首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Cross-cultural Differences in Using Nonverbal Behaviors to Identify Indirect Replies 利用非语言行为识别间接回复的跨文化差异
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00454-z
Hio Tong Pang, Xiaolin Zhou, Mingyuan Chu
{"title":"Cross-cultural Differences in Using Nonverbal Behaviors to Identify Indirect Replies","authors":"Hio Tong Pang, Xiaolin Zhou, Mingyuan Chu","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00454-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00454-z","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139861385","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
To Nod or Not to Nod: How Does Interviewer Nonverbal Behavior Affect Rapport Perceptions and Recall in Truth Tellers and Lie Tellers? 点头还是不点头:采访者的非语言行为如何影响说实话者和说谎者的关系感知和回忆?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-31 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00452-1
Haneen Deeb, Sharon Leal, Aldert Vrij, Samantha Mann, Oliwia Dabrowna

Researchers have often claimed that the interviewer’s nonverbal behavior such as nodding facilitates rapport building, the number of recalled details, and verbal veracity cues. However, there is no experiment to-date that isolated the effects of nodding in information gathering interviews. We thus examined the effects of interviewer’s nodding behavior on rapport perceptions and on the number and accuracy of total details provided by truth tellers and lie tellers. Participants (N = 150) watched a video recording and then reported it truthfully or falsely to an interviewer. The interviewer showed demeanor that was either supportive with nodding, supportive without nodding, or neutral. Truth tellers reported more total details than lie tellers and these effects were similar across demeanor conditions. No significant effects emerged for rapport perceptions and accuracy of total details. These results suggest that the interviewer’s nodding behavior does not affect rapport perceptions and details provided by truth tellers and lie tellers.

研究人员经常声称,访谈者的非语言行为(如点头)有助于建立融洽的关系、回忆细节的数量以及言语真实性线索。然而,迄今为止,还没有一项实验能将点头在信息收集访谈中的效果独立出来。因此,我们研究了访谈者点头行为对融洽感的影响,以及对说真话者和说谎话者所提供的全部细节的数量和准确性的影响。参与者(N = 150)观看了一段视频录像,然后向面试官如实或不如实地汇报。面试官表现出点头支持、不点头支持或中立的态度。说真话的人比说假话的人报告了更多的细节,这些效果在不同的态度条件下是相似的。融洽感和总细节的准确性没有明显的影响。这些结果表明,面试官的点头行为不会影响说实话者和说谎话者的融洽感和提供的细节。
{"title":"To Nod or Not to Nod: How Does Interviewer Nonverbal Behavior Affect Rapport Perceptions and Recall in Truth Tellers and Lie Tellers?","authors":"Haneen Deeb, Sharon Leal, Aldert Vrij, Samantha Mann, Oliwia Dabrowna","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00452-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00452-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Researchers have often claimed that the interviewer’s nonverbal behavior such as nodding facilitates rapport building, the number of recalled details, and verbal veracity cues. However, there is no experiment to-date that isolated the effects of nodding in information gathering interviews. We thus examined the effects of interviewer’s nodding behavior on rapport perceptions and on the number and accuracy of total details provided by truth tellers and lie tellers. Participants (<i>N</i> = 150) watched a video recording and then reported it truthfully or falsely to an interviewer. The interviewer showed demeanor that was either supportive with nodding, supportive without nodding, or neutral. Truth tellers reported more total details than lie tellers and these effects were similar across demeanor conditions. No significant effects emerged for rapport perceptions and accuracy of total details. These results suggest that the interviewer’s nodding behavior does not affect rapport perceptions and details provided by truth tellers and lie tellers.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139647724","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mining Bodily Cues to Deception 挖掘欺骗的身体线索
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-16 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-023-00450-9

