首页 > 最新文献

Word of Mouth最新文献

英文 中文
Responding to Children’s Emotions 回应孩子的情绪
Q4 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-07-21 DOI: 10.1177/10483950221101453d
C. Westby
{"title":"Responding to Children’s Emotions","authors":"C. Westby","doi":"10.1177/10483950221101453d","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10483950221101453d","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":39491,"journal":{"name":"Word of Mouth","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-07-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43791959","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Using Narratives to Assess Bilingual Children 用叙事来评估双语儿童
Q4 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-07-21 DOI: 10.1177/10483950221101453
C. Westby
Identifying a language difference versus a language disorder in young bilingual children is a challenging task for speech-language pathologists. Narrative assessments are increasingly employed in identifying bilingual students with language impairment. Research on dual language learners (DLLs) has shown that children achieve language competency in their second language (L2) first in narrative organization (macrostructure), then vocabulary, and last morphology (Paradis, 2016). Narrative tasks are often used to assess the language of schoolage children because they tap a range of functional language skills (as opposed to decontextualized language assessments that, for example, ask children to name pictures, point to pictures, and finish sentences). Narratives require a variety of receptive/comprehension language skills and expressive/production language skills. Children must be able to remember a story they hear and understand who the story characters were, what they did, and why; they must also be able to produce stories in which they coherently link ideas and organize those ideas around a common theme (Gillam & Pearson, 2004). Difficulty in performing these tasks is associated with language impairment (Tsimpli et al., 2016). A significant discrepancy between receptive and expressive ability has been a hallmark of language impairment (Gibson et al., 2018). Recent research indicates that a receptive–expressive gap occurs in vocabulary and semantic testing for bilingual children (Gibson et al., 2012, 2014b), but not with typically developing (TD) children; furthermore, the discrepancy is exacerbated for bilingual children with primary language impairment (PLI; Gibson et al., 2014a). Such a discrepancy would not be predicted in theory because standardized tests are designed based on normative research to yield outcomes that should be comparable. A newsletter dedicated to speech & language in school-age children
识别年轻双语儿童的语言差异和语言障碍对言语语言病理学家来说是一项具有挑战性的任务。叙事评估越来越多地用于识别有语言障碍的双语学生。对双语学习者(DLL)的研究表明,儿童在第二语言(L2)中首先在叙事组织(宏观结构)、词汇和最后的形态方面获得了语言能力(Paradis,2016)。叙事任务通常用于评估学龄儿童的语言,因为它们利用了一系列功能性语言技能(而不是去文本化的语言评估,例如,要求儿童命名图片、指向图片和完成句子)。叙述需要多种接受/理解语言技能和表达/生产语言技能。孩子们必须能够记住他们听到的故事,并理解故事中的人物是谁,他们做了什么,以及为什么;他们还必须能够制作出连贯地将想法联系起来并围绕共同主题组织这些想法的故事(Gillam&Pearson,2004)。执行这些任务的困难与语言障碍有关(Tsimpli等人,2016)。接受能力和表达能力之间的显著差异是语言障碍的标志(Gibson et al.,2018)。最近的研究表明,双语儿童在词汇和语义测试中存在接受-表达差距(Gibson et al.,20122014b),但在典型发育(TD)儿童中没有;此外,对于有初级语言障碍的双语儿童来说,这种差异更加严重(PLI;Gibson等人,2014a)。这种差异在理论上是无法预测的,因为标准化测试是基于规范性研究设计的,目的是产生具有可比性的结果。一份致力于学龄儿童言语和语言的时事通讯
{"title":"Using Narratives to Assess Bilingual Children","authors":"C. Westby","doi":"10.1177/10483950221101453","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10483950221101453","url":null,"abstract":"Identifying a language difference versus a language disorder in young bilingual children is a challenging task for speech-language pathologists. Narrative assessments are increasingly employed in identifying bilingual students with language impairment. Research on dual language learners (DLLs) has shown that children achieve language competency in their second language (L2) first in narrative organization (macrostructure), then vocabulary, and last morphology (Paradis, 2016). Narrative tasks are often used to assess the language of schoolage