首页 > 最新文献

Japanese Psychological Research最新文献

英文 中文
Assessment of Construct Validity of Recovery Measures through Theoretical Formulations as Applied to Cardiovascular Parameters 通过应用于心血管参数的理论公式评估恢复测量的结构有效性
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-22 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12492
Yukihiro Sawada
The recovery period is generally excluded under stress testing, despite its importance, because recovery measures lack information on construct validity. The smaller the carryover effect, the higher the construct validity. Theoretical formulae were derived for three recovery measures (i.e., total carryover [TCO], mean recovery rate [MRR], and area under the curve [AUC]) to assess to what extent this criterion is fulfilled. The cardiovascular parameters (particularly blood pressure [BP] and heart rate [HR]) were targeted. After applying this criterion to theoretical formulae and previous experimental findings, the three recovery measures were ordered as follows: MRR for BP (highly valid) > AUC for both BP and HR (moderately valid) > MRR for HR and TCO for both BP and HR (slightly valid). The unexpected finding of MRR for HR was due to the invisible carryover effect of vagal rebound on HR. The implication of the sharp contrast in MRR for BP versus HR is discussed, indicating their qualitative differences as cardiovascular parameters.
尽管恢复期很重要,但在压力测试中一般不包括恢复期,因为恢复期的测量方法缺乏构造有效性方面的信息。结转效应越小,结构效度越高。我们推导出了三种恢复测量指标(即总带量[TCO]、平均恢复率[MRR]和曲线下面积[AUC])的理论公式,以评估在多大程度上符合这一标准。心血管参数(尤其是血压[BP]和心率[HR])是目标。在将这一标准应用于理论公式和以往的实验结果后,三种恢复测量方法的排序如下:血压的 MRR(高度有效)> 血压和心率的 AUC(中度有效)> 心率的 MRR 以及血压和心率的 TCO(轻微有效)。心率的 MRR 这一意外发现是由于迷走神经反弹对心率的隐形传导效应。讨论了血压与心率的 MRR 形成鲜明对比的含义,表明它们作为心血管参数存在质的差异。
{"title":"Assessment of Construct Validity of Recovery Measures through Theoretical Formulations as Applied to Cardiovascular Parameters","authors":"Yukihiro Sawada","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12492","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12492","url":null,"abstract":"The recovery period is generally excluded under stress testing, despite its importance, because recovery measures lack information on construct validity. The smaller the carryover effect, the higher the construct validity. Theoretical formulae were derived for three recovery measures (i.e., total carryover [TCO], mean recovery rate [MRR], and area under the curve [AUC]) to assess to what extent this criterion is fulfilled. The cardiovascular parameters (particularly blood pressure [BP] and heart rate [HR]) were targeted. After applying this criterion to theoretical formulae and previous experimental findings, the three recovery measures were ordered as follows: MRR for BP (highly valid) > AUC for both BP and HR (moderately valid) > MRR for HR and TCO for both BP and HR (slightly valid). The unexpected finding of MRR for HR was due to the invisible carryover effect of vagal rebound on HR. The implication of the sharp contrast in MRR for BP versus HR is discussed, indicating their qualitative differences as cardiovascular parameters.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2023-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139246903","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Achievement Goal Impacts Students' Preferences for “Personalized Problems” in Computer-Adaptive Tests1 成绩目标影响学生对计算机自适应测试中 "个性化问题 "的偏好1
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-22 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12485
Takayuki Goto, Kei Kano, Takayuki Shiose

The contemporary development of psychometric theories and information technologies enables students to work on algorithm-based personalized tests in classroom settings. This study aims to investigate the relationship between students' achievement goals and what they prefer as “personalized problems” in computer-adaptive tests. We theoretically contrast achievement goals with developing competency through mastery and demonstrating competency through performance goals. We asked elementary and secondary school students to work on a computer-adaptive test and to complete questionnaires about what they prefer as “personalized problems” in computer-adaptive tests. The results revealed that while mastery goals positively predicted preference for challenging problems, performance goals positively predicted preference for problems that guaranteed students’ success. Moreover, only the preference for challenging problems positively predicted the intention to take computer-adaptive tests in the future. These results suggest that simply introducing a computer-adaptive test into the classroom may not be effective. We discuss how educational technologies should be integrated into human teaching activities.

