首页 > 最新文献

Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society最新文献

英文 中文
Masculine Defaults in Academic Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM) Fields. 学术科学、技术、工程和数学(STEM)领域的男性默认值。
1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/15291006231170829
Alexandra Garr-Schultz, Gregg A Muragishi, Therese Anne Mortejo, Sapna Cheryan
Ceci et al. (2023) provide a synthesis of research on the biases (or lack thereof) that women experience in tenure-track science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) academic fields. We expand on their analysis and suggest that in addition to differential treatment (e.g., women receiving lower teaching evaluation ratings than men), gender bias can take another form in masculine defaults. We argue that to achieve gender equity, we must identify and eliminate or balance many of the masculine defaults that pervade the culture of academic STEM fields. Below, we first define masculine defaults and distinguish them from differential treatment, discuss the need to examine masculine defaults, and consider how they are perpetuated within academic STEM fields. Next, we provide examples of masculine defaults prevalent in academic STEM fields across four levels of culture and how they may disadvantage women. Finally, we discuss recommendations for addressing masculine defaults in academic STEM fields.
{"title":"Masculine Defaults in Academic Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM) Fields.","authors":"Alexandra Garr-Schultz, Gregg A Muragishi, Therese Anne Mortejo, Sapna Cheryan","doi":"10.1177/15291006231170829","DOIUrl":"10.1177/15291006231170829","url":null,"abstract":"Ceci et al. (2023) provide a synthesis of research on the biases (or lack thereof) that women experience in tenure-track science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) academic fields. We expand on their analysis and suggest that in addition to differential treatment (e.g., women receiving lower teaching evaluation ratings than men), gender bias can take another form in masculine defaults. We argue that to achieve gender equity, we must identify and eliminate or balance many of the masculine defaults that pervade the culture of academic STEM fields. Below, we first define masculine defaults and distinguish them from differential treatment, discuss the need to examine masculine defaults, and consider how they are perpetuated within academic STEM fields. Next, we provide examples of masculine defaults prevalent in academic STEM fields across four levels of culture and how they may disadvantage women. Finally, we discuss recommendations for addressing masculine defaults in academic STEM fields.","PeriodicalId":37882,"journal":{"name":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","volume":"24 1","pages":"1-9"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10274226","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Call for Commentaries. 呼吁评论。
1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/15291006231187882
{"title":"Call for Commentaries.","authors":"","doi":"10.1177/15291006231187882","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/15291006231187882","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":37882,"journal":{"name":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","volume":"24 1","pages":"74"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10230098","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring Gender Bias in Six Key Domains of Academic Science: An Adversarial Collaboration. 在学术科学的六个关键领域探索性别偏见:一个对抗性的合作。
1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/15291006231163179
Stephen J Ceci, Shulamit Kahn, Wendy M Williams

We synthesized the vast, contradictory scholarly literature on gender bias in academic science from 2000 to 2020. In the most prestigious journals and media outlets, which influence many people's opinions about sexism, bias is frequently portrayed as an omnipresent factor limiting women's progress in the tenure-track academy. Claims and counterclaims regarding the presence or absence of sexism span a range of evaluation contexts. Our approach relied on a combination of meta-analysis and analytic dissection. We evaluated the empirical evidence for gender bias in six key contexts in the tenure-track academy: (a) tenure-track hiring, (b) grant funding, (c) teaching ratings, (d) journal acceptances, (e) salaries, and (f) recommendation letters. We also explored the gender gap in a seventh area, journal productivity, because it can moderate bias in other contexts. We focused on these specific domains, in which sexism has most often been alleged to be pervasive, because they represent important types of evaluation, and the extensive research corpus within these domains provides sufficient quantitative data for comprehensive analysis. Contrary to the omnipresent claims of sexism in these domains appearing in top journals and the media, our findings show that tenure-track women are at parity with tenure-track men in three domains (grant funding, journal acceptances, and recommendation letters) and are advantaged over men in a fourth domain (hiring). For teaching ratings and salaries, we found evidence of bias against women; although gender gaps in salary were much smaller than often claimed, they were nevertheless concerning. Even in the four domains in which we failed to find evidence of sexism disadvantaging women, we nevertheless acknowledge that broad societal structural factors may still impede women's advancement in academic science. Given the substantial resources directed toward reducing gender bias in academic science, it is imperative to develop a clear understanding of when and where such efforts are justified and of how resources can best be directed to mitigate sexism when and where it exists.

