518799643
P. Prévost, Laurice Tuller, Racha Zebib, M. Barthez, J. Malvy, F. Bonnet-Brilhault
Background and aims Impaired production of third person accusative pronominal clitics is a signature of language impairment in French-speaking children. It has been found to be a prominent and persistent difficulty in children and adolescents with specific language impairment. Previous studies have reported that many children with autism spectrum disorder also have low performance on these clitics. However, it remains unclear whether these difficulties in children with autism spectrum disorder are due to structural language impairment or to pragmatic deficits. This is because pragmatics skills, notoriously weak in children with autism spectrum disorder, are also needed for appropriate use of pronouns. Use of pronouns without clear referents and difficulty with discourse pronouns (first and second person), which require taking into account the point of view of one’s interlocutor (perspective shifting), have frequently been reported for autism spectrum disorder. Methods We elicited production of nominative, reflexive and accusative third and first person pronominal clitics in 19 verbal children with autism spectrum disorder (aged 6–12, high and low functioning, with structural language impairment, or with normal language) and 19 age-matched children with specific language impairment. If pragmatics is behind difficulties on these elements, performance on first-person clitics would be expected to be worse than performance on third person clitics, since it requires perspective shifting. Furthermore, worse performance for first person clitics was expected in the children with autism spectrum disorder compared to the children with specific language impairment, since weak pragmatics is an integral part of impairment in the former, but not in the latter. More generally, different error patterns would be expected in the two groups, if the source of difficulty with clitics is different (a pragmatic deficit vs. a structural language deficit). Results Similar patterns of relative difficulties were found in the autism spectrum disorder language impairment and specific language impairment groups, with third person accusative clitics being produced at lower rates than first-person pronouns and error patterns being essentially identical. First-person pronouns did not pose particular difficulties in the children with autism spectrum disorder (language impairment or normal language) with respect to third-person pronouns or to the children with specific language impairment. Performance was not related to nonverbal intelligence in the autism spectrum disorder group. Conclusions The elicitation task used in this study included explicit instruction, and focus on perspective shifting (both visual and verbal), allowing for potential pragmatic effects to be controlled. Moreover, the task elicited a variety of types of clitics in morphosyntactic contexts of varying complexity, providing ample opportunities for employment of perspective shifting, which may have also curtail
背景和目的第三人称宾格代词群的产生障碍是法语儿童语言障碍的标志。研究发现,对于有特定语言障碍的儿童和青少年来说,这是一个突出而持久的困难。先前的研究报告称,许多患有自闭症谱系障碍的儿童在这些派系方面的表现也很低。然而,目前尚不清楚自闭症谱系障碍儿童的这些困难是由于结构性语言障碍还是语用缺陷。这是因为语用学技能在自闭症谱系障碍儿童中是出了名的薄弱,正确使用代词也需要语用学技能。使用没有明确指称的代词和难以使用话语代词(第一人称和第二人称),需要考虑对话者的观点(视角转换),这经常被报道为自闭症谱系障碍。方法我们在19名患有自闭症谱系障碍的言语儿童(6-12岁,高功能和低功能,有结构语言障碍或正常语言)和19名患有特定语言障碍的年龄匹配儿童中引发主格、反身和宾格第三人称和第一人称代词群的产生。如果语用学是这些元素困难的背后原因,那么第一人称派系的表现可能会比第三人称派系的差,因为这需要视角的转变。此外,与有特定语言障碍的儿童相比,自闭症谱系障碍儿童的第一人称clitics表现更差,因为弱语用学是前者障碍的一个组成部分,但后者不是。更普遍地说,如果派系的困难来源不同(语用缺陷与结构语言缺陷),那么这两组人的错误模式会有所不同。结果自闭症谱系障碍语言障碍组和特定语言障碍组的相对困难模式相似,第三人称宾格群的产生率低于第一人称代词,错误模式基本相同。相对于第三人称代词或有特定语言障碍的儿童,第一人称代词在自闭症谱系障碍(语言障碍或正常语言)儿童中没有造成特别的困难。自闭症谱系障碍组的表现与非语言智力无关。结论本研究中使用的启发任务包括明确的指导,并侧重于视角转换(视觉和言语),从而控制潜在的语用效果。此外,这项任务在不同复杂性的形态句法语境中引发了各种类型的派系,为视角转换提供了充足的机会,这也可能限制了第三人称对第一人称的坚持。任务的这些特性使得儿童在使用第三人称宾格群语时遇到的困难具有明确的语法性质。自闭症谱系障碍儿童结构语言能力的评估需要仔细考虑任务需求。通过明确和突出任务的语用要求,可以规避语用能力对结构语言表现的影响。筛选出这种对结构语言表现的潜在影响,对于理解自闭症谱系障碍儿童的语言特征,从而了解哪些儿童可以从哪种语言干预中受益至关重要。
{"title":"Pragmatic versus structural difficulties in the production of pronominal clitics in French-speaking children with autism spectrum disorder","authors":"P. Prévost, Laurice Tuller, Racha Zebib, M. Barthez, J. Malvy, F. Bonnet-Brilhault","doi":"10.1177/2396941518799643","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941518799643","url":null,"abstract":"Background and aims Impaired production of third person accusative pronominal clitics is a signature of language impairment in French-speaking children. It has been found to be a prominent and persistent difficulty in children and adolescents with specific language impairment. Previous studies have reported that many children with autism spectrum disorder also have low performance on these clitics. However, it remains unclear whether these difficulties in children with autism spectrum disorder are due to