children because they tap a range of functional language skills (as opposed to decontextualized language assessments that, for example, ask children to name pictures, point to pictures, and finish sentences). Narratives require a variety of receptive/comprehension language skills and expressive/production language skills. Children must be able to remember a story they hear and understand who the story characters were, what they did, and why; they must also be able to produce stories in which they coherently link ideas and organize those ideas around a common theme (Gillam & Pearson, 2004). Difficulty in performing these tasks is associated with language impairment (Tsimpli et al., 2016). A significant discrepancy between receptive and expressive ability has been a hallmark of language impairment (Gibson et al., 2018). Recent research indicates that a receptive–expressive gap occurs in vocabulary and semantic testing for bilingual children (Gibson et al., 2012, 2014b), but not with typically developing (TD) children; furthermore, the discrepancy is exacerbated for bilingual children with primary language impairment (PLI; Gibson et al., 2014a). Such a discrepancy would not be predicted in theory because standardized tests are designed based on normative research to yield outcomes that should be comparable. A newsletter dedicated to speech & language in school-age children","PeriodicalId":39491,"journal":{"name":"Word of Mouth","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-07-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43816755","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Our Choice of Words Matters 我们的措辞很重要
Q4 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-07-21 DOI: 10.1177/10483950221101453b
C. Westby
Dodd, B. (2014). Differential diagnosis of pediatric speech sound disorder. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 1(3), 189– 196. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40474-014-0017-3 Dodd, B., Ttofari-Eecen, K., Brommeyer, K., Ng, K., Reilly, S., & Morgan, A. (2018). Delayed and disordered development of articulation and phonology between four and seven years. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 34(2), 87–99. https:// doi. org/10.1177/0265659017735958 Glaspey, A., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2007). A dynamic approach to phonological assessment. International Journal of SpeechLanguage Pathology, 9(4), 286–296. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/14417 040701435418 Ireland, M., McLeod, S., Farquharson, K., & Crowe, K. (2020). Evaluating children in U.S. public schools with speech sound disorders. Topics in Language Disorders, 40(4), 326–340. https://doi.org/10.1097/tld.0000000000000226 Kok, E. C. E., & To, C. K. (2019). Revisiting the cutoff criteria of Intelligibility in Context Scale–Traditional Chinese. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 50(4), 629–638. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_LSHSS-18-0073 Lof, G. L. (1996). Factors associated with speech-sound stimulability. Journal of Communication Disorders, 29(4), 255–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/0021-9924(96)00013-5 McGill, N., Crowe, K., & McLeod, S. (2020). “Many wasted months”: Stakeholders’ perspectives about waiting for speechlanguage pathology services. International Journal of SpeechLanguage Pathology, 22(3), 313–326. https://doi.org/10.1080/1 7549507.2020.1747541 McLeod, S. (2020). Intelligibility in Context Scale: Cross-linguistic use, validity, and reliability. Speech, Language and Hearing, 23(1), 9–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/2050571X.2020.1718837 McLeod, S., Harrison, L. J., & McCormack, J. (2012). The Intelligibility in Context Scale: Validity and reliability of a subjective rating measure. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(2), 648–656. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388 McLeod, S., Harrison, L. J., & Wang, C. (2019). A longitudinal population study of literacy and numeracy outcomes for children identified with speech, language, and communication needs in early childhood. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 47, 507–517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.07.004 McLeod, S., & Masso, S. (2019). Screening children’s speech: The impact of imitated elicitation and word position. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 50(1), 71–82. https:// doi.org/10.1044/2018_LSHSS-17-0141 Morgan, A., Ttofari Eecen, K., Pezic, A., Brommeyer, K., Mei, C., Eadie, P., Reilly, S., & Dodd, B. (2017). Who to refer for speech therapy at 4 years of age versus who to “watch and wait.” The Journal of Pediatrics, 185, 200–204.e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.jpeds.2017.02.059 Ng, K. Y. M., To, C. K. S., & McLeod, S. (2014). Validation of the Intelligibility in Context Scale as a screening tool for preschoolers in Hong Kong. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 28(5), 316–328. https://doi.org/10.3109/0
Dodd,B.(2014)。儿童语音障碍的鉴别诊断。当前发育障碍报告,1(3),189-196。https://doi.org/10.1007/s40474-014-0017-3Dodd,B.、Ttofari Eecen,K.、Brommeyer,K.、Ng,K.、Reilly,S.和Morgan,A.(2018)。发音和音韵学在四到七年之间发展迟缓和紊乱。儿童语言教学与治疗,34(2),87-99。https://doi。org/10.1177/0265659017735958 Glaspey,A.和Stoel Gammon,C.(2007)。语音评估的动态方法。国际言语病理学杂志,9(4),286-296。https://doi.org/10.1080/14417 040701435418 Ireland,M.、McLeod,S.、Farquharson,K.和Crowe,K.(2020)。评估美国公立学校中有语音障碍的儿童。语言障碍专题,40(4),326–340。https://doi.org/10.1097/tld.0000000000000226Kok,E.C.E.和To,C.K.(2019)。重新审视语境量表中可理解性的分界标准——繁体中文。学校的语言、言语和听力服务,50(4),629–638。https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_LSHSS-18-0073Lof,G.L.(1996)。与语音刺激性相关的因素。《沟通障碍杂志》,29(4),255-278。https://doi.org/10.1016/0021-9924(96)00013-5 McGill,N.、Crowe,K.和McLeod,S.(2020)。“浪费了好几个月”:利益相关者对等待演讲语言病理学服务的看法。国际言语病理学杂志,22(3),313–326。https://doi.org/10.1080/17549507.2020.17747541 McLeod,S.(2020)。语境量表中的可理解性:跨语言使用、有效性和可靠性。言语、语言和听力,23(1),9-16。https://doi.org/10.1080/2050571X.2020.1718837McLeod,S.、Harrison,L.J.和McCormack,J.(2012)。语境中的可理解性量表:主观评分的有效性和可靠性。《言语、语言和听力研究杂志》,55(2),648–656。https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388McLeod,S.、Harrison,L.J.和Wang,C.(2019)。一项关于儿童早期言语、语言和沟通需求的识字和算术结果的纵向人群研究。《幼儿研究季刊》,47507-517。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.07.004McLeod,S.和Masso,S.(2019)。筛选儿童言语:模仿启发和单词位置的影响。学校的语言、言语和听力服务,50(1),71–82。https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_LSHSS-17-0141 Morgan,A.、Ttofari Eecen,K.、Pezic,A.、Brommeyer,K.、Mei,C.、Eadie,P.、Reilly,S.和Dodd,B.(2017)。谁应该在4岁时接受言语治疗,谁应该“观察并等待”。《儿科杂志》,185200-204.e1。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.02.059 Ng,K.Y.M.,To,C.K.S.,&McLeod,S.(2014)。香港学龄前儿童上下文智力量表筛查工具的验证。临床语言学与语音学,28(5),316-328。https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2013.865789Powell,T.W.和Miccio,A.W.(1996)。刺激性:一种有用的临床工具。《沟通障碍杂志》,29(4),237-253。https://doi.org/10.1016/0021-9924(96)00012-3 Roulstone,S.、Miller,L.L.、Wren,Y.和Peters,T.J.(2009)。Avon父母和儿童纵向研究中8岁儿童言语障碍的自然史:2岁和5岁时的错误率。国际言语语言病理学杂志,11(5),381–391。https://doi.org/10.1080/17549500903125111 Subtelny,J.(1977)。言语评估对训练的影响。F.Bess(Ed.),《儿童耳聋》(第183-194页)。Grune&Stratton。To,C.K.S.,Cheung,P.S.和McLeod,S.(2013)。儿童香港粤语声母、元音和声调习得的人群研究。《言语、语言和听力研究杂志》,56(1),103-122。https://doi.org/10.1044/10924388(2012/11-0080)Tourville,J.A.和Guenther,F.H.(2011)。DIVA模型:一种关于语音获取和产生的神经理论。语言与认知过程,26(7),952–981。https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960903498424 Williams,A.L.、McLeod,S.和McCauley,R.J.(2021)。儿童语音障碍的干预措施(第2版)。布鲁克斯。Wren,Y.、Miller,L.L.、Peters,T.J.、Emond,A.和Roulstone,S.(2016)。八岁时持续性语音障碍的患病率和预测因素:一项人群队列研究的结果。《言语、语言和听力研究杂志》,59(4),647–673。https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSLHRS-14-0282