我们综合了从2000年到2020年关于学术科学中性别偏见的大量相互矛盾的学术文献。在影响许多人对性别歧视看法的最负盛名的期刊和媒体中,偏见经常被描绘成一个无处不在的因素,限制了女性在终身教职方面的进步。关于存在或不存在性别歧视的主张和反诉跨越了一系列评估背景。我们的方法依赖于荟萃分析和分析性解剖的结合。我们评估了终身教职学院中六个关键背景下性别偏见的经验证据:(a)终身教职招聘,(b)拨款,(c)教学评级,(d)期刊接受,(e)工资和(f)推荐信。我们还探讨了第七个领域的性别差异,即期刊生产率,因为它可以在其他情况下缓和偏见。我们专注于这些特定的领域,在这些领域中,性别歧视最常被认为是普遍存在的,因为它们代表了重要的评估类型,而这些领域中广泛的研究语料库为全面分析提供了足够的定量数据。与这些领域中出现在顶级期刊和媒体上的无所不在的性别歧视主张相反,我们的研究结果表明,终身教职女性在三个领域(资助、期刊接受和推荐信)与终身教职男性平等,在第四个领域(招聘)比男性更有优势。在教学评分和工资方面,我们发现了对女性存在偏见的证据;虽然薪金方面的性别差距比通常声称的要小得多,但仍然令人关切。即使在我们没有发现不利于女性的性别歧视证据的四个领域,我们也承认,广泛的社会结构因素可能仍然阻碍女性在学术科学领域的进步。考虑到用于减少学术科学中的性别偏见的大量资源,必须清楚地了解这种努力何时何地是合理的,以及如何最好地将资源用于减轻何时何地存在的性别歧视。
{"title":"Exploring Gender Bias in Six Key Domains of Academic Science: An Adversarial Collaboration.","authors":"Stephen J Ceci,&nbsp;Shulamit Kahn,&nbsp;Wendy M Williams","doi":"10.1177/15291006231163179","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/15291006231163179","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We synthesized the vast, contradictory scholarly literature on gender bias in academic science from 2000 to 2020. In the most prestigious journals and media outlets, which influence many people's opinions about sexism, bias is frequently portrayed as an omnipresent factor limiting women's progress in the tenure-track academy. Claims and counterclaims regarding the presence or absence of sexism span a range of evaluation contexts. Our approach relied on a combination of meta-analysis and analytic dissection. We evaluated the empirical evidence for gender bias in six key contexts in the tenure-track academy: (a) tenure-track hiring, (b) grant funding, (c) teaching ratings, (d) journal acceptances, (e) salaries, and (f) recommendation letters. We also explored the gender gap in a seventh area, journal productivity, because it can moderate bias in other contexts. We focused on these specific domains, in which sexism has most often been alleged to be pervasive, because they represent important types of evaluation, and the extensive research corpus within these domains provides sufficient quantitative data for comprehensive analysis. Contrary to the omnipresent claims of sexism in these domains appearing in top journals and the media, our findings show that tenure-track women are at parity with tenure-track men in three domains (grant funding, journal acceptances, and recommendation letters) and are advantaged over men in a fourth domain (hiring). For teaching ratings and salaries, we found evidence of bias against women; although gender gaps in salary were much smaller than often claimed, they were nevertheless concerning. Even in the four domains in which we failed to find evidence of sexism disadvantaging women, we nevertheless acknowledge that broad societal structural factors may still impede women's advancement in academic science. Given the substantial resources directed toward reducing gender bias in academic science, it is imperative to develop a clear understanding of when and where such efforts are justified and of how resources can best be directed to mitigate sexism when and where it exists.</p>","PeriodicalId":37882,"journal":{"name":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","volume":"24 1","pages":"15-73"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/51/b2/10.1177_15291006231163179.PMC10394402.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9930508","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
Stories in Action. 行动中的故事。
1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1177/15291006231161337
James Walsh, Naomi Vaida, Alin Coman, Susan T Fiske

Stories have played a central role in human social and political life for thousands of years. Despite their ubiquity in culture and custom, however, they feature only peripherally in formal government policymaking. Government policy has tended to rely on tools with more predictable responses-incentives, transfers, and prohibitions. We argue that stories can and should feature more centrally in government policymaking. We lay out how stories can make policy more effective, specifying how they complement established policy tools. We provide a working definition of stories' key characteristics, contrasting them with other forms of communication. We trace the evolution of stories from their ancient origins to their role in mediating the impact of modern technologies on society. We then provide an account of the mechanisms underlying stories' impacts on their audiences. We conclude by describing three functions of stories-learning, persuasion, and collective action.