structural language impairment or to pragmatic deficits. This is because pragmatics skills, notoriously weak in children with autism spectrum disorder, are also needed for appropriate use of pronouns. Use of pronouns without clear referents and difficulty with discourse pronouns (first and second person), which require taking into account the point of view of one’s interlocutor (perspective shifting), have frequently been reported for autism spectrum disorder. Methods We elicited production of nominative, reflexive and accusative third and first person pronominal clitics in 19 verbal children with autism spectrum disorder (aged 6–12, high and low functioning, with structural language impairment, or with normal language) and 19 age-matched children with specific language impairment. If pragmatics is behind difficulties on these elements, performance on first-person clitics would be expected to be worse than performance on third person clitics, since it requires perspective shifting. Furthermore, worse performance for first person clitics was expected in the children with autism spectrum disorder compared to the children with specific language impairment, since weak pragmatics is an integral part of impairment in the former, but not in the latter. More generally, different error patterns would be expected in the two groups, if the source of difficulty with clitics is different (a pragmatic deficit vs. a structural language deficit). Results Similar patterns of relative difficulties were found in the autism spectrum disorder language impairment and specific language impairment groups, with third person accusative clitics being produced at lower rates than first-person pronouns and error patterns being essentially identical. First-person pronouns did not pose particular difficulties in the children with autism spectrum disorder (language impairment or normal language) with respect to third-person pronouns or to the children with specific language impairment. Performance was not related to nonverbal intelligence in the autism spectrum disorder group. Conclusions The elicitation task used in this study included explicit instruction, and focus on perspective shifting (both visual and verbal), allowing for potential pragmatic effects to be controlled. Moreover, the task elicited a variety of types of clitics in morphosyntactic contexts of varying complexity, providing ample opportunities for employment of perspective shifting, which may have also curtail","PeriodicalId":36716,"journal":{"name":"Autism and Developmental Language Impairments","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/2396941518799643","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48879995","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 15
Everyday executive function and adaptive skills in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: Cross-sectional developmental trajectories 自闭症谱系障碍儿童和青少年的日常执行功能和适应技能:横断面发展轨迹
Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2018-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/2396941518800775
Evangelia-Chrysanthi Kouklari, S. Tsermentseli, C. Monks
Background and aims The development of Executive Function in Autism Spectrum Disorder has been investigated using mainly performance-based executive function measures. Less is known about the development of everyday executive function skills. The present study aimed to identify the developmental patterns of everyday executive function of children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder compared to neurotypical controls. The association between executive function and adaptive skills was also investigated. Methods The present study used a cross-sectional developmental trajectory approach and data were collected from 57 children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder, matched to 63 controls of the same age (7–15 years). Results Results showed age-related performance declines in most everyday executive function domains (e.g. inhibition, working memory, planning) in autism spectrum disorder, whereas for executive function emotional control and shift, non-significant differences emerged across age in autism spectrum disorder. Everyday executive function predicted adaptive skills over and above age and IQ, in participants overall. Conclusions and implications These results suggest that several everyday executive function problems increase in adolescence in autism spectrum disorder and that these everyday executive function developmental patterns deviate to a great extent from those of typical development. Shedding more light on the developmental course of all types of executive function processes as well as their association with crucial social outcomes in autism spectrum disorder could contribute to a better theoretical understanding of the heterogeneity of the neurocognitive development in autism spectrum disorder.