{"title":"Our Choice of Words Matters","authors":"C. Westby","doi":"10.1177/10483950221101453b","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10483950221101453b","url":null,"abstract":"Dodd, B. (2014). Differential diagnosis of pediatric speech sound disorder. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 1(3), 189– 196. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40474-014-0017-3 Dodd, B., Ttofari-Eecen, K., Brommeyer, K., Ng, K., Reilly, S., & Morgan, A. (2018). Delayed and disordered development of articulation and phonology between four and seven years. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 34(2), 87–99. https:// doi. org/10.1177/0265659017735958 Glaspey, A., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2007). A dynamic approach to phonological assessment. International Journal of SpeechLanguage Pathology, 9(4), 286–296. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/14417 040701435418 Ireland, M., McLeod, S., Farquharson, K., & Crowe, K. (2020). Evaluating children in U.S. public schools with speech sound disorders. Topics in Language Disorders, 40(4), 326–340. https://doi.org/10.1097/tld.0000000000000226 Kok, E. C. E., & To, C. K. (2019). Revisiting the cutoff criteria of Intelligibility in Context Scale–Traditional Chinese. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 50(4), 629–638. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_LSHSS-18-0073 Lof, G. L. (1996). Factors associated with speech-sound stimulability. Journal of Communication Disorders, 29(4), 255–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/0021-9924(96)00013-5 McGill, N., Crowe, K., & McLeod, S. (2020). “Many wasted months”: Stakeholders’ perspectives about waiting for speechlanguage pathology services. International Journal of SpeechLanguage Pathology, 22(3), 313–326. https://doi.org/10.1080/1 7549507.2020.1747541 McLeod, S. (2020). Intelligibility in Context Scale: Cross-linguistic use, validity, and reliability. Speech, Language and Hearing, 23(1), 9–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/2050571X.2020.1718837 McLeod, S., Harrison, L. J., & McCormack, J. (2012). The Intelligibility in Context Scale: Validity and reliability of a subjective rating measure. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(2), 648–656. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388 McLeod, S., Harrison, L. J., & Wang, C. (2019). A longitudinal population study of literacy and numeracy outcomes for children identified with speech, language, and communication needs in early childhood. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 47, 507–517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.07.004 McLeod, S., & Masso, S. (2019). Screening children’s speech: The impact of imitated elicitation and word position. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 50(1), 71–82. https:// doi.org/10.1044/2018_LSHSS-17-0141 Morgan, A., Ttofari Eecen, K., Pezic, A., Brommeyer, K., Mei, C., Eadie, P., Reilly, S., & Dodd, B. (2017). Who to refer for speech therapy at 4 years of age versus who to “watch and wait.” The Journal of Pediatrics, 185, 200–204.e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.jpeds.2017.02.059 Ng, K. Y. M., To, C. K. S., & McLeod, S. (2014). Validation of the Intelligibility in Context Scale as a screening tool for preschoolers in Hong Kong. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 28(5), 316–328. https://doi.org/10.3109/0","PeriodicalId":39491,"journal":{"name":"Word of Mouth","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-07-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42079195","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Birdie’s Tree 伯迪树
Q4 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-07-21 DOI: 10.1177/10483950221101453c
Carol Westby
{"title":"Birdie’s Tree","authors":"Carol Westby","doi":"10.1177/10483950221101453c","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10483950221101453c","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":39491,"journal":{"name":"Word of Mouth","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-07-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43664714","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Outgrowing Speech Sound Disorders 发育不良的语音障碍
Q4 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-07-21 DOI: 10.1177/10483950221101453a
C. Westby