当代心理测量理论和信息技术的发展,使学生能够在课堂上进行基于算法的个性化测试。本研究旨在探究学生的成绩目标与他们喜欢的计算机自适应测试中的 "个性化问题 "之间的关系。我们从理论上将成就目标与通过掌握来发展能力和通过表现目标来展示能力进行了对比。我们要求中小学生进行计算机自适应测试,并就他们喜欢的计算机自适应测试中的 "个性化问题 "填写调查问卷。结果表明,掌握目标对挑战性问题的偏好呈正向预测,而成绩目标对保证学生成功的问题的偏好呈正向预测。此外,只有对具有挑战性问题的偏好才会对今后参加计算机自适应测试的意愿产生积极的预测作用。这些结果表明,仅仅在课堂上引入计算机自适应测试可能并不有效。我们讨论了教育技术应如何与人类教学活动相结合。
{"title":"Achievement Goal Impacts Students' Preferences for “Personalized Problems” in Computer-Adaptive Tests1","authors":"Takayuki Goto,&nbsp;Kei Kano,&nbsp;Takayuki Shiose","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12485","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jpr.12485","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The contemporary development of psychometric theories and information technologies enables students to work on algorithm-based personalized tests in classroom settings. This study aims to investigate the relationship between students' achievement goals and what they prefer as “personalized problems” in computer-adaptive tests. We theoretically contrast achievement goals with developing competency through mastery and demonstrating competency through performance goals. We asked elementary and secondary school students to work on a computer-adaptive test and to complete questionnaires about what they prefer as “personalized problems” in computer-adaptive tests. The results revealed that while mastery goals positively predicted preference for challenging problems, performance goals positively predicted preference for problems that guaranteed students’ success. Moreover, only the preference for challenging problems positively predicted the intention to take computer-adaptive tests in the future. These results suggest that simply introducing a computer-adaptive test into the classroom may not be effective. We discuss how educational technologies should be integrated into human teaching activities.</p>","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2023-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jpr.12485","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139247154","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychometric Examination of the Japanese Version of the Emotion Beliefs Questionnaire1234 日文版情感信念问卷的心理测试1234
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-22 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12491
M. Kashimura, K. Ishizu, Rodrigo Becerra
Emotion beliefs include beliefs regarding usefulness and controllability of emotions, and they play an important role in emotion regulation. This study developed a Japanese version of the Emotion Beliefs Questionnaire (EBQ‐J) and examined its reliability and validity. A total of 317 participants were surveyed (212 women, 98 men, 7 not indicated; mean age: 20.06 ± 2.84 years). A preliminary survey was conducted with 23 undergraduate students to develop a draft of the Japanese version. The results confirmed the face validity of the measurement. The first cross‐sectional survey examined descriptive statistics, structural validity, internal consistency, construct validity, and gender differences in measurement scores. The second, short‐term longitudinal and cross‐sectional survey evaluated the test–retest reliability and construct validity. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the EBQ‐J had a four‐first‐order‐factor and two‐second‐order‐factor structure. The results were consistent with the original EBQ. The measurement demonstrated good internal consistency, moderate to excellent test–retest reliability, and partially verified construct validity. The EBQ‐J could assess emotion beliefs across positive and negative emotions and explain emotion regulation processes.
情绪信念包括对情绪有用性和可控性的信念,在情绪调节中起着重要作用。本研究编制了日语版情绪信念问卷(EBQ-J),并对其信度和效度进行了检验。共调查了 317 名参与者(212 名女性,98 名男性,7 名未说明;平均年龄:20.06 ± 2.84 岁)。为了编写日语版本的草稿,对 23 名本科生进行了初步调查。结果证实了测量的面效度。第一项横向调查研究了描述性统计、结构效度、内部一致性、建构效度以及测量得分的性别差异。第二项短期纵向和横向调查评估了重复测试的可靠性和结构效度。确认性因素分析表明,EBQ-J 具有四个一阶因素和两个二阶因素结构。结果与原始 EBQ 一致。测量结果显示了良好的内部一致性、中等至优秀的测试-再测信度以及部分验证的建构效度。EBQ-J可以评估积极情绪和消极情绪的情绪信念,并解释情绪调节过程。