背景与目的研究自闭症谱系障碍患者执行功能的发展,主要采用基于绩效的执行功能测量方法。人们对日常执行功能技能的发展知之甚少。本研究旨在确定自闭症谱系障碍儿童和青少年的日常执行功能的发展模式与神经正常对照。执行功能和适应技能之间的关系也被调查。方法采用横断面发育轨迹分析方法,收集57例自闭症谱系障碍儿童和青少年的数据,并与63例同年龄(7-15岁)的对照组相匹配。结果自闭症谱系障碍患者在大多数日常执行功能领域(如抑制、工作记忆、计划)的表现与年龄相关,而在执行功能领域(如情绪控制和转移)的表现在不同年龄间差异不显著。总的来说,日常执行功能预测了参与者在年龄和智商之上的适应能力。这些结果表明自闭症谱系障碍青少年的日常执行功能问题有所增加,这些日常执行功能的发展模式在很大程度上偏离了典型的发展模式。了解自闭症谱系障碍中各类执行功能过程的发展过程及其与关键社会结果的关联,有助于从理论上更好地理解自闭症谱系障碍神经认知发展的异质性。
{"title":"Everyday executive function and adaptive skills in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: Cross-sectional developmental trajectories","authors":"Evangelia-Chrysanthi Kouklari, S. Tsermentseli, C. Monks","doi":"10.1177/2396941518800775","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941518800775","url":null,"abstract":"Background and aims The development of Executive Function in Autism Spectrum Disorder has been investigated using mainly performance-based executive function measures. Less is known about the development of everyday executive function skills. The present study aimed to identify the developmental patterns of everyday executive function of children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder compared to neurotypical controls. The association between executive function and adaptive skills was also investigated. Methods The present study used a cross-sectional developmental trajectory approach and data were collected from 57 children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder, matched to 63 controls of the same age (7–15 years). Results Results showed age-related performance declines in most everyday executive function domains (e.g. inhibition, working memory, planning) in autism spectrum disorder, whereas for executive function emotional control and shift, non-significant differences emerged across age in autism spectrum disorder. Everyday executive function predicted adaptive skills over and above age and IQ, in participants overall. Conclusions and implications These results suggest that several everyday executive function problems increase in adolescence in autism spectrum disorder and that these everyday executive function developmental patterns deviate to a great extent from those of typical development. Shedding more light on the developmental course of all types of executive function processes as well as their association with crucial social outcomes in autism spectrum disorder could contribute to a better theoretical understanding of the heterogeneity of the neurocognitive development in autism spectrum disorder.","PeriodicalId":36716,"journal":{"name":"Autism and Developmental Language Impairments","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/2396941518800775","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44296327","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
‘I felt closed in and like I couldn’t breathe’: A qualitative study exploring the mainstream educational experiences of autistic young people “我觉得自己被封闭了,好像无法呼吸”:一项探索自闭症年轻人主流教育经历的定性研究
Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2018-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/2396941518804407
Craig Goodall
Background and aim More autistic young people are being educated in mainstream schools. While existing research suggests that the outcomes of mainstream inclusion are mixed for autistic young people, few studies have examined their views and experiences directly. This article discusses the educational experiences of 12 autistic young people (aged 11–17 years) from their perspectives and how education could be improved to better support the others with autism. Methods A flexible qualitative participatory approach was used which incorporated a range of methods, including individual semi-structured interviews, diamond ranking activities and draw-and-tell activities. A Children’s Research Advisory Group (CRAG) advised on the methods used and matters to be explored. Results The young people offered insights into how education has been for them in mainstream school, mostly negative, but with islets of positive experience. Several described themselves as being socially, emotionally and physically isolated from peers, with loneliness and bullying experienced by some. Participants felt unsupported and misunderstood by teachers within a social and sensory environment that was antithetical to their needs. Some spoke of the dread they felt before and during school and the negative impact their experiences in mainstream has had on their wellbeing. Many participants suggested simple strategies and curriculum adaptations that they felt would have helped make their time at mainstream more successful. These include having more breaks, smaller class sizes, less homework, instructions broken down, safe places to use when anxious and teachers who listen to their concerns and take account of their needs. In short they want to be understood, supported and included. Conclusions There exists scope to better support autistic young people in mainstream education, as evidenced by the literature and the participants in this paper. The participants demonstrate that mainstream is not working for all and that changes, such as smaller class sizes, flexible pedagogy and understanding could improve education for autistic learners. Implications Autistic young people can and should be central to the discussion on school improvement. They also show that being academically able for mainstream school should not be the only aspect when deciding on the suitability of mainstream school for meeting their needs. The young person’s social and emotional wellbeing must be considered to ensure they can flourish, and not flounder.