Clinicians are often asked whether a child will outgrow speech errors without intervention. There are two potential outcomes for children with speech sound disorders (SSDs): normalization or long-term, persistent SSD (Roulstone et al., 2009; Wren et al., 2016). Understanding whether children outgrow speech errors without intervention is of clinical importance because there are fewer speech-language pathologists (SLPs) than the number of children who need their services, and there are often long waiting lists. Access to speechlanguage pathology services can impact children, their families, and society (McGill et al., 2020). Some children who do not receive speech-language pathology services may be able to resolve their speech errors at a later time point, whereas other children continue to show a large number of errors even when they receive belated therapy. For these latter children, delays in services or insufficient intervention frequency may lead to poor speech outcomes affecting children’s education, social development, and occupational prospects (McLeod et al., 2019). This study investigated four consistently reported risk factors of SSD related to children’s speech and language profiles (low stimulability, intelligibility, presence of atypical errors, and expressive language difficulties) and how these factors related to children’s time to normalization. This article presents the findings of a naturalistic cohort study investigating speech normalization and normalization rates in Cantonesespeaking preschool children in Hong Kong, China, who may be at risk for SSD. The objectives of this study were to quantify speech normalization rates at 2.5-year follow-up and to investigate predictors of time to normalization. The researchers examined (a) children who resolved nonadult realizations of speech sounds (i.e., had normalized production of speech sounds) and (b) those who had persisting speech sound difficulties (did not normalize) over 2.5 years. Method
临床医生经常被问到,如果不进行干预,孩子是否会因言语错误而长大。患有语音障碍(SSD)的儿童有两种潜在的结果:正常化或长期、持续的SSD(Roulstone等人,2009;Wren等人,2016)。了解儿童是否在没有干预的情况下长大后会出现言语错误具有临床重要性,因为言语语言病理学家(SLP)的人数少于需要他们服务的儿童人数,而且等待他们的名单往往很长。获得语言病理学服务会影响儿童、他们的家庭和社会(McGill等人,2020)。一些没有接受言语语言病理学服务的儿童可能能够在以后的时间点解决他们的言语错误,而其他儿童即使接受了迟来的治疗,也会继续表现出大量的错误。对于后一类儿童,服务延迟或干预频率不足可能导致言语效果不佳,影响儿童的教育、社会发展和职业前景(McLeod等人,2019)。本研究调查了与儿童言语和语言特征相关的四个持续报道的SSD风险因素(低刺激性、可理解性、非典型错误的存在和表达语言困难),以及这些因素如何与儿童的正常化时间相关。本文介绍了一项自然队列研究的结果,该研究调查了中国香港学龄前儿童的言语正常化和正常化率,他们可能有SSD的风险。本研究的目的是量化2.5年随访时的语音正常化率,并研究正常化时间的预测因素。研究人员检查了(a)解决了非成年人语音实现(即语音产生正常化)的儿童和(b)在2.5年内持续存在语音困难(未正常化)。方法
{"title":"Outgrowing Speech Sound Disorders","authors":"C. Westby","doi":"10.1177/10483950221101453a","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10483950221101453a","url":null,"abstract":"Clinicians are often asked whether a child will outgrow speech errors without intervention. There are two potential outcomes for children with speech sound disorders (SSDs): normalization or long-term, persistent SSD (Roulstone et al., 2009; Wren et al., 2016). Understanding whether children outgrow speech errors without intervention is of clinical importance because there are fewer speech-language pathologists (SLPs) than the number of children who need their services, and there are often long waiting lists. Access to speechlanguage pathology services can impact children, their families, and society (McGill et al., 2020). Some children who do not receive speech-language pathology services may be able to resolve their speech errors at a later time point, whereas other children continue to show a large number of errors even when they receive belated therapy. For these latter children, delays in services or insufficient intervention frequency may lead to poor speech outcomes affecting children’s education, social development, and occupational prospects (McLeod et al., 2019). This study investigated four consistently reported risk factors of SSD related to children’s speech and language profiles (low stimulability, intelligibility, presence of atypical errors, and expressive language difficulties) and how these factors related to children’s time to normalization. This article presents the findings of a naturalistic cohort