首页 > 最新文献

Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews最新文献

英文 中文
The commentaries: Some are visual illusions 评论:有些是视觉错觉
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2010-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/CCBR.2010.50012
R. Weisman
{"title":"The commentaries: Some are visual illusions","authors":"R. Weisman","doi":"10.3819/CCBR.2010.50012","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50012","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50012","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70233198","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
What's the use of picture discrimination experiments? 图片辨别实验有什么用?
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2010-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/CCBR.2010.50010
S. Lea
If we want to know whether it is useful to examine picture set discriminations, we need to ask which of these reasons lies behind the experiment in question. Of course, things are never as simple as that. Researchers are not always either clear or consistent about why we do experiments, and often more than one of these motivations may be detectable in the design and description of their research. Furthermore, those who read and cite research often attribute motives to the researchers that they did not in fact hold, or interpret results in ways that the original authors would not endorse. Nonetheless, the research techniques that are appropriate depend critically on which of these motivations are operative, and it follows that how we should assess the experiments and our results depends critically on which of these goals they are aiming at. In this commentary, I argue that the underlying motivation for most of the research that Weisman and Spetch (2010) discuss is (c) above, to investigate object representation. However, much of Weisman and Spetch’s (2010) critique assumes that it is (a), to investigate the discriminability of the real objects. They build a damning case against the use of much of the published research to answer that question. But it is not clear that there is anyone in the dock – and in any case, it will be argued below that it is not improper to argue from the discriminability of pictures to the discriminability of the objects depicted, though the converse argument does indeed fail. Weisman and Spetch (2010) also argue strongly for the use of playback experiments, which are indeed the most usual and useful method for answering questions driven by (b) above, discovering the releasers of natural responses, but of limited use if what we are really interested in is object representation.
如果我们想知道检查图片集区分是否有用,我们需要问一下这些原因中的哪一个是实验背后的原因。当然,事情从来没有那么简单。研究人员并不总是清楚或一致地知道我们为什么要做实验,而且通常在他们的研究设计和描述中可以发现不止一种动机。此外,那些阅读和引用研究成果的人经常将动机归因于他们实际上并不持有的研究人员,或者以原作者不赞同的方式解释结果。然而,适当的研究技术主要取决于这些动机中的哪一个是有效的,因此,我们应该如何评估实验和我们的结果主要取决于它们所针对的目标。在这篇评论中,我认为Weisman和Spetch(2010)讨论的大多数研究的潜在动机是(c),调查对象表征。然而,Weisman和Spetch(2010)的大部分批评都假设它是(a),调查真实物体的可辨别性。他们提出了一个确凿的证据,反对使用大量已发表的研究来回答这个问题。但不清楚是否有人在被告席上——无论如何,下面将讨论,从图片的可辨别性论证到所描绘物体的可辨别性论证并不是不恰当的,尽管相反的论证确实失败了。Weisman和Spetch(2010)也强烈主张使用回放实验,这确实是回答上述(b)驱动的问题的最常用和有用的方法,发现自然反应的释放,但如果我们真正感兴趣的是对象表示,则使用有限。
{"title":"What's the use of picture discrimination experiments?","authors":"S. Lea","doi":"10.3819/CCBR.2010.50010","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50010","url":null,"abstract":"If we want to know whether it is useful to examine picture set discriminations, we need to ask which of these reasons lies behind the experiment in question. Of course, things are never as simple as that. Researchers are not always either clear or consistent about why we do experiments, and often more than one of these motivations may be detectable in the design and description of their research. Furthermore, those who read and cite research often attribute motives to the researchers that they did not in fact hold, or interpret results in ways that the original authors would not endorse. Nonetheless, the research techniques that are appropriate depend critically on which of these motivations are operative, and it follows that how we should assess the experiments and our results depends critically on which of these goals they are aiming at. In this commentary, I argue that the underlying motivation for most of the research that Weisman and Spetch (2010) discuss is (c) above, to investigate object representation. However, much of Weisman and Spetch’s (2010) critique assumes that it is (a), to investigate the discriminability of the real objects. They build a damning case against the use of much of the published research to answer that question. But it is not clear that there is anyone in the dock – and in any case, it will be argued below that it is not improper to argue from the discriminability of pictures to the discriminability of the objects depicted, though the converse argument does indeed fail. Weisman and Spetch (2010) also argue strongly for the use of playback experiments, which are indeed the most usual and useful method for answering questions driven by (b) above, discovering the releasers of natural responses, but of limited use if what we are really interested in is object representation.","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50010","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70233519","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Picture perception in birds: Perspective from primatologists 鸟类的图像感知:来自灵长类动物学家的视角