几千年来,故事在人类社会和政治生活中发挥了核心作用。然而,尽管它们在文化和习俗中无处不在,但它们在正式的政府政策制定中只是次要的。政府政策往往依赖于更具可预测性的工具——激励、转移和禁令。我们认为,故事可以而且应该在政府决策中占据更重要的位置。我们阐述了故事如何使政策更有效,说明了它们如何补充已建立的政策工具。我们提供了故事的关键特征的工作定义,并将其与其他形式的交流进行对比。我们追溯故事的演变,从它们的古老起源到它们在调解现代技术对社会的影响方面的作用。然后,我们提供了故事对其受众影响的潜在机制。最后,我们描述了故事的三个功能——学习、说服和集体行动。
{"title":"Stories in Action.","authors":"James Walsh,&nbsp;Naomi Vaida,&nbsp;Alin Coman,&nbsp;Susan T Fiske","doi":"10.1177/15291006231161337","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/15291006231161337","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Stories have played a central role in human social and political life for thousands of years. Despite their ubiquity in culture and custom, however, they feature only peripherally in formal government policymaking. Government policy has tended to rely on tools with more predictable responses-incentives, transfers, and prohibitions. We argue that stories can and should feature more centrally in government policymaking. We lay out how stories can make policy more effective, specifying how they complement established policy tools. We provide a working definition of stories' key characteristics, contrasting them with other forms of communication. We trace the evolution of stories from their ancient origins to their role in mediating the impact of modern technologies on society. We then provide an account of the mechanisms underlying stories' impacts on their audiences. We conclude by describing three functions of stories-learning, persuasion, and collective action.</p>","PeriodicalId":37882,"journal":{"name":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","volume":"23 3","pages":"99-141"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10173355/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9505599","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
About the Authors. 关于作者。
1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1177/15291006231175660
{"title":"About the Authors.","authors":"","doi":"10.1177/15291006231175660","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/15291006231175660","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":37882,"journal":{"name":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","volume":"23 3","pages":"ii"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9440181","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Call for Commentaries. 呼吁评论。
1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1177/15291006231170139
{"title":"Call for Commentaries.","authors":"","doi":"10.1177/15291006231170139","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/15291006231170139","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":37882,"journal":{"name":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","volume":"23 3","pages":"142"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9448812","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Scaling Up Change: A Critical Review and Practical Guide to Harnessing Social Norms for Climate Action. 《扩大变化:利用社会规范促进气候行动的批判性审查和实践指南》。
1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-10-01 DOI: 10.1177/15291006221105279
Sara M Constantino, Gregg Sparkman, Gordon T Kraft-Todd, Cristina Bicchieri, Damon Centola, Bettina Shell-Duncan, Sonja Vogt, Elke U Weber

Anthropogenic carbon emissions have the potential to trigger changes in climate and ecosystems that would be catastrophic for the well-being of humans and other species. Widespread shifts in production and consumption patterns are urgently needed to address climate change. Although transnational agreements and national policy are necessary for a transition to a fully decarbonized global economy, fluctuating political priorities and lobbying by vested interests have slowed these efforts. Against this backdrop, bottom-up pressure from social movements and shifting social norms may offer a complementary path to a more sustainable