Abstract

A significant body of research has investigated potential correlates of deception and bodily behavior. The vast majority of these studies consider discrete, subjectively coded bodily movements such as specific hand or head gestures. Such studies fail to consider quantitative aspects of body movement such as the precise movement direction, magnitude and timing. In this paper, we employ an innovative data mining approach to systematically study bodily correlates of deception. We re-analyze motion capture data from a previously published deception study, and experiment with different data coding options. We report how deception detection rates are affected by variables such as body part, the coding of the pose and movement, the length of the observation, and the amount of measurement noise. Our results demonstrate the feasibility of a data mining approach, with detection rates above 65%, significantly outperforming human judgement (52.80%). Owing to the systematic analysis, our analyses allow for an understanding of the importance of various coding factor. Moreover, we can reconcile seemingly discrepant findings in previous research. Our approach highlights the merits of data-driven research to support the validation and development of deception theory.

摘要 大量研究调查了欺骗和身体行为的潜在相关性。这些研究绝大多数考虑的是离散的、主观编码的肢体动作,如特定的手势或头部动作。这些研究没有考虑身体动作的定量方面,如精确的动作方向、幅度和时间。在本文中,我们采用了一种创新的数据挖掘方法来系统地研究欺骗行为的身体相关因素。我们重新分析了之前发表的一项欺骗研究中的动作捕捉数据,并试验了不同的数据编码选项。我们报告了欺骗检测率如何受到身体部位、姿势和动作编码、观察长度以及测量噪音量等变量的影响。我们的结果证明了数据挖掘方法的可行性,其检测率超过 65%,明显优于人类判断(52.80%)。通过系统分析,我们可以了解各种编码因素的重要性。此外,我们还能调和以往研究中看似不一致的发现。我们的方法凸显了数据驱动研究在支持欺骗理论的验证和发展方面的优势。
{"title":"Mining Bodily Cues to Deception","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s10919-023-00450-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00450-9","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>A significant body of research has investigated potential correlates of deception and bodily behavior. The vast majority of these studies consider discrete, subjectively coded bodily movements such as specific hand or head gestures. Such studies fail to consider quantitative aspects of body movement such as the precise movement direction, magnitude and timing. In this paper, we employ an innovative data mining approach to systematically study bodily correlates of deception. We re-analyze motion capture data from a previously published deception study, and experiment with different data coding options. We report how deception detection rates are affected by variables such as body part, the coding of the pose and movement, the length of the observation, and the amount of measurement noise. Our results demonstrate the feasibility of a data mining approach, with detection rates above 65%, significantly outperforming human judgement (52.80%). Owing to the systematic analysis, our analyses allow for an understanding of the importance of various coding factor. Moreover, we can reconcile seemingly discrepant findings in previous research. Our approach highlights the merits of data-driven research to support the validation and development of deception theory.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139500557","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Dynamic Disadvantage? Social Perceptions of Dynamic Morphed Emotions Differ from Videos and Photos 动态劣势?动态变形情绪的社会认知与视频和照片不同
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-13 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-023-00448-3
Casey Becker, Russell Conduit, Philippe A. Chouinard, Robin Laycock

Dynamic face stimuli are increasingly used in face perception research, as increasing evidence shows they are perceived differently from static photographs. One popular method for creating dynamic faces is the dynamic morph, which can animate the transition between expressions by blending two photographs together. Although morphs offer increased experimental control, their unnatural motion differs from the biological facial motion captured in video recordings. This study aimed to compare ratings of emotion intensity and genuineness in video recordings, dynamic morphs, and static photographs of happy, sad, fearful, and angry expressions. We found that video recordings were perceived to have greater emotional intensity than dynamic morphs, and video recordings of happy expressions were perceived as more genuine compared to happy dynamic morphs. Unexpectedly, static photographs and video recordings had similar ratings for genuineness and intensity. Overall, these results suggest that dynamic morphs may be an inappropriate substitute for video recordings, as they may elicit misleading dynamic effects.