study investigating speech normalization and normalization rates in Cantonesespeaking preschool children in Hong Kong, China, who may be at risk for SSD. The objectives of this study were to quantify speech normalization rates at 2.5-year follow-up and to investigate predictors of time to normalization. The researchers examined (a) children who resolved nonadult realizations of speech sounds (i.e., had normalized production of speech sounds) and (b) those who had persisting speech sound difficulties (did not normalize) over 2.5 years. Method","PeriodicalId":39491,"journal":{"name":"Word of Mouth","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-07-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47816309","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Therapy as a Journey of Change 作为改变之旅的治疗
Q4 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-05-01 DOI: 10.1177/10483950221086338d
C. Westby
humanity in historical, cultural, scientific, and spiritual contexts. The materials for the Honoring Tribal Legacies curriculum are quite extensive. It will take considerable time to review all that is available. Unless educators or SLPs are working with Plains Indians and Northwest Native students, they will likely use only selective portions of the curriculum. I suggest beginning with Inglebret’s unit, Honoring Tribal Legacies in Telling the Lewis and Clark Story. For many students, this unit will be sufficient to introduce activities that can serve as mirrors, windows, and sliding glass doors for Native and non-Native students. To gain a better understanding of the multiliteracy pedagogy framework, I also suggest reading Inglebret’s chapter 2 in volume 2 of the digital handbook, Place-Based Multiliteracies Framework. This chapter provides details on the components of multiliteracies pedagogy.
历史、文化、科学和精神语境中的人性。尊重部落遗产课程的材料相当广泛。审查所有可用的资料需要相当长的时间。除非教育工作者或slp与平原印第安人和西北土著学生一起工作,否则他们很可能只使用课程的选择性部分。我建议从英格尔布雷特的单位开始,在讲述刘易斯和克拉克的故事中尊重部落遗产。对于许多学生来说,本单元将足以介绍一些活动,这些活动可以作为镜子、窗户和滑动玻璃门,供土著和非土著学生使用。为了更好地理解多元读写教学法框架,我还建议阅读数字手册第二卷中Inglebret的第二章《基于地点的多元读写框架》。本章详细介绍了多元素养教学法的组成部分。
{"title":"Therapy as a Journey of Change","authors":"C. Westby","doi":"10.1177/10483950221086338d","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10483950221086338d","url":null,"abstract":"humanity in historical, cultural, scientific, and spiritual contexts. The materials for the Honoring Tribal Legacies curriculum are quite extensive. It will take considerable time to review all that is available. Unless educators or SLPs are working with Plains Indians and Northwest Native students, they will likely use only selective portions of the curriculum. I suggest beginning with Inglebret’s unit, Honoring Tribal Legacies in Telling the Lewis and Clark Story. For many students, this unit will be sufficient to introduce activities that can serve as mirrors, windows, and sliding glass doors for Native and non-Native students. To gain a better understanding of the multiliteracy pedagogy framework, I also suggest reading Inglebret’s chapter 2 in volume 2 of the digital handbook, Place-Based Multiliteracies Framework. This chapter provides details on the components of multiliteracies pedagogy.","PeriodicalId":39491,"journal":{"name":"Word of Mouth","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47651643","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Play in Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder 自闭症谱系障碍儿童的游戏
Q4 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-05-01 DOI: 10.1177/10483950221086338b
C. Westby
By teaching with diverse books, we push open doors and encourage students to explore worlds and lives unlike theirs. In doing so, we support the multiple cultural influences (values, practices, experiences, etc.) affecting both readers and authors, and the ways in which these influence meaning making. We want students to recognize the benefits of stepping through those doors, taking on the perspective of someone different from us, and communicating that diverse perspectives and experiences are truly valued and valuable.