economy. Furthermore, norm change may be an important component of decarbonization policies by accelerating or strengthening the impacts of other demand-side measures. Individual actions and policy support are social processes-they are intimately linked to expectations about the actions and beliefs of others. Although prevailing social norms often reinforce the status quo and unsustainable development pathways, social dynamics can also create widespread and rapid shifts in cultural values and practices, including increasing pressure on politicians to enact ambitious policy. We synthesize literature on social-norm influence, measurement, and change from the perspectives of psychology, anthropology, sociology, and economics. We discuss the opportunities and challenges for the use of social-norm and social-tipping interventions to promote climate action. Social-norm interventions aimed at addressing climate change or other social dilemmas are promising but no panacea. They require in-depth contextual knowledge, ethical consideration, and situation-specific tailoring and testing to understand whether they can be effectively implemented at scale. Our review aims to provide practitioners with insights and tools to reflect on the promises and pitfalls of such interventions in diverse contexts.

人为碳排放有可能引发气候和生态系统的变化,这对人类和其他物种的福祉将是灾难性的。为应对气候变化,迫切需要广泛改变生产和消费模式。虽然跨国协定和国家政策对于向完全脱碳的全球经济过渡是必要的,但政治优先事项的波动和既得利益集团的游说减缓了这些努力。在这种背景下,来自社会运动和不断变化的社会规范的自下而上的压力可能为更可持续的经济提供一条互补的道路。此外,规范变化可以加速或加强其他需求侧措施的影响,从而成为脱碳政策的重要组成部分。个人行为和政策支持是社会过程——它们与对他人行为和信念的期望密切相关。虽然普遍的社会规范往往会加强现状和不可持续的发展道路,但社会动态也会造成文化价值观和做法的广泛和迅速转变,包括对政治家施加越来越大的压力,要求他们制定雄心勃勃的政策。我们从心理学、人类学、社会学和经济学的角度综合了有关社会规范影响、测量和变化的文献。我们讨论了利用社会规范和社会小费干预措施促进气候行动的机遇和挑战。旨在解决气候变化或其他社会困境的社会规范干预是有希望的,但不是万灵药。它们需要深入的背景知识、伦理考虑和特定情况的裁剪和测试,以了解它们是否可以有效地大规模实施。我们的审查旨在为从业者提供见解和工具,以反思在不同背景下此类干预措施的承诺和陷阱。
{"title":"Scaling Up Change: A Critical Review and Practical Guide to Harnessing Social Norms for Climate Action.","authors":"Sara M Constantino,&nbsp;Gregg Sparkman,&nbsp;Gordon T Kraft-Todd,&nbsp;Cristina Bicchieri,&nbsp;Damon Centola,&nbsp;Bettina Shell-Duncan,&nbsp;Sonja Vogt,&nbsp;Elke U Weber","doi":"10.1177/15291006221105279","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/15291006221105279","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Anthropogenic carbon emissions have the potential to trigger changes in climate and ecosystems that would be catastrophic for the well-being of humans and other species. Widespread shifts in production and consumption patterns are urgently needed to address climate change. Although transnational agreements and national policy are necessary for a transition to a fully decarbonized global economy, fluctuating political priorities and lobbying by vested interests have slowed these efforts. Against this backdrop, bottom-up pressure from social movements and shifting social norms may offer a complementary path to a more sustainable economy. Furthermore, norm change may be an important component of decarbonization policies by accelerating or strengthening the impacts of other demand-side measures. Individual actions and policy support are social processes-they are intimately linked to expectations about the actions and beliefs of others. Although prevailing social norms often reinforce the status quo and unsustainable development pathways, social dynamics can also create widespread and rapid shifts in cultural values and practices, including increasing pressure on politicians to enact ambitious policy. We synthesize literature on social-norm influence, measurement, and change from the perspectives of psychology, anthropology, sociology, and economics. We discuss the opportunities and challenges for the use of social-norm and social-tipping interventions to promote climate action. Social-norm interventions aimed at addressing climate change or other social dilemmas are promising but no panacea. They require in-depth contextual knowledge, ethical consideration, and situation-specific tailoring and testing to understand whether they can be effectively implemented at scale. Our review aims to provide practitioners with insights and tools to reflect on the promises and pitfalls of such interventions in diverse contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":37882,"journal":{"name":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","volume":"23 2","pages":"50-97"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"33504852","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 38