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2010-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/CCBR.2010.50007
J. Fagot, C. Parron
In their target article, Weisman and Spetch (2010) question the validity of pictures to present real things to birds, mostly because pictures are primarily made for the human eye, and not for the eye of birds with different functional properties. Here, we argue that this issue of picture validity is similarly critical for primatologists, even when they study the "higher" nonhuman primates with a more similar visual system, and emphasize cognitive limitations in referential abilities that may be an important source of differences in picture processing modes between human and animals. In their target paper, Weisman and Spetch (2010) review the literature on picture perception in birds and adequately question the validity of pictorial stimuli (photographs, digitized pictures, and movies) to present real objects or scenes to these animals. In their review, they raise the important issue that with the current technology, pictures are primarily designed to be perceived by human eyes and might therefore, not be well adapted to birds possessing a different visual system. They document the risk for anthropocentric errors in experimental research using pictures with birds, and call for new empirical evidence showing a correspondence between the perception of pictures and the perception of objects in birds. Despite the widespread use of pictures in animal psychology, investigators have rarely questioned the realistic nature of pictures and their ecological validity. In that respect, Weisman and Spetch's paper (2010) is among the very few to explicitly do so (see also Cabe, 1976, Fagot 2000). They must be applauded for bringing back this important issue to the attention of the field. In general, we are supportive of their position that a greater care must be given to the use of pictures in experimental animal research. However, consideration of the primate literature suggests that
在他们的目标文章中,Weisman和Spetch(2010)质疑图片向鸟类展示真实事物的有效性,主要是因为图片主要是为人眼制作的,而不是为具有不同功能属性的鸟类制作的。在这里,我们认为,对于灵长类动物学家来说,图像有效性问题同样至关重要,即使他们研究具有更相似视觉系统的“高等”非人类灵长类动物,并强调参考能力的认知局限性,这可能是人类和动物之间图像处理模式差异的重要来源。在他们的目标论文中,Weisman和Spetch(2010)回顾了关于鸟类图像感知的文献,并充分质疑了图像刺激(照片、数字化图片和电影)向这些动物呈现真实物体或场景的有效性。在他们的评论中,他们提出了一个重要的问题,即在目前的技术下,图片主要是为人眼所感知的,因此可能不太适合拥有不同视觉系统的鸟类。他们记录了在使用鸟类图片的实验研究中出现以人类为中心的错误的风险,并呼吁找到新的经验证据,证明鸟类对图片的感知与对物体的感知之间存在对应关系。尽管在动物心理学中广泛使用图片,研究人员很少质疑图片的现实性质及其生态有效性。在这方面,Weisman和Spetch的论文(2010)是少数明确这样做的论文之一(另见Cabe, 1976, Fagot 2000)。必须赞扬他们使这一重要问题重新引起该领域的注意。总的来说,我们支持他们的立场,即在实验动物研究中必须更加谨慎地使用图片。然而,考虑到灵长类动物的文献表明
{"title":"Picture perception in birds: Perspective from primatologists","authors":"J. Fagot, C. Parron","doi":"10.3819/CCBR.2010.50007","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50007","url":null,"abstract":"In their target article, Weisman and Spetch (2010) question the validity of pictures to present real things to birds, mostly because pictures are primarily made for the human eye, and not for the eye of birds with different functional properties. Here, we argue that this issue of picture validity is similarly critical for primatologists, even when they study the \"higher\" nonhuman primates with a more similar visual system, and emphasize cognitive limitations in referential abilities that may be an important source of differences in picture processing modes between human and animals. In their target paper, Weisman and Spetch (2010) review the literature on picture perception in birds and adequately question the validity of pictorial stimuli (photographs, digitized pictures, and movies) to present real objects or scenes to these animals. In their review, they raise the important issue that with the current technology, pictures are primarily designed to be perceived by human eyes and might therefore, not be well adapted to birds possessing a different visual system. They document the risk for anthropocentric errors in experimental research using pictures with birds, and call for new empirical evidence showing a correspondence between the perception of pictures and the perception of objects in birds. Despite the widespread use of pictures in animal psychology, investigators have rarely questioned the realistic nature of pictures and their ecological validity. In that respect, Weisman and Spetch's paper (2010) is among the very few to explicitly do so (see also Cabe, 1976, Fagot 2000). They must be applauded for bringing back this important issue to the attention of the field. In general, we are supportive of their position that a greater care must be given to the use of pictures in experimental animal research. However, consideration of the primate literature suggests that","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50007","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70232821","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Rudiments of mind: Insights through the chick model on number and space cognition in animals 心灵的雏形:通过小鸡模型对动物数量和空间认知的洞察