在人脸感知研究中,越来越多地使用动态人脸刺激,因为越来越多的证据表明,人们对动态人脸的感知与静态照片不同。创建动态人脸的一种流行方法是动态变形,它可以通过将两张照片混合在一起,使表情之间的过渡变得生动。虽然变形提供了更多的实验控制,但其不自然的运动与视频记录中捕捉到的生物面部运动不同。本研究旨在比较视频记录、动态变形和静态照片中快乐、悲伤、恐惧和愤怒表情的情绪强度和真实度。我们发现,与动态变形相比,视频录像被认为具有更强的情绪强度;与快乐的动态变形相比,快乐表情的视频录像被认为更真实。出乎意料的是,静态照片和视频录像在真实性和强度方面的评分相似。总之,这些结果表明,动态变形可能不适合替代视频记录,因为它们可能会引起误导性的动态效果。
{"title":"A Dynamic Disadvantage? Social Perceptions of Dynamic Morphed Emotions Differ from Videos and Photos","authors":"Casey Becker, Russell Conduit, Philippe A. Chouinard, Robin Laycock","doi":"10.1007/s10919-023-00448-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00448-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Dynamic face stimuli are increasingly used in face perception research, as increasing evidence shows they are perceived differently from static photographs. One popular method for creating dynamic faces is the dynamic morph, which can animate the transition between expressions by blending two photographs together. Although morphs offer increased experimental control, their unnatural motion differs from the biological facial motion captured in video recordings. This study aimed to compare ratings of emotion intensity and genuineness in video recordings, dynamic morphs, and static photographs of happy, sad, fearful, and angry expressions. We found that video recordings were perceived to have greater emotional intensity than dynamic morphs, and video recordings of happy expressions were perceived as more genuine compared to happy dynamic morphs. Unexpectedly, static photographs and video recordings had similar ratings for genuineness and intensity. Overall, these results suggest that dynamic morphs may be an inappropriate substitute for video recordings, as they may elicit misleading dynamic effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139465063","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Impaired Emotional Mimicry Responses Towards Objectified Women 对被物化女性的情感模仿反应受损
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-09 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-023-00449-2
Daniela Ruzzante, Jeroen Vaes

Sexual objectification mostly targets women and occurs whenever they are treated as bodies for the use or consumption of others and stripped of their full humanity. While research has mostly focused on sexual harassment and aggression as the main behavioral consequence of sexual objectification, only a few studies have tried to focus on more subtle consequences towards sexually objectified targets. Spontaneous mimicry is an implicit behavior that influences our social interactions in general. It involves the imitation of other people’s postures, gestures and emotions and allows one to understand other’s emotions and intentions. Therefore, impairments in mimicry behavior are bound to have potentially damaging consequences in everyday social interactions for women who fall victim of sexual objectification. In two studies, using electromyography we measured participants’ mimicry behavior towards objectified and non-objectified women who expressed happiness and anger. Results indicated that both male and female participants attributed less mental and human traits and showed less mimicry behavior towards sexually objectified rather than non-objectified women especially when they expressed happiness. Given the fundamental role of mimicry in creating successful everyday interpersonal interactions, the results of this research advance our understanding on the more subtle, but daily consequences of sexual objectification.