Interventions Based on Social Norms Could Benefit From Considering Adversarial Information Environments: Comment on Constantino et al. (2022). 基于社会规范的干预可以从考虑对抗性信息环境中受益:评论Constantino等人(2022)。
1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-10-01 DOI: 10.1177/15291006221114132
Stephan Lewandowsky, Sander van der Linden
In 1859, John Tyndall presented his findings to the Royal Society about a perfectly colorless and odorless gas, known as “carbonic acid,” that he had discovered to be nearly opaque to radiant heat despite being transparent to visible light. We now refer to carbonic acid as carbon dioxide (CO2), and before the end of the 19th century, a Swedish physicist had already identified its potential to alter the Earth’s climate as a result of the combustion of fossil fuels (Arrhenius, 1896). More than 120 years later, scientists continue to warn the world about the adverse effects of CO2 emissions (Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change [IPCC], 2022), but to date the policy response has been inadequate, and we are on track to exceed purportedly “safe” global temperature increases (IPCC, 2022). There are many reasons for our collective failure to respond adequately to climate change, ranging from well-organized political opposition (Lewandowsky, 2021) to the inherent sociopsychological challenges posed by a problem that requires global collective action and large-scale behavior change by millions of people around the world (e.g., Smith & Mayer, 2018). The article by Constantino and colleagues focuses on the role of social norms in facilitating the widespread shift in behavioral practices that is required to deal with climate change. Humans are social animals and hence sensitive to perceived social norms: We tend to engage in behaviors on the basis of expectations of what others around us do or think what should be done. When those norms change, people’s behaviors also change. The key point made by Constantino and colleagues is that localized interventions can incentivize change in a subset of a population, creating minorities committed to a prosocial or proenvironmental nonnormative belief or behavior. The tendency to conform, in turn, leads others to adopt this nonnormative behavior, which begins to spread through social networks. Once a critical mass has adopted the nonnormative behavior, these social dynamics trigger abrupt, widespread, and nonlinear change, eventually tipping societies toward more sustainable equilibria (p. 51). Framed within this overarching approach, Constantino and colleagues provide admirably detailed insights into how those large-scale changes can be triggered through local interventions. An illustrative case involves the spread of solar panels across Germany during the early 2000s: It was initially observed that in communities in which a small group of early adaptors were in close proximity (e.g., in the same street), local cascades were triggered that relatively quickly created communities in which people without solar panels were in the minority. These local clusters, however, failed to spread into neighboring communities until policy makers launched a “100,000 Roofs” program that provided reduced-interest loans and other incentives to create bridges into neighboring communities to trigger further local cascades. By 2016, Ger
{"title":"Interventions Based on Social Norms Could Benefit From Considering Adversarial Information Environments: Comment on Constantino et al. (2022).","authors":"Stephan Lewandowsky,&nbsp;Sander van der Linden","doi":"10.1177/15291006221114132","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/15291006221114132","url":null,"abstract":"In 1859, John Tyndall presented his findings to the Royal Society about a perfectly colorless and odorless gas, known as “carbonic acid,” that he had discovered to be nearly opaque to radiant heat despite being transparent to visible light. We now refer to carbonic acid as carbon dioxide (CO2), and