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2010-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/CCBR.2010.50004
G. Vallortigara, L. Regolin, C. Chiandetti, R. Rugani
Studies on human infants, focused on the ontogenetic origins of knowledge, have provided evidence for a small set of separable systems of core knowledge dealing with the representation of objects, number, and space. We investigated core knowledge systems from a comparative perspective, making use of the domestic chick as a model system, and filial imprinting as a key to animal mind. Here, we discuss evidence showing precocious abilities in the chick for representing: (i) the cardinal and ordinal/sequential aspects of numerical cognition, and (ii) the distance, angle, and sense relations among extended surfaces in the surrounding layout. Some of the abilities associated with core knowledge systems of number and space were observed in the absence of (or with very reduced) visual experience, supporting a nativistic account of the origins of knowledge.
对人类婴儿的研究,集中在知识的个体起源上,已经为一小部分处理对象、数字和空间表示的核心知识的可分离系统提供了证据。我们从比较的角度考察了核心知识系统,以家鸡为模型系统,以孝子印记为动物心智的关键。在这里,我们讨论了小鸡表现出早熟能力的证据:(i)数字认知的基数和序数/顺序方面,以及(ii)周围布局中扩展表面之间的距离、角度和感觉关系。一些与数字和空间的核心知识系统相关的能力是在缺乏(或非常减少)视觉经验的情况下观察到的,这支持了知识起源的本土论。
{"title":"Rudiments of mind: Insights through the chick model on number and space cognition in animals","authors":"G. Vallortigara, L. Regolin, C. Chiandetti, R. Rugani","doi":"10.3819/CCBR.2010.50004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50004","url":null,"abstract":"Studies on human infants, focused on the ontogenetic origins of knowledge, have provided evidence for a small set of separable systems of core knowledge dealing with the representation of objects, number, and space. We investigated core knowledge systems from a comparative perspective, making use of the domestic chick as a model system, and filial imprinting as a key to animal mind. Here, we discuss evidence showing precocious abilities in the chick for representing: (i) the cardinal and ordinal/sequential aspects of numerical cognition, and (ii) the distance, angle, and sense relations among extended surfaces in the surrounding layout. Some of the abilities associated with core knowledge systems of number and space were observed in the absence of (or with very reduced) visual experience, supporting a nativistic account of the origins of knowledge.","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50004","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70232873","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 49
Do animals recognize pictures as representations of 3D objects 动物能识别3D物体的图片吗
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2010-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/CCBR.2010.50008
M. Jitsumori
A picture is something in which objects can be recognized, even though the objects themselves are not actually present when it is observed. For observers to recognize objects in a picture, visual inputs have to be matched to the representations of the visual objects in memory. Patients who suffer from visual agnosia often exhibit greater impairments in recognizing line-drawings and photographs than real objects, while leaving abilities of perceiving 2D features intact. Hiraoka, Suzuki, Hirayama, and Mori (2009) reported a patient who does not show apparent impairment with real objects and even their photographs, but has difficulties in recognizing line-drawings and silhouettes (i.e., the stimuli are missing much of 2D features of the real objects). In contrast, Turnbull, Driver, and McCarthy (2004) reported the performance of a patient on a variety of experimental tasks that investigated the patient’s ability to extract 2D and 3D information from 2D stimuli. His performance was relatively intact on the 2D tasks (e.g., picture-matching across rotations in the picture plane), but was greatly impaired on the 3D tasks (mental rotation in depth, relative depth judgments within line-drawings and shaded photographs, and the discrimination of line-drawings depicting “possible” and “impossible” 3D objects). The finding suggested that his difficulties in recognizing visually presented objects was due to the deficit in deriving 3D structure from the pictorial depth-cues within 2D images. It has also been reported that patients with agnosic deficits often show a greater impairment in the recognition of visually presented living things (e.g., animals) as compared to non-living things (e.g., tools). Wolk, Coslett, and Glosser (2005) argued that such category-specific visual agnosia suggests that information about an object’s form, as well as sensory-motor information specifying the manner of manipulation, may contribute to object recognition. Generally, visual agnosia is assumed to be derived from impairments in the processes by which visual information contacts stored knowledge of objects or representations of 3D objects in memory.