{"title":"Psychometric Examination of the Japanese Version of the Emotion Beliefs Questionnaire1234","authors":"M. Kashimura, K. Ishizu, Rodrigo Becerra","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12491","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12491","url":null,"abstract":"Emotion beliefs include beliefs regarding usefulness and controllability of emotions, and they play an important role in emotion regulation. This study developed a Japanese version of the Emotion Beliefs Questionnaire (EBQ‐J) and examined its reliability and validity. A total of 317 participants were surveyed (212 women, 98 men, 7 not indicated; mean age: 20.06 ± 2.84 years). A preliminary survey was conducted with 23 undergraduate students to develop a draft of the Japanese version. The results confirmed the face validity of the measurement. The first cross‐sectional survey examined descriptive statistics, structural validity, internal consistency, construct validity, and gender differences in measurement scores. The second, short‐term longitudinal and cross‐sectional survey evaluated the test–retest reliability and construct validity. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the EBQ‐J had a four‐first‐order‐factor and two‐second‐order‐factor structure. The results were consistent with the original EBQ. The measurement demonstrated good internal consistency, moderate to excellent test–retest reliability, and partially verified construct validity. The EBQ‐J could assess emotion beliefs across positive and negative emotions and explain emotion regulation processes.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2023-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139247201","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Approach–Avoidance Responses to Affective Facial Expressions and Bodily Posture1,2 对情感面部表情和身体姿态的接近-回避反应1,2
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-19 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12487
Shinnosuke Ikeda
Human beings perceive the emotions of others through various cues, such as facial expressions, voice, and bodily posture. These social signals have been acquired evolutionarily, and reports suggest that emotions are recognized to some extent in a culturally universal pattern. It has also been suggested that an observer's approach or avoidance responses toward the expressor occur at the initial stage of emotion perception. However, such approach–avoidance reactions have hitherto been examined mainly in response to facial expressions and not bodily postures. Therefore, this study examined approach–avoidance responses to anger and fear as visualized through facial expressions and bodily postures. The study sample comprised 58 university students. The results showed that, as in a previous study, approach responses to fear and avoidance responses to anger were dominant in both facial expression and bodily posture conditions. This suggests that bodily posture and facial expression are social signals that can elicit an approach–avoidance response from the observer.
人类通过面部表情、声音和身体姿势等各种线索感知他人的情绪。这些社会信号是在进化过程中获得的,有报告表明,情绪在某种程度上是以一种文化通用模式被识别的。也有人认为,观察者对表达者的接近或回避反应发生在情绪感知的初始阶段。然而,迄今为止,这种接近-回避反应主要是针对面部表情而非身体姿势进行研究的。因此,本研究考察了通过面部表情和身体姿势视觉化的愤怒和恐惧的接近-回避反应。研究样本包括 58 名大学生。结果显示,与之前的研究一样,在面部表情和身体姿势条件下,对恐惧的接近反应和对愤怒的回避反应都占主导地位。这表明,身体姿势和面部表情是一种社会信号,可以引起观察者的接近-回避反应。
{"title":"Approach–Avoidance Responses to Affective Facial Expressions and Bodily Posture1,2","authors":"Shinnosuke Ikeda","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12487","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12487","url":null,"abstract":"Human beings perceive the emotions of others through various cues, such as facial expressions, voice, and bodily posture. These social signals have been acquired evolutionarily, and reports suggest that emotions are recognized to some extent in a culturally universal pattern. It has also been suggested that an observer's approach or avoidance responses toward the expressor occur at the initial stage of emotion perception. However, such approach–avoidance reactions have hitherto been examined mainly in response to facial expressions and not bodily postures. Therefore, this study examined approach–avoidance responses to anger and fear as visualized through facial expressions and bodily postures. The study sample comprised 58 university students. The results showed that, as in a previous study, approach responses to fear and avoidance responses to anger were dominant in both facial expression and bodily posture conditions. This suggests that bodily posture and facial expression are social signals that can elicit an approach–avoidance response from the observer.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2023-11-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139260113","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Relation between Motivation and Goal Attainment: A Correlational Meta-Analysis 动机与目标实现之间的关系:相关性元分析
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-19 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12486
Ahlam Lee