背景和目的越来越多的自闭症年轻人在主流学校接受教育。虽然现有研究表明,主流包容的结果对自闭症年轻人来说是喜忧参半的,但很少有研究直接研究他们的观点和经历。本文从12名自闭症青年(11-17岁)的角度讨论了他们的教育经历,以及如何改进教育以更好地支持其他自闭症患者。方法采用一种灵活的定性参与方法,其中包括一系列方法,包括个人半结构化访谈、钻石排名活动和绘画和讲述活动。儿童研究咨询小组(CRAG)就所使用的方法和需要探索的事项提供建议。结果年轻人对主流学校的教育情况有了深刻的了解,大多是消极的,但也有积极的经验。一些人形容自己在社交、情感和身体上与同龄人隔绝,有些人经历了孤独和欺凌。参与者在与他们的需求相反的社会和感官环境中感到教师的支持和误解。一些人谈到了他们在上学前和上学期间感受到的恐惧,以及他们在主流社会的经历对他们的健康产生的负面影响。许多参与者提出了一些简单的策略和课程调整,他们认为这些策略和调整有助于使他们在主流社会的时间更加成功。这些措施包括有更多的休息时间,减少班级规模,减少家庭作业,分解说明,在焦虑时使用安全的地方,以及倾听他们的担忧并考虑他们需求的老师。简而言之,他们希望得到理解、支持和包容。结论正如文献和本文参与者所证明的那样,在主流教育中有更好地支持自闭症青年的空间。参与者证明,主流并非对所有人都有效,班级规模缩小、灵活的教学法和理解等变化可以改善自闭症学习者的教育。影响自闭症年轻人可以而且应该成为学校改进讨论的核心。他们还表明,在决定主流学校是否适合满足他们的需求时,学术能力不应该是唯一的方面。必须考虑年轻人的社会和情感健康,以确保他们能够蓬勃发展,而不是陷入困境。
{"title":"‘I felt closed in and like I couldn’t breathe’: A qualitative study exploring the mainstream educational experiences of autistic young people","authors":"Craig Goodall","doi":"10.1177/2396941518804407","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941518804407","url":null,"abstract":"Background and aim More autistic young people are being educated in mainstream schools. While existing research suggests that the outcomes of mainstream inclusion are mixed for autistic young people, few studies have examined their views and experiences directly. This article discusses the educational experiences of 12 autistic young people (aged 11–17 years) from their perspectives and how education could be improved to better support the others with autism. Methods A flexible qualitative participatory approach was used which incorporated a range of methods, including individual semi-structured interviews, diamond ranking activities and draw-and-tell activities. A Children’s Research Advisory Group (CRAG) advised on the methods used and matters to be explored. Results The young people offered insights into how education has been for them in mainstream school, mostly negative, but with islets of positive experience. Several described themselves as being socially, emotionally and physically isolated from peers, with loneliness and bullying experienced by some. Participants felt unsupported and misunderstood by teachers within a social and sensory environment that was antithetical to their needs. Some spoke of the dread they felt before and during school and the negative impact their experiences in mainstream has had on their wellbeing. Many participants suggested simple strategies and curriculum adaptations that they felt would have helped make their time at mainstream more successful. These include having more breaks, smaller class sizes, less homework, instructions broken down, safe places to use when anxious and teachers who listen to their concerns and take account of their needs. In short they want to be understood, supported and included. Conclusions There exists scope to better support autistic young people in mainstream education, as evidenced by the literature and the participants in this paper. The participants demonstrate that mainstream is not working for all and that changes, such as smaller class sizes, flexible pedagogy and understanding could improve education for autistic learners. Implications Autistic young people can and should be central to the discussion on school improvement. They also show that being academically able for mainstream school should not be the only aspect when deciding on the suitability of mainstream school for meeting their needs. The young person’s social and emotional wellbeing must be considered to ensure they can flourish, and not flounder.","PeriodicalId":36716,"journal":{"name":"Autism and Developmental Language Impairments","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/2396941518804407","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49482341","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 58
A systematic review of the comparative pragmatic differences in conversational skills of individuals with autism 自闭症患者会话技能比较语用差异的系统评价
Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2018-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/2396941518803806
Cheong Ying Sng, M. Carter, J. Stephenson