性物化主要针对妇女,只要她们被当作供他人使用或消费的躯体,被剥夺了全部人性,就会发生性物化。研究主要集中在性骚扰和性侵犯上,将其作为性物化的主要行为后果,只有少数研究试图关注对性物化目标造成的更微妙的后果。自发模仿是一种影响我们社会交往的隐性行为。它包括模仿他人的姿势、手势和情绪,使人能够理解他人的情绪和意图。因此,模仿行为的障碍必然会在日常社会交往中对遭受性物化的女性产生潜在的破坏性后果。在两项研究中,我们使用肌电图测量了参与者对被物化和未被物化的女性表达快乐和愤怒时的模仿行为。结果表明,男性和女性参与者对性客体化女性而非非客体化女性的心理和人性特征的归因较少,表现出的模仿行为也较少,尤其是当她们表达快乐时。鉴于模仿在创造成功的日常人际交往中的基本作用,这项研究的结果有助于我们了解性物化更微妙但更日常的后果。
{"title":"Impaired Emotional Mimicry Responses Towards Objectified Women","authors":"Daniela Ruzzante, Jeroen Vaes","doi":"10.1007/s10919-023-00449-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00449-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Sexual objectification mostly targets women and occurs whenever they are treated as bodies for the use or consumption of others and stripped of their full humanity. While research has mostly focused on sexual harassment and aggression as the main behavioral consequence of sexual objectification, only a few studies have tried to focus on more subtle consequences towards sexually objectified targets. Spontaneous mimicry is an implicit behavior that influences our social interactions in general. It involves the imitation of other people’s postures, gestures and emotions and allows one to understand other’s emotions and intentions. Therefore, impairments in mimicry behavior are bound to have potentially damaging consequences in everyday social interactions for women who fall victim of sexual objectification. In two studies, using electromyography we measured participants’ mimicry behavior towards objectified and non-objectified women who expressed happiness and anger. Results indicated that both male and female participants attributed less mental and human traits and showed less mimicry behavior towards sexually objectified rather than non-objectified women especially when they expressed happiness. Given the fundamental role of mimicry in creating successful everyday interpersonal interactions, the results of this research advance our understanding on the more subtle, but daily consequences of sexual objectification.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139410111","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Effect of Synchrony of Happiness on Facial Expression of Negative Emotion When Lying 快乐的同步性对说谎时面部负面情绪表达的影响
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-16 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-023-00447-4

Abstract

Meta-analyses have not shown emotions to be significant predictors of deception. Criticisms of this conclusion argued that individuals must be engaged with each other in higher stake situations for such emotions to manifest, and that these emotions must be evaluated in their verbal context (Frank and Svetieva in J Appl Res Memory Cognit 1:131–133, 10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.04.006, 2012). This study examined behavioral synchrony as a marker of engagement in higher stakes truthful and deceptive interactions, and then compared the differences in facial expressions of fear, contempt, disgust, anger, and sadness not consistent with the verbal content. Forty-eight pairs of participants were randomly assigned to interviewer and interviewee, and the interviewee was assigned to steal either a watch or a ring and to lie about the item they stole, and tell the truth about the other, under conditions of higher stakes of up to $30 rewards for successful deception, and $0 plus having to write a 15-min essay for unsuccessful deception. The interviews were coded for expression of emotions using EMFACS (Friesen and Ekman in EMFACS-7; emotional facial action coding system, 1984). Synchrony was demonstrated by the pairs of participants expressing overlapping instances of happiness (AU6 + 12). A 3 (low, moderate, high synchrony) × 2 (truth, lie) mixed-design ANOVA found that negative facial expressions of emotion were a significant predictor of deception, but only when they were not consistent with the verbal content, in the moderate and high synchrony conditions. This finding is consistent with data and theorizing that shows that with higher stakes, or with higher engagement, emotions can be a predictor of deception.