This meta-analysis investigated the strengths of the relationship between various types of motivations and accompanying future outcomes that individuals intend to change, based upon 337 effect sizes from 62 studies. Considerable variation exists within and between the effect sizes of the 14 types of motivations, ranging from a small negative effect size, r = −.13, I2 = 93.85% (k = 13), to a medium positive effect size, r = .38, I2 = 0.0% (k = 3). The following factors moderated some of the 14 summary effect sizes: (a) the type of assessment data (self-report vs. physical data); (b) the type of future outcomes (physical behavior, psychological state, and intellectual ability); (c) the use of a motivational intervention; (d) the use of a longitudinal design; and (e) the time period between the point that measured motivation and future outcomes. The moderating effects suggest that the effect size of motivations may fluctuate across various domains, while future outcomes may be almost unaffected or even affected negatively by particular types of motivations, although certain other types of motivations play positive roles.

这项荟萃分析基于 62 项研究的 337 个效应大小,调查了各种类型的动机与个人打算改变的未来结果之间的关系强度。14 种动机的效应大小内部和之间存在很大差异,从小幅负效应大小(r = -.13,I2 = 93.85% (k = 13))到中幅正效应大小(r = .38,I2 = 0.0% (k = 3))不等。以下因素调节了 14 个摘要效应大小中的一些效应:(a) 评估数据的类型(自我报告与身体数据);(b) 未来结果的类型(身体行为、心理状态和智力能力);(c) 动机干预的使用;(d) 纵向设计的使用;(e) 衡量动机的时间点与未来结果之间的时间段。调节效应表明,动机的效应大小可能会在各个领域波动,而未来结果可能几乎不受特定类型动机的影响,甚至会受到负面影响,尽管某些其他类型的动机发挥了积极作用。
{"title":"The Relation between Motivation and Goal Attainment: A Correlational Meta-Analysis","authors":"Ahlam Lee","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12486","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jpr.12486","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This meta-analysis investigated the strengths of the relationship between various types of motivations and accompanying future outcomes that individuals intend to change, based upon 337 effect sizes from 62 studies. Considerable variation exists within and between the effect sizes of the 14 types of motivations, ranging from a small negative effect size, <i>r</i> = −.13, <i>I</i><sup>2</sup> = 93.85% (<i>k</i> = 13), to a medium positive effect size, <i>r</i> = .38, <i>I</i><sup>2</sup> = 0.0% (<i>k</i> = 3). The following factors moderated some of the 14 summary effect sizes: (a) the type of assessment data (self-report vs. physical data); (b) the type of future outcomes (physical behavior, psychological state, and intellectual ability); (c) the use of a motivational intervention; (d) the use of a longitudinal design; and (e) the time period between the point that measured motivation and future outcomes. The moderating effects suggest that the effect size of motivations may fluctuate across various domains, while future outcomes may be almost unaffected or even affected negatively by particular types of motivations, although certain other types of motivations play positive roles.</p>","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2023-11-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jpr.12486","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139260613","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do Physical Attractiveness and Personality Traits Predict Romantic Partner Evaluations? A Speed‐Dating Study in Japan1,2 外貌吸引力和人格特质能预测浪漫伴侣的评价吗?日本快速约会研究1,2
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-19 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12489
M. Kito, Toshihiko Souma, Takashi Nishimura, Junko Yamada, Yuji Kanemasa, Junichi Taniguchi, Taishi Kawamoto
The speed‐dating procedure allows researchers to closely investigate relationship‐initiation situations. Although previous speed‐dating studies identified physical attractiveness, earning prospects, and reciprocity as predictors of romantic attraction, few such studies have been conducted outside Europe or North America. The current speed‐dating study examined what factors would predict attraction ratings by potential partners among the Japanese—an understudied sample. Participants included 27 men and 28 women who first completed a questionnaire to measure individual attributes. During each speed‐dating session, 12–15 men and women talked for 3 minutes and evaluated each opposite‐gender participant on attraction and dating intentions. Results of the social relations model showed that men evaluated women who were more physically attractive and achieved higher education as more attractive. These results imply that men tend to generalize their first impressions of women's physical attractiveness to other aspects of the partner, and that Japanese men may consider dual income as a necessity for marriage.
通过快速约会程序,研究人员可以密切调查恋爱关系的启动情况。尽管之前的快速约会研究发现,外貌吸引力、收入前景和互惠性是恋爱吸引力的预测因素,但在欧洲或北美以外的地区很少有此类研究。本次速配研究探讨了哪些因素可以预测潜在伴侣对日本人--一个研究不足的样本--的吸引力评级。参与者包括 27 名男性和 28 名女性,他们首先填写了一份调查问卷,以测量个人特质。在每次快速约会过程中,12-15 名男性和女性交谈 3 分钟,并对每位异性参与者的吸引力和约会意向进行评估。社会关系模型的结果表明,男性认为外表更迷人、学历更高的女性更有吸引力。这些结果表明,男性倾向于将他们对女性外貌吸引力的第一印象推广到伴侣的其他方面,而且日本男性可能认为双收入是婚姻的必要条件。
{"title":"Do Physical Attractiveness and Personality Traits Predict Romantic Partner Evaluations? A Speed‐Dating Study in Japan1,2","authors":"M. Kito, Toshihiko Souma, Takashi Nishimura, Junko Yamada, Yuji Kanemasa, Junichi Taniguchi, Taishi Kawamoto","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12489","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12489","url":null,"abstract":"The speed‐dating procedure allows researchers to closely investigate relationship‐initiation situations. Although previous speed‐dating studies identified physical attractiveness, earning prospects, and reciprocity as predictors of romantic attraction, few such studies have been conducted outside Europe or North America. The current speed‐dating study examined what factors would predict attraction ratings by potential partners among the Japanese—an understudied sample. Participants included 27 men and 28 women who first completed a questionnaire to measure individual attributes. During each speed‐dating session, 12–15 men and women talked for 3 minutes and evaluated each opposite‐gender participant on attraction and dating intentions. Results of the social relations model showed that men evaluated women who were more physically attractive and achieved higher education as more attractive. These results imply that men tend to generalize their first impressions of women's physical attractiveness to other aspects of the partner, and that Japanese men may consider dual income as a necessity for marriage.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2023-11-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139260385","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Japanese Psychological Research 日本心理学研究
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12484