before the end of the 19th century, a Swedish physicist had already identified its potential to alter the Earth’s climate as a result of the combustion of fossil fuels (Arrhenius, 1896). More than 120 years later, scientists continue to warn the world about the adverse effects of CO2 emissions (Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change [IPCC], 2022), but to date the policy response has been inadequate, and we are on track to exceed purportedly “safe” global temperature increases (IPCC, 2022). There are many reasons for our collective failure to respond adequately to climate change, ranging from well-organized political opposition (Lewandowsky, 2021) to the inherent sociopsychological challenges posed by a problem that requires global collective action and large-scale behavior change by millions of people around the world (e.g., Smith & Mayer, 2018). The article by Constantino and colleagues focuses on the role of social norms in facilitating the widespread shift in behavioral practices that is required to deal with climate change. Humans are social animals and hence sensitive to perceived social norms: We tend to engage in behaviors on the basis of expectations of what others around us do or think what should be done. When those norms change, people’s behaviors also change. The key point made by Constantino and colleagues is that localized interventions can incentivize change in a subset of a population, creating minorities committed to a prosocial or proenvironmental nonnormative belief or behavior. The tendency to conform, in turn, leads others to adopt this nonnormative behavior, which begins to spread through social networks. Once a critical mass has adopted the nonnormative behavior, these social dynamics trigger abrupt, widespread, and nonlinear change, eventually tipping societies toward more sustainable equilibria (p. 51). Framed within this overarching approach, Constantino and colleagues provide admirably detailed insights into how those large-scale changes can be triggered through local interventions. An illustrative case involves the spread of solar panels across Germany during the early 2000s: It was initially observed that in communities in which a small group of early adaptors were in close proximity (e.g., in the same street), local cascades were triggered that relatively quickly created communities in which people without solar panels were in the minority. These local clusters, however, failed to spread into neighboring communities until policy makers launched a “100,000 Roofs” program that provided reduced-interest loans and other incentives to create bridges into neighboring communities to trigger further local cascades. By 2016, Ger","PeriodicalId":37882,"journal":{"name":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","volume":"23 2","pages":"43-49"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9574027/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"33504853","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The Science of Visual Data Communication: What Works. 视觉数据交流的科学:什么是有效的。
1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.1177/15291006211051956
Steven L Franconeri, Lace M Padilla, Priti Shah, Jeffrey M Zacks, Jessica Hullman

Effectively designed data visualizations allow viewers to use their powerful visual systems to understand patterns in data across science, education, health, and public policy. But ineffectively designed visualizations can cause confusion, misunderstanding, or even distrust-especially among viewers with low graphical literacy. We review research-backed guidelines for creating effective and intuitive visualizations oriented toward communicating data to students, coworkers, and the general public. We describe how the visual system can quickly extract broad statistics from a display, whereas poorly designed displays can lead to misperceptions and illusions. Extracting global statistics is fast, but comparing between subsets of values is slow. Effective graphics avoid taxing working memory, guide attention, and respect familiar conventions. Data visualizations can play a critical role in teaching and communication, provided that designers tailor those visualizations to their audience.