在一幅画中,物体是可以被识别出来的,即使在观察它的时候,物体本身并不存在。为了让观察者识别图片中的物体,视觉输入必须与记忆中视觉物体的表示相匹配。患有视觉失认症的患者通常在识别线条画和照片方面表现出比真实物体更大的障碍,而感知二维特征的能力却完好无损。Hiraoka, Suzuki, Hirayama, and Mori(2009)报道了一名患者,该患者对真实物体甚至照片都没有表现出明显的损伤,但在识别线条画和轮廓方面存在困难(即,刺激缺失了真实物体的许多二维特征)。相比之下,Turnbull, Driver和McCarthy(2004)报道了患者在各种实验任务中的表现,这些实验任务调查了患者从2D刺激中提取2D和3D信息的能力。他在2D任务中的表现相对完好(例如,在画面平面的旋转中进行图片匹配),但在3D任务中(深度的心理旋转,线条图和阴影照片中的相对深度判断,以及描绘“可能”和“不可能”3D物体的线条图的区分),他的表现大大受损。这一发现表明,他在识别视觉上呈现的物体方面的困难是由于在从2D图像的图像深度线索中导出3D结构方面的缺陷。也有报道称,与非生物(如工具)相比,患有失认症的患者在识别视觉上呈现的生物(如动物)方面往往表现出更大的障碍。Wolk, Coslett和Glosser(2005)认为,这种类别特异性视觉失认症表明,关于物体形状的信息,以及指定操作方式的感觉运动信息,可能有助于物体识别。一般来说,视觉失认症被认为是由于视觉信息接触过程的障碍,存储的物体知识或三维物体的表征在记忆中。
{"title":"Do animals recognize pictures as representations of 3D objects","authors":"M. Jitsumori","doi":"10.3819/CCBR.2010.50008","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50008","url":null,"abstract":"A picture is something in which objects can be recognized, even though the objects themselves are not actually present when it is observed. For observers to recognize objects in a picture, visual inputs have to be matched to the representations of the visual objects in memory. Patients who suffer from visual agnosia often exhibit greater impairments in recognizing line-drawings and photographs than real objects, while leaving abilities of perceiving 2D features intact. Hiraoka, Suzuki, Hirayama, and Mori (2009) reported a patient who does not show apparent impairment with real objects and even their photographs, but has difficulties in recognizing line-drawings and silhouettes (i.e., the stimuli are missing much of 2D features of the real objects). In contrast, Turnbull, Driver, and McCarthy (2004) reported the performance of a patient on a variety of experimental tasks that investigated the patient’s ability to extract 2D and 3D information from 2D stimuli. His performance was relatively intact on the 2D tasks (e.g., picture-matching across rotations in the picture plane), but was greatly impaired on the 3D tasks (mental rotation in depth, relative depth judgments within line-drawings and shaded photographs, and the discrimination of line-drawings depicting “possible” and “impossible” 3D objects). The finding suggested that his difficulties in recognizing visually presented objects was due to the deficit in deriving 3D structure from the pictorial depth-cues within 2D images. It has also been reported that patients with agnosic deficits often show a greater impairment in the recognition of visually presented living things (e.g., animals) as compared to non-living things (e.g., tools). Wolk, Coslett, and Glosser (2005) argued that such category-specific visual agnosia suggests that information about an object’s form, as well as sensory-motor information specifying the manner of manipulation, may contribute to object recognition. Generally, visual agnosia is assumed to be derived from impairments in the processes by which visual information contacts stored knowledge of objects or representations of 3D objects in memory.","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50008","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70232991","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Understanding how pictures are seen is important for comparative visual cognition 理解图片是如何被看到的对于比较视觉认知是很重要的
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2010-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/CCBR.2010.50013
M. Spetch
My motivation for co-authoring the target article (Weisman & Spetch, 2010) was two-fold. First, l hoped it would restimulate discussion and consideration of the questions, issues and limitations surrounding the use of pictures as surrogates for real world objects in comparative cognition research (Bovet & Vauclair, 2000; Fagot, 2000). Second, I hoped it would encourage more research aimed at assessing the real world validity of findings from animal research using pictures. The commentaries on our target article reveal a spicy variety of reactions and opinions, which is encouraging for the first goal. The commentators made many excellent points, some which I agree with and some which I question. My coauthor, Ron Weisman has provided a detailed reply to many of the commentaries so I will limit my reply to a few selected points.