Volume 65

Original Articles

Review

Brief Reports

Editorial

Erratum

Announcement

第65卷原创文章评论简报编辑勘误表公告
{"title":"Japanese Psychological Research","authors":"","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12484","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12484","url":null,"abstract":"<p><b>Volume 65</b></p><p><b>Original Articles</b></p><p>\u0000 \u0000 </p><p><b>Review</b></p><p>\u0000 \u0000 </p><p><b>Brief Reports</b></p><p>\u0000 \u0000 </p><p><b>Editorial</b></p><p>\u0000 \u0000 </p><p><b>Erratum</b></p><p>\u0000 \u0000 </p><p><b>Announcement</b></p><p>\u0000 \u0000 </p>","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jpr.12484","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50119104","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Editorial: Psychological Science of Interoception 社论:感觉心理学
IF 0.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12477
Keiko Ishii, Hideki Ohira

Interoception, which denotes the sensory connection between the body and the brain regarding the internal state and visceral organs (Sherrington, 1948), is gaining increasing attention among researchers. It serves a vital role in maintaining homeostatic operations and bodily control while also providing the foundation for mental processes and emotional responses. This underscores the close interconnection and responsiveness among brain function, mental processes, and internal physiological condition (Barrett, 2017; Craig, 2015; Damasio, 2018). Interoception is considered a multidimensional construct that encompasses objective, subjective, and metacognitive aspects. These include interoceptive accuracy, indicated by performance in behavioral tests, such as heartbeat detection; interoceptive sensibility, measured through self-reported questionnaires; and interoceptive awareness, which refers to the alignment between objective interoceptive accuracy and subjective confidence (Garfinkel & Critchley, 2013). Evidence on interoceptive neural circuits at the neuroanatomical and functional levels has been accumulating but remains limited, as highlighted in a recent special issue of Trends in Neurosciences (“The Neuroscience of Interoception”). For a summary of gaps and challenges in the field, refer to Chen et al. (2021) in the cited special issue. Additionally, the full impact of recent developments in the understanding of interoception in psychological science is not yet fully elucidated. Hence, the goal of this special issue of Japanese Psychological Research was to showcase recent empirical and theoretical advancements in psychological research on interoception. Consequently, the three articles featured in this special issue have tackled significant topics concerning the cultural foundations of self, attentional focus, and effortful control.