通过用不同的书籍进行教学,我们打开了大门,鼓励学生探索与他们不同的世界和生活。在这样做的过程中,我们支持影响读者和作者的多种文化影响(价值观、实践、经验等),以及这些影响意义形成的方式。我们希望学生们认识到走进这些门的好处,从与我们不同的人的角度出发,并交流不同的观点和经验是真正有价值的。
{"title":"Play in Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder","authors":"C. Westby","doi":"10.1177/10483950221086338b","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10483950221086338b","url":null,"abstract":"By teaching with diverse books, we push open doors and encourage students to explore worlds and lives unlike theirs. In doing so, we support the multiple cultural influences (values, practices, experiences, etc.) affecting both readers and authors, and the ways in which these influence meaning making. We want students to recognize the benefits of stepping through those doors, taking on the perspective of someone different from us, and communicating that diverse perspectives and experiences are truly valued and valuable.","PeriodicalId":39491,"journal":{"name":"Word of Mouth","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47344557","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Using Books as Mirrors, Windows, Sliding Glass Doors 使用书籍作为镜子、窗户、滑动玻璃门
Q4 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-05-01 DOI: 10.1177/10483950221086338a
C. Westby
{"title":"Using Books as Mirrors, Windows, Sliding Glass Doors","authors":"C. Westby","doi":"10.1177/10483950221086338a","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10483950221086338a","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":39491,"journal":{"name":"Word of Mouth","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46557412","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Honoring Tribal Legacies 尊重部落遗产
Q4 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-05-01 DOI: 10.1177/10483950221086338c
C. Westby
Some authors have suggested that the symbolic play deficit in ASD is due to children’s difficulty generating play ideas (a performance deficit), rather than difficulty comprehending symbolic pretend (a competence aspect), when adults provide them with supportive scaffolding and ideas. Are the differences noted in the pretend play of children with ASD an issue of competence (they don’t possess pretend qualities in their play) or performance (they understand pretend play and can engage in it but they rarely do) (Kasari et al., 2013)? Yet even though they do somewhat better in scaffolded play, children with ASD still exhibit significant play deficits in both spontaneous and scaffolded play, indicating their play deficits are in both competence and performance; the play deficit is not limited to problems with generativity (Rutherford et al., 2007).
一些作者认为,ASD的象征性游戏缺陷是由于当成年人为他们提供支持性的脚手架和想法时,儿童难以产生游戏想法(表现缺陷),而不是难以理解象征性假装(能力方面)。ASD儿童在假装游戏中所表现出的差异是能力问题(他们在游戏中不具备假装的品质)还是表现问题(他们理解假装游戏,并能参与其中,但很少这样做)(Kasari et al., 2013)?然而,尽管ASD儿童在框架式游戏中表现得更好,但他们在自发游戏和框架式游戏中仍然表现出明显的游戏缺陷,这表明他们的游戏缺陷既存在于能力上,也存在于表现上;游戏缺陷并不局限于生成问题(Rutherford et al., 2007)。
{"title":"Honoring Tribal Legacies","authors":"C. Westby","doi":"10.1177/10483950221086338c","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10483950221086338c","url":null,"abstract":"Some authors have suggested that the symbolic play deficit in ASD is due to children’s difficulty generating play ideas (a performance deficit), rather than difficulty comprehending symbolic pretend (a competence aspect), when adults provide them with supportive scaffolding and ideas. Are the differences noted in the pretend play of children with ASD an issue of competence (they don’t possess pretend qualities in their play) or performance (they understand pretend play and can engage in it but they rarely do) (Kasari et al., 2013)? Yet even though they do somewhat better in scaffolded play, children with ASD still exhibit significant play deficits in both spontaneous and scaffolded play, indicating their play deficits are in both competence and performance; the play deficit is not limited to problems with generativity (Rutherford et al., 2007).","PeriodicalId":39491,"journal":{"name":"Word of Mouth","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45182880","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Transtheoretical Model Applied to Stuttering 跨理论模型在口吃中的应用
Q4 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-05-01 DOI: 10.1177/10483950221086338
C. Westby
{"title":"The Transtheoretical Model Applied to Stuttering","authors":"C. Westby","doi":"10.1177/10483950221086338","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10483950221086338","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":39491,"journal":{"name":"Word of Mouth","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43867601","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Word of Mouth
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1