我与人合著这篇目标文章(Weisman & Spetch, 2010)的动机是双重的。首先,我希望它能重新激发人们对在比较认知研究中使用图片代替现实世界物体的问题、问题和局限性的讨论和考虑(Bovet & Vauclair, 2000;束,2000)。其次,我希望它能鼓励更多旨在利用图片评估动物研究结果在现实世界中的有效性的研究。对我们目标文章的评论揭示了各种各样的反应和观点,这对第一个目标是令人鼓舞的。评论员提出了许多很好的观点,有些我同意,有些我质疑。我的合著者Ron Weisman已经对许多评论提供了详细的回复,所以我将把我的回复限制在几个要点上。
{"title":"Understanding how pictures are seen is important for comparative visual cognition","authors":"M. Spetch","doi":"10.3819/CCBR.2010.50013","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50013","url":null,"abstract":"My motivation for co-authoring the target article (Weisman & Spetch, 2010) was two-fold. First, l hoped it would restimulate discussion and consideration of the questions, issues and limitations surrounding the use of pictures as surrogates for real world objects in comparative cognition research (Bovet & Vauclair, 2000; Fagot, 2000). Second, I hoped it would encourage more research aimed at assessing the real world validity of findings from animal research using pictures. The commentaries on our target article reveal a spicy variety of reactions and opinions, which is encouraging for the first goal. The commentators made many excellent points, some which I agree with and some which I question. My coauthor, Ron Weisman has provided a detailed reply to many of the commentaries so I will limit my reply to a few selected points.","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50013","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70233478","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Comparative Vision Science: Seeing Eye to Eye? 比较视觉科学:眼对眼?
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2010-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/ccbr.2010.50011
Fabian A Soto, Edward A Wasserman

In the study of comparative cognition and perception, disparities in the diverse approaches that researchers take in studying behavior sometimes obscure the interpretation of a particular empirical finding. We describe an approach to the study of comparative cognition and perception which focuses on explaining the ways in which different biological systems solve the computational challenges that are posed by their natural environments. Within this investigative framework, the task of detecting correspondence between a three-dimensional object and its two-dimensional photographic representation falls outside the mainstream of most research in animal visual cognition and is of limited value for divulging the principles or mechanisms that underlie the visual abilities of animals. More productive pursuits seek to elucidate the principles and mechanisms of object recognition and categorization, and to illuminate how they contribute to the animal's survival in the visual world.

在比较认知和知觉的研究中,研究人员在研究行为时采用的不同方法的差异有时会模糊对特定实证发现的解释。我们描述了一种研究比较认知和感知的方法,其重点是解释不同生物系统解决其自然环境所带来的计算挑战的方式。在这个调查框架内,检测三维物体与其二维图像表现之间的对应关系的任务超出了大多数动物视觉认知研究的主流,并且对于揭示动物视觉能力的原理或机制的价值有限。更有成效的追求是寻求阐明物体识别和分类的原理和机制,并阐明它们如何有助于动物在视觉世界中的生存。
{"title":"Comparative Vision Science: Seeing Eye to Eye?","authors":"Fabian A Soto,&nbsp;Edward A Wasserman","doi":"10.3819/ccbr.2010.50011","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/ccbr.2010.50011","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the study of comparative cognition and perception, disparities in the diverse approaches that researchers take in studying behavior sometimes obscure the interpretation of a particular empirical finding. We describe an approach to the study of comparative cognition and perception which focuses on explaining the ways in which different biological systems solve the computational challenges that are posed by their natural environments. Within this investigative framework, the task of detecting correspondence between a three-dimensional object and its two-dimensional photographic representation falls outside the mainstream of most research in animal visual cognition and is of limited value for divulging the principles or mechanisms that underlie the visual abilities of animals. More productive pursuits seek to elucidate the principles and mechanisms of object recognition and categorization, and to illuminate how they contribute to the animal's survival in the visual world.</p>","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3819/ccbr.2010.50011","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"29291656","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