Ubukata et al. (2023) investigated cultural variations in interoceptive accuracy. Previous findings on these cultural differences were limited and somewhat contradictory: Although some evidence has suggested lower levels of interoceptive accuracy in East Asians and Western Africans compared to European Americans (Chentsova-Dutton & Dzokoto, 2014; Ma-Kellams et al., 2012), another line of research reported no cultural differences (Maister & Tsakiris, 2014). By implementing more refined task instructions aimed at addressing specific measurement issues that affect interoceptive accuracy, Ubukata et al. (2023) demonstrated that Japanese participants were more skilled at detecting heartbeats than their European counterparts. Grounded in self-construals and the distinct analytic versus holistic thought patterns that characterize various cultures, Ubukata et al. (2023) would provide insights into the sociocultural factors influencing

感觉,表示身体和大脑之间关于内部状态和内脏器官的感觉联系(Sherrington,1948),越来越受到研究人员的关注。它在维持稳态操作和身体控制方面发挥着至关重要的作用,同时也为心理过程和情绪反应提供了基础。这强调了大脑功能、心理过程和内部生理状况之间的密切联系和反应性(Barrett,2017;克雷格,2015;达马西奥,2018)。内感觉被认为是一种多维结构,包括客观、主观和元认知方面。其中包括通过行为测试(如心跳检测)的表现来表明的内感受准确性;通过自我报告问卷测量的内感受敏感性;以及内感受意识,指的是客观内感受准确性和主观信心之间的一致性(Garfinkel&;Critchley,2013)。神经解剖学和功能水平上关于感受间神经回路的证据一直在积累,但仍然有限,正如最近一期《神经科学趋势》(“感受间神经科学”)所强调的那样。有关该领域的差距和挑战的摘要,请参阅陈等人(2021)引用的特刊。此外,心理科学中对内感受理解的最新发展的全面影响尚未完全阐明。因此,本期《日本心理学研究》特刊的目的是展示近年来关于内感受的心理学研究的经验和理论进展。因此,本期特刊的三篇文章探讨了关于自我的文化基础、注意力集中和努力控制的重要话题。Ubukata等人(2023)研究了内感受准确性的文化差异。先前关于这些文化差异的研究结果是有限的,而且有些矛盾:尽管一些证据表明,与欧洲裔美国人相比,东亚人和西非人的内感受准确性水平较低(Chentsova Dutton和Dzokoto,2014;Ma Kellams等人,2012),但另一项研究报告没有文化差异(Maister和Tsakiris,2014)。Ubukata等人(2023)通过实施更精细的任务指令,旨在解决影响内感受准确性的特定测量问题,证明日本参与者比欧洲参与者更善于检测心跳。Ubukata等人(2023)以自我建构和不同文化特有的分析与整体思维模式为基础,将深入了解影响互觉的社会文化因素。Mizuno和Masaki(2023)研究了内感受技能的提高是否会缓解内部专注期间的表现下降。他们分析了在高尔夫推杆表演中,对身体感觉和运动的内在关注如何影响竞技游泳运动员和非运动员。研究结果表明,专注于运动,而不是专注于身体感觉,会导致成绩下降。此外,那些专注于运动并表现出较高内感受性的人往往表现得更好。有趣的是,这些倾向在竞技游泳运动员和非运动员之间没有显著差异。这项研究在探索内感受、内部焦点和运动表现之间的联系方面是独特的。利用内感受感知的多维评估(MAIA;Mehling等人,2012),Fujikawa等人(2023)探讨了内感受感知过程与行为努力的特质控制之间的联系。具体来说,MAIA的两个组成部分——注意力调节,表示维持和管理对身体感觉的注意力的能力;信任,表明个人认为自己的身体安全可靠,与努力控制呈正相关。这些发现对旨在增强对身体感觉的关注和促进自我调节活动的干预措施具有启示意义。正如这期特刊所表明的那样,理解互感在心理科学中的作用的努力可以对各个领域产生影响和影响,包括文化、运动表现和临床环境。这强调了互觉作为内部状态的表征的重要性,这些状态对维持自我至关重要。尽管本期特刊的文章数量有限,但我们预计这些文章将从心理学的角度为互觉研究提供潜在的突破性见解。
{"title":"Editorial: Psychological Science of Interoception","authors":"Keiko Ishii,&nbsp;Hideki Ohira","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12477","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12477","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Interoception, which denotes the sensory connection between the body and the brain regarding the internal state and visceral organs (Sherrington, <span>1948</span>), is gaining increasing attention among researchers. It serves a vital role in maintaining homeostatic operations and bodily control while also providing the foundation for mental processes and emotional responses. This underscores the close interconnection and responsiveness among brain function, mental processes, and internal physiological condition (Barrett, <span>2017</span>; Craig, <span>2015</span>; Damasio, <span>2018</span>). Interoception is considered a multidimensional construct that encompasses objective, subjective, and metacognitive aspects. These include interoceptive accuracy, indicated by performance in behavioral tests, such as heartbeat detection; interoceptive sensibility, measured through self-reported questionnaires; and interoceptive awareness, which refers to the alignment between objective interoceptive accuracy and subjective confidence (Garfinkel &amp; Critchley, <span>2013</span>). Evidence on interoceptive neural circuits at the neuroanatomical and functional levels has been accumulating but remains limited, as highlighted in a recent special issue of <i>Trends in Neurosciences</i> (“The Neuroscience of Interoception”). For a summary of gaps and challenges in the field, refer to Chen et al. (<span>2021</span>) in the cited special issue. Additionally, the full impact of recent developments in the understanding of interoception in psychological science is not yet fully elucidated. Hence, the goal of this special issue of <i>Japanese Psychological Research</i> was to showcase recent empirical and theoretical advancements in psychological research on interoception. Consequently, the three articles featured in this special issue have tackled significant topics concerning the cultural foundations of self, attentional focus, and effortful control.