Background and aims: Given that problems with social interaction and communication are defining features of autism spectrum disorder, it stands to reason that individuals with autism spectrum disorder have difficulties in conversation. There is a growing body of research on the conversation skills of individuals with autism spectrum disorder, including research conducted to compare these skills to those of typically developing individuals and those with other disabilities. Such comparisons may offer insight into the extent to which conversational skills may be deficient and whether deficits are unique to a particular diagnostic group. Main contribution: This review provides an examination of comparative studies of pragmatic aspects of conversation that included individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Only a small number of consistent findings emerged from the analysis. Groups with autism spectrum disorder find it difficult to stay on topic and provide novel, relevant information. They also tend to perseverate more and initiate and respond less during conversation but, contrary to expectation, similar numbers of turns were offered to partners, and there was little difference in the way communication breakdowns were repaired or clarified. There was a contradictory finding on the use of eye gaze. Conclusions and implications: Some consistent findings were reported but overall, fewer than expected between group differences were found. The fragmented nature of the research and inconsistent operational definitions of variables measured made analysis problematic. Further research and replication of studies is recommended before definitive conclusions can be drawn.
背景和目的:鉴于社交和沟通问题是自闭症谱系障碍的特征,自闭症谱系疾病患者在对话中存在困难是理所当然的。越来越多的研究对自闭症谱系障碍患者的谈话技能进行了研究,包括将这些技能与典型的发育中个体和其他残疾者的对话技能进行比较。这样的比较可以深入了解会话技能的缺陷程度,以及缺陷是否是特定诊断组特有的。主要贡献:这篇综述对谈话的语用方面进行了比较研究,其中包括自闭症谱系障碍患者。分析中只有少量一致的发现。患有自闭症谱系障碍的群体发现很难专注于主题并提供新颖、相关的信息。他们在谈话中也倾向于更多地坚持,更少地主动和回应,但与预期相反,向伴侣提供的轮次数量相似,在修复或澄清沟通障碍的方式上几乎没有差异。关于眼睛凝视的使用,有一个矛盾的发现。结论和意义:报告了一些一致的发现,但总体而言,发现的组间差异小于预期。研究的分散性和测量变量的操作定义不一致,使得分析存在问题。在得出明确结论之前,建议进行进一步的研究和重复研究。
{"title":"A systematic review of the comparative pragmatic differences in conversational skills of individuals with autism","authors":"Cheong Ying Sng, M. Carter, J. Stephenson","doi":"10.1177/2396941518803806","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941518803806","url":null,"abstract":"Background and aims: Given that problems with social interaction and communication are defining features of autism spectrum disorder, it stands to reason that individuals with autism spectrum disorder have difficulties in conversation. There is a growing body of research on the conversation skills of individuals with autism spectrum disorder, including research conducted to compare these skills to those of typically developing individuals and those with other disabilities. Such comparisons may offer insight into the extent to which conversational skills may be deficient and whether deficits are unique to a particular diagnostic group. Main contribution: This review provides an examination of comparative studies of pragmatic aspects of conversation that included individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Only a small number of consistent findings emerged from the analysis. Groups with autism spectrum disorder find it difficult to stay on topic and provide novel, relevant information. They also tend to perseverate more and initiate and respond less during conversation but, contrary to expectation, similar numbers of turns were offered to partners, and there was little difference in the way communication breakdowns were repaired or clarified. There was a contradictory finding on the use of eye gaze. Conclusions and implications: Some consistent findings were reported but overall, fewer than expected between group differences were found. The fragmented nature of the research and inconsistent operational definitions of variables measured made analysis problematic. Further research and replication of studies is recommended before definitive conclusions can be drawn.","PeriodicalId":36716,"journal":{"name":"Autism and Developmental Language Impairments","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/2396941518803806","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45091352","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 41
期刊
Autism and Developmental Language Impairments
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1