Cross-species Assessment of the Linguistic Origins of Color Categories 颜色类别语言起源的跨物种评估
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2010-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/CCBR.2010.50005
J. Davidoff, J. Fagot
This article considers the relation between language and categorical perception (CP) of colour. Two opposite theories are reviewed, the universalist position arguing that categories are universal with an essentially biological origin, and the relativist position that holds that colour categories are essentially arbitrary and derive from colour terms of the speaker’s language. A review of the human literature presents developmental, neuropsychological, cross-­cultural, neuro-­imaging and computer simulation evidence that CP of colours has at least partly linguistic origins. As animal studies also contribute to this debate, we then review evidence of CP in the visual and auditory domains, and pinpoint the inconsistencies of the literature. To make a direct comparison between humans and monkeys, experimental studies compared humans and baboons for their colour thresholds and in a recognition memory task designed to assess CP of colours. Only humans showed better between-­category than within-­category discrimination performance, suggesting species differences in the processing of a colour continuum. That study along with some of our previous research supports the theory of a linguistic origin for colour categories in humans.
本文探讨了语言与颜色范畴知觉之间的关系。本文回顾了两种相反的理论,普遍主义立场认为,颜色类别是普遍的,本质上是生物起源,而相对主义立场认为,颜色类别本质上是任意的,源于说话者语言的颜色术语。对人类文献的回顾提出了发育、神经心理学、跨文化、神经成像和计算机模拟的证据,表明颜色CP至少部分源于语言。由于动物研究也有助于这一争论,我们随后回顾了CP在视觉和听觉领域的证据,并指出文献中的不一致之处。为了直接比较人类和猴子,实验研究比较了人类和狒狒的颜色阈值,并在一项旨在评估颜色CP的识别记忆任务中进行了比较。只有人类表现出比类别内更好的类别间辨别表现,这表明物种在处理颜色连续体方面存在差异。这项研究以及我们之前的一些研究支持了人类颜色类别的语言起源理论。
{"title":"Cross-species Assessment of the Linguistic Origins of Color Categories","authors":"J. Davidoff, J. Fagot","doi":"10.3819/CCBR.2010.50005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50005","url":null,"abstract":"This article considers the relation between language and categorical perception (CP) of colour. Two opposite theories are reviewed, the universalist position arguing that categories are universal with an essentially biological origin, and the relativist position that holds that colour categories are essentially arbitrary and derive from colour terms of the speaker’s language. A review of the human literature presents developmental, neuropsychological, cross-­cultural, neuro-­imaging and computer simulation evidence that CP of colours has at least partly linguistic origins. As animal studies also contribute to this debate, we then review evidence of CP in the visual and auditory domains, and pinpoint the inconsistencies of the literature. To make a direct comparison between humans and monkeys, experimental studies compared humans and baboons for their colour thresholds and in a recognition memory task designed to assess CP of colours. Only humans showed better between-­category than within-­category discrimination performance, suggesting species differences in the processing of a colour continuum. That study along with some of our previous research supports the theory of a linguistic origin for colour categories in humans.","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50005","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70233102","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 15
On Categories, Pictures, and the Goals of Comparative Psychology 论比较心理学的范畴、图景与目标
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2010-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/CCBR.2010.50009
O. Lazareva
The ability to categorize objects and events has long been an object of an intense interest and rigorous research in both humans and nonhuman animals (see Lazareva & Wasserman, 2008; Mareschal, Quinn, & Lea, 2010, for reviews). Until the seminal Herrnstein and Loveland’s study (1964), most of the comparative research used simple and well-defined stimuli (e.g., 1000-Hz tone or a 450 nm light) as discriminanda. Although easily controllable, such stimuli have little relationship to the tasks faced by animals in their natural environments: After all, discriminating a hawk from a conspecific is unlikely to be based on the difference in a single wavelength or pure tone.