</p><p>Ubukata et al. (<span>2023</span>) investigated cultural variations in interoceptive accuracy. Previous findings on these cultural differences were limited and somewhat contradictory: Although some evidence has suggested lower levels of interoceptive accuracy in East Asians and Western Africans compared to European Americans (Chentsova-Dutton &amp; Dzokoto, <span>2014</span>; Ma-Kellams et al., <span>2012</span>), another line of research reported no cultural differences (Maister &amp; Tsakiris, <span>2014</span>). By implementing more refined task instructions aimed at addressing specific measurement issues that affect interoceptive accuracy, Ubukata et al. (<span>2023</span>) demonstrated that Japanese participants were more skilled at detecting heartbeats than their European counterparts. Grounded in self-construals and the distinct analytic versus holistic thought patterns that characterize various cultures, Ubukata et al. (<span>2023</span>) would provide insights into the sociocultural factors influencing","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jpr.12477","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50119103","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prospects for Clinical Reports on Selective Mutism Conducted in Japan: A Scoping Review1 日本选择性缄默症临床报告的展望:范围综述
4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-09-11 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12476
Daichi Iimura, Natsuki Tsujita, Hiromichi Hagihara
Abstract This scoping review examined clinical reports for selective mutism (SM) in Japan, the progress of literature, participants' demographic characteristics, and achievements of interventions. Multiple online databases were used to identify articles published before May 2021; a total of 175 articles and 218 participants were selected. The majority of articles followed a single‐case design, and there has been an increase in the number of articles published over the last few decades. The participants' ages and genders were similar across different countries. Only 20.2% of participants were diagnosed with SM; 31.7% had comorbidities, with SM symptoms being the most common complaint. The most common intervention program was a weekly hour‐long treatment session and less than a year of intervention. About half of the participants used assessment tools, with 21.6% reporting a quantitative outcome. The most commonly used intervention methods were behavioral therapy and play therapy. This review systematically maps SM research in Japan and advocates further high‐quality research.
本研究回顾了日本选择性缄默症(SM)的临床报告、文献进展、参与者的人口学特征以及干预措施的成果。多个在线数据库用于识别2021年5月之前发表的文章;共有175篇文章和218名参与者入选。大多数文章遵循单一案例设计,并且在过去几十年中发表的文章数量有所增加。参与者的年龄和性别在不同的国家是相似的。只有20.2%的参与者被诊断为SM;31.7%有合并症,其中SM症状是最常见的主诉。最常见的干预方案是每周一小时的治疗疗程和少于一年的干预。大约一半的参与者使用了评估工具,21.6%的参与者报告了定量结果。最常用的干预方法是行为治疗和游戏治疗。这篇综述系统地描绘了日本的SM研究,并提倡进一步的高质量研究。