有效设计的数据可视化使观看者能够使用其强大的视觉系统来理解科学、教育、卫生和公共政策领域的数据模式。但是,设计效果不佳的可视化可能会导致混乱、误解甚至不信任,尤其是在图形文化水平较低的观众中。我们回顾了研究支持的指导方针,以创建有效和直观的可视化,面向学生,同事和公众交流数据。我们描述了视觉系统如何从显示器中快速提取广泛的统计数据,而设计不良的显示器可能导致误解和错觉。提取全局统计数据很快,但是比较子集之间的值很慢。有效的图形避免占用工作记忆,引导注意力,并尊重熟悉的惯例。数据可视化可以在教学和交流中发挥关键作用,前提是设计师为他们的受众量身定制这些可视化。
{"title":"The Science of Visual Data Communication: What Works.","authors":"Steven L Franconeri,&nbsp;Lace M Padilla,&nbsp;Priti Shah,&nbsp;Jeffrey M Zacks,&nbsp;Jessica Hullman","doi":"10.1177/15291006211051956","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/15291006211051956","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Effectively designed data visualizations allow viewers to use their powerful visual systems to understand patterns in data across science, education, health, and public policy. But ineffectively designed visualizations can cause confusion, misunderstanding, or even distrust-especially among viewers with low graphical literacy. We review research-backed guidelines for creating effective and intuitive visualizations oriented toward communicating data to students, coworkers, and the general public. We describe how the visual system can quickly extract broad statistics from a display, whereas poorly designed displays can lead to misperceptions and illusions. Extracting global statistics is fast, but comparing between subsets of values is slow. Effective graphics avoid taxing working memory, guide attention, and respect familiar conventions. Data visualizations can play a critical role in teaching and communication, provided that designers tailor those visualizations to their audience.</p>","PeriodicalId":37882,"journal":{"name":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","volume":"22 3","pages":"110-161"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39588144","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 88
Test a Witness's Memory of a Suspect Only Once. 只测试一次证人对嫌疑犯的记忆。
1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.1177/15291006211026259
John T Wixted, Gary L Wells, Elizabeth F Loftus, Brandon L Garrett

Eyewitness misidentifications are almost always made with high confidence in the courtroom. The courtroom is where eyewitnesses make their last identification of defendants suspected of (and charged with) committing a crime. But what did those same eyewitnesses do on the first identification test, conducted early in a police investigation? Despite testifying with high confidence in court, many eyewitnesses also testified that they had initially identified the suspect with low confidence or failed to identify the suspect at all. Presenting a lineup leaves the eyewitness with a memory trace of the faces in the lineup, including that of the suspect. As a result, the memory signal generated by the face of that suspect will be stronger on a later test involving the same witness, even if the suspect is innocent. In that sense, testing memory contaminates memory. These considerations underscore the importance of a newly proposed recommendation for conducting eyewitness identifications: Avoid repeated identification procedures with the same witness and suspect. This recommendation applies not only to additional tests conducted by police investigators but also to the final test conducted in the courtroom, in front of the judge and jury.

在法庭上,目击者的错误指认几乎总是充满信心的。法庭是目击证人对涉嫌(或被控)犯罪的被告进行最后鉴定的地方。但是,在警方调查的早期进行的第一次鉴定测试中,这些目击者做了什么?尽管在法庭上的证词可信度很高,但许多目击者也作证说,他们最初指认嫌疑人的可信度很低,或者根本没有指认出嫌疑人。指认指认会让目击者对指认中的人留下记忆痕迹,包括嫌疑人的脸。因此,在随后涉及同一证人的测试中,即使嫌疑人是无辜的,由该嫌疑人的脸产生的记忆信号也会更强。从这个意义上说,测试内存会污染内存。这些考虑强调了一项新近提出的关于进行目击者辨认的建议的重要性:避免对同一证人和嫌疑人进行重复辨认程序。这项建议不仅适用于警察调查人员进行的额外测试,也适用于在法庭上、在法官和陪审团面前进行的最后测试。