对物体和事件进行分类的能力长期以来一直是人类和非人类动物强烈兴趣和严格研究的对象(见Lazareva & Wasserman, 2008;Mareschal, Quinn, & Lea, 2010,供评论)。直到开创性的Herrnstein和Loveland的研究(1964),大多数比较研究使用简单和定义明确的刺激(例如,1000赫兹的音调或450纳米的光)作为判别。虽然很容易控制,但这种刺激与动物在自然环境中面临的任务几乎没有关系:毕竟,区分一只鹰和一只同种动物不太可能基于单一波长或纯音的差异。
{"title":"On Categories, Pictures, and the Goals of Comparative Psychology","authors":"O. Lazareva","doi":"10.3819/CCBR.2010.50009","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50009","url":null,"abstract":"The ability to categorize objects and events has long been an object of an intense interest and rigorous research in both humans and nonhuman animals (see Lazareva & Wasserman, 2008; Mareschal, Quinn, & Lea, 2010, for reviews). Until the seminal Herrnstein and Loveland’s study (1964), most of the comparative research used simple and well-defined stimuli (e.g., 1000-Hz tone or a 450 nm light) as discriminanda. Although easily controllable, such stimuli have little relationship to the tasks faced by animals in their natural environments: After all, discriminating a hawk from a conspecific is unlikely to be based on the difference in a single wavelength or pure tone.","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3819/CCBR.2010.50009","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70233127","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Time and Associative Learning. 时间与联想学习
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2010-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/ccbr.2010.50001
Peter D Balsam, Michael R Drew, C R Gallistel

In a basic associative learning paradigm, learning is said to have occurred when the conditioned stimulus evokes an anticipatory response. This learning is widely believed to depend on the contiguous presentation of conditioned and unconditioned stimulus. However, what it means to be contiguous has not been rigorously defined. Here we examine the empirical bases for these beliefs and suggest an alternative view based on the hypothesis that learning about the temporal relationships between events determines the speed of emergence, vigor and form of conditioned behavior. This temporal learning occurs very rapidly and prior to the appearance of the anticipatory response. The temporal relations are learned even when no anticipatory response is evoked. The speed with which an anticipatory response emerges is proportional to the informativeness of the predictive cue (CS) regarding the rate of occurrence of the predicted event (US). This analysis gives an account of what we mean by "temporal pairing" and is in accord with the data on speed of acquisition and basic findings in the cue competition literature. In this account, learning depends on perceiving and encoding temporal regularities rather than stimulus contiguities.

在基本的联想学习范式中,当条件刺激唤起预期反应时,学习就发生了。人们普遍认为,这种学习取决于条件刺激和非条件刺激的连续呈现。然而,"连续 "的含义尚未得到严格定义。在这里,我们研究了这些观点的经验基础,并提出了另一种观点,即对事件间时间关系的学习决定了条件行为出现的速度、强度和形式。这种时间学习发生得非常快,而且是在预期反应出现之前。即使在没有诱发预期反应的情况下,时间关系也会被学习。预期反应出现的速度与预测线索(CS)在预测事件(US)发生率方面的信息量成正比。这一分析说明了我们所说的 "时间配对 "的含义,并与关于习得速度的数据和线索竞争文献中的基本发现相一致。在这种解释中,学习取决于对时间规律性的感知和编码,而不是刺激的连续性。
{"title":"Time and Associative Learning.","authors":"Peter D Balsam, Michael R Drew, C R Gallistel","doi":"10.3819/ccbr.2010.50001","DOIUrl":"10.3819/ccbr.2010.50001","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a basic associative learning paradigm, learning is said to have occurred when the conditioned stimulus evokes an anticipatory response. This learning is widely believed to depend on the contiguous presentation of conditioned and unconditioned stimulus. However, what it means to be contiguous has not been rigorously defined. Here we examine the empirical bases for these beliefs and suggest an alternative view based on the hypothesis that learning about the temporal relationships between events determines the speed of emergence, vigor and form of conditioned behavior. This temporal learning occurs very rapidly and prior to the appearance of the anticipatory response. The temporal relations are learned even when no anticipatory response is evoked. The speed with which an anticipatory response emerges is proportional to the informativeness of the predictive cue (CS) regarding the rate of occurrence of the predicted event (US). This analysis gives an account of what we mean by \"temporal pairing\" and is in accord with the data on speed of acquisition and basic findings in the cue competition literature. In this account, learning depends on perceiving and encoding temporal regularities rather than stimulus contiguities.</p>","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3045055/pdf/nihms207291.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"29704702","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1