{"title":"Prospects for Clinical Reports on Selective Mutism Conducted in Japan: A Scoping Review<sup>1</sup>","authors":"Daichi Iimura, Natsuki Tsujita, Hiromichi Hagihara","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12476","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12476","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This scoping review examined clinical reports for selective mutism (SM) in Japan, the progress of literature, participants' demographic characteristics, and achievements of interventions. Multiple online databases were used to identify articles published before May 2021; a total of 175 articles and 218 participants were selected. The majority of articles followed a single‐case design, and there has been an increase in the number of articles published over the last few decades. The participants' ages and genders were similar across different countries. Only 20.2% of participants were diagnosed with SM; 31.7% had comorbidities, with SM symptoms being the most common complaint. The most common intervention program was a weekly hour‐long treatment session and less than a year of intervention. About half of the participants used assessment tools, with 21.6% reporting a quantitative outcome. The most commonly used intervention methods were behavioral therapy and play therapy. This review systematically maps SM research in Japan and advocates further high‐quality research.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136023684","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is School Absenteeism a Spectrum? Development and Validation of Japanese Futoko Spectrum Quotient (FSQ)1 旷课是一种谱系吗?日本Futoko谱商(FSQ)的开发与验证
4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-09-08 DOI: 10.1111/jpr.12480
Yuejiang Hou, Yuki Harada, Masayoshi Ota, Hiromichi Kato
Abstract This study aimed to develop and validate a scale called the Futoko Spectrum Quotient (FSQ), which measures school absenteeism ( futoko ), as a method to achieve the early identification of at‐risk students. We recruited 673 students and 119 futoko students from secondary schools and educational support centers. The results showed that the bifactor model had better fit, proposing a general (G) factor representing avoidance motivation and an orthogonal specific (S) factor representing psychological distress. We then examined the measurement invariance and continuity of the latent bifactor construct of the FSQ, correlations of the FSQ with external variables, and measurement reliability to validate the FSQ. Our findings highlight the need to focus on students’ mental state prior to the emergence of absence behaviors and the significance of the present scale in theory and practice.
本研究旨在开发和验证一种名为Futoko谱商(FSQ)的量表,该量表用于测量学校缺勤(Futoko),作为早期识别有风险学生的方法。我们从中学和教育支持中心招募了673名学生和119名富托科学生。结果表明,双因子模型拟合较好,提出了一个代表回避动机的一般因子(G)和一个代表心理困扰的正交特定因子(S)。然后,我们检验了FSQ的潜在双因素结构的测量不变性和连续性,FSQ与外部变量的相关性,以及测量信度来验证FSQ。我们的研究结果强调了需要关注学生在缺席行为出现之前的心理状态,以及本量表在理论和实践中的意义。
{"title":"Is School Absenteeism a Spectrum? Development and Validation of Japanese Futoko Spectrum Quotient (<scp>FSQ</scp>)<sup>1</sup>","authors":"Yuejiang Hou, Yuki Harada, Masayoshi Ota, Hiromichi Kato","doi":"10.1111/jpr.12480","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12480","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This study aimed to develop and validate a scale called the Futoko Spectrum Quotient (FSQ), which measures school absenteeism ( futoko ), as a method to achieve the early identification of at‐risk students. We recruited 673 students and 119 futoko students from secondary schools and educational support centers. The results showed that the bifactor model had better fit, proposing a general (G) factor representing avoidance motivation and an orthogonal specific (S) factor representing psychological distress. We then examined the measurement invariance and continuity of the latent bifactor construct of the FSQ, correlations of the FSQ with external variables, and measurement reliability to validate the FSQ. Our findings highlight the need to focus on students’ mental state prior to the emergence of absence behaviors and the significance of the present scale in theory and practice.","PeriodicalId":46699,"journal":{"name":"Japanese Psychological Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136298783","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Japanese Psychological Research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1