{"title":"Test a Witness's Memory of a Suspect Only Once.","authors":"John T Wixted,&nbsp;Gary L Wells,&nbsp;Elizabeth F Loftus,&nbsp;Brandon L Garrett","doi":"10.1177/15291006211026259","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/15291006211026259","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Eyewitness misidentifications are almost always made with high confidence in the courtroom. The courtroom is where eyewitnesses make their <i>last</i> identification of defendants suspected of (and charged with) committing a crime. But what did those same eyewitnesses do on the <i>first</i> identification test, conducted early in a police investigation? Despite testifying with high confidence in court, many eyewitnesses also testified that they had initially identified the suspect with low confidence or failed to identify the suspect at all. Presenting a lineup leaves the eyewitness with a memory trace of the faces in the lineup, including that of the suspect. As a result, the memory signal generated by the face of that suspect will be stronger on a later test involving the same witness, even if the suspect is innocent. In that sense, testing memory contaminates memory. These considerations underscore the importance of a newly proposed recommendation for conducting eyewitness identifications: <i>Avoid repeated identification procedures with the same witness and suspect</i>. This recommendation applies not only to additional tests conducted by police investigators but also to the final test conducted in the courtroom, in front of the judge and jury.</p>","PeriodicalId":37882,"journal":{"name":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","volume":"22 1_suppl","pages":"1S-18S"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39674423","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
期刊
Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society
全部 Clean Technol. Environ. Policy BIOGEOSCIENCES Energy Storage Exp. Anim. Nat. Clim. Change ARCHAEOMETRY Aust. J. Earth Sci. Int. J. Paleopathol. ACTA PETROL SIN Environ. Toxicol. Pharmacol. Clean-Soil Air Water Energy Ecol Environ Chem. Ecol. European Journal of Clinical and Experimental Medicine Big Earth Data Public Adm Dev Environmental Control in Biology Basin Res. EUREKA: Physics and Engineering Erziehungswissenschaftliche Revue EXPERT REV ANTICANC Environmental Claims Journal Archaeol. Anthropol. Sci. Hydrogeol. J. J. Geog. Sci. Nat. Hazards Earth Syst. Sci. Yan Ke Xue Bao (Hong Kong) EXPERT OPIN DRUG DEL 化工催化剂及甲醇技术 Environ. Geochem. Health REV MINERAL GEOCHEM J. Atmos. Sol. Terr. Phys. Acta Geochimica Indonesian Journal of Chemical Science and Technology (IJCST) Vadose Zone J. Environmental Health Insights Acta Oceanolog. Sin. J. Meteorolog. Res. Environ. Eng. Manage. J. FAM PRACT Environmental dermatology : the official journal of the Japanese Society for Contact Dermatitis ECOL RESTOR Atmos. Meas. Tech. Int. J. Climatol. EUR RESPIR REV TERR ATMOS OCEAN SCI ERN: Regulation (IO) (Topic) Engineering Science and Technology, an International Journal 数据分析与知识发现 Exp. Eye Res. Finance and Development Atmos. Res. Quat. Sci. Rev. AAPG Bull. Mon. Weather Rev. Annu. Rev. Earth Planet. Sci. J. Atmos. Oceanic Technol. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. REV BRAS PALEONTOLOG J. Asian Earth Sci. ARCT ANTARCT ALP RES Am. Mineral. Carbon Balance Manage. Infusion & Chemotherapy Sustainable Energy Fuels Geosci. Model Dev. Am. J. Sci. EUR UROL FOCUS IZV-PHYS SOLID EART+ Adv. Meteorol. Expert Rev. Clin. Pharmacol. ACTA GEOL POL 电力系统及其自动化学报 Aquat. Geochem. Asia-Pac. J. Atmos. Sci. Ann. Glaciol. Environ. Pollut. Bioavailability Int. J. Biometeorol. Atmos. Chem. Phys. 胜利油田党校学报 Geobiology ACTA GEOL SIN-ENGL J. Hydrol. Phys. Chem. Miner. Appl. Geochem. Acta Geophys. Contrib. Mineral. Petrol. 青铜器与金文 J. Atmos. Chem. Org. Geochem. Appl. Clay Sci. Environ. Prot. Eng. Environ. Eng. Res. ArcheoSci.-Rev. Archeom. Geochim. Cosmochim. Acta Energy Environ. COMP BIOCHEM PHYS C Clim. Change Pure Appl. Geophys. Ecol. Indic.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1