摘要 Meta 分析表明,情绪并不是预测欺骗的重要因素。对这一结论的批评意见认为,个体必须在更高利害关系的情境中相互接触才能表现出这种情绪,而且这些情绪必须在言语语境中进行评估(Frank 和 Svetieva,J Appl Res Memory Cognit 1:131-133, 10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.04.006, 2012)。本研究将行为同步性作为参与风险较高的真实和欺骗性互动的标志,然后比较了与言语内容不一致的恐惧、蔑视、厌恶、愤怒和悲伤等面部表情的差异。48 对参与者被随机分配为访谈者和被访谈者,被访谈者被分配去偷手表或戒指,并对所偷物品撒谎,对另一方则说实话,成功的欺骗行为可获得高达 30 美元的奖励,而不成功的欺骗行为则需要支付 0 美元并撰写一篇 15 分钟的文章。使用 EMFACS(Friesen 和 Ekman,EMFACS-7;情绪面部动作编码系统,1984 年)对访谈的情绪表达进行编码。同步性表现在参与者表达了重叠的快乐(AU6 + 12)。通过 3(低同步、中同步、高同步)×2(真实、谎言)混合设计方差分析发现,在中同步和高同步条件下,负面面部情绪表达对欺骗有显著的预测作用,但仅限于与言语内容不一致的情况。这一发现与数据和理论研究一致,即在风险较高或参与度较高的情况下,情绪可以预测欺骗行为。
{"title":"The Effect of Synchrony of Happiness on Facial Expression of Negative Emotion When Lying","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s10919-023-00447-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00447-4","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Meta-analyses have not shown emotions to be significant predictors of deception. Criticisms of this conclusion argued that individuals must be engaged with each other in higher stake situations for such emotions to manifest, and that these emotions must be evaluated in their verbal context (Frank and Svetieva in J Appl Res Memory Cognit 1:131–133, 10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.04.006, 2012). This study examined behavioral synchrony as a marker of engagement in higher stakes truthful and deceptive interactions, and then compared the differences in facial expressions of fear, contempt, disgust, anger, and sadness not consistent with the verbal content. Forty-eight pairs of participants were randomly assigned to interviewer and interviewee, and the interviewee was assigned to steal either a watch or a ring and to lie about the item they stole, and tell the truth about the other, under conditions of higher stakes of up to $30 rewards for successful deception, and $0 plus having to write a 15-min essay for unsuccessful deception. The interviews were coded for expression of emotions using EMFACS (Friesen and Ekman in EMFACS-7; emotional facial action coding system, 1984). Synchrony was demonstrated by the pairs of participants expressing overlapping instances of happiness (AU6 + 12). A 3 (low, moderate, high synchrony) × 2 (truth, lie) mixed-design ANOVA found that negative facial expressions of emotion were a significant predictor of deception, but only when they were not consistent with the verbal content, in the moderate and high synchrony conditions. This finding is consistent with data and theorizing that shows that with higher stakes, or with higher engagement, emotions can be a predictor of deception.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138683259","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Too-Much-Mimicry Effect: Strong (vs. Subtle) Mimicry Impairs Liking and Trust in Distributive Negotiations 过度模仿效应:强烈的(相对于微妙的)模仿会损害分配谈判中的好感和信任
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-22 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-023-00446-5
Janet Wessler, David D. Loschelder, Johannes C. Fendel, Malte Friese

We examined whether mimicking an interaction partner is universally advantageous or, provided the mimicry is particularly strong, whether it has detrimental impacts on interpersonal and negotiation outcomes. Participants interacted with a confederate who engaged in no, subtle, or strong mimicry and then negotiated. In laboratory Experiment 1 (N = 71) and Experiment 2 (N = 149), subtly (vs. not) mimicked participants liked the confederate more, while strongly (vs. subtly) mimicked participants liked and trusted less. In Experiment 2, strongly (vs. subtly) mimicked participants were less susceptible to the first-offer anchor. The online Experiment 3 (N = 180) corroborated the too-much-mimicry effect: When participants became aware of mimicry, it exerted detrimental effects on liking and trust irrespective of the experimental condition. Experiment 1 and Experiment 3 found no too-much-mimicry effect on anchoring susceptibility. These findings show that (a) sufficiently subtle mimicry positively influences interpersonal outcomes and (b) too much mimicry backfires.

我们研究了模仿互动伙伴是否普遍有利,或者,如果模仿特别强,是否对人际关系和谈判结果有不利影响。参与者与一个不模仿、微妙模仿或强烈模仿的同盟者进行互动,然后进行谈判。在实验室实验1 (N = 71)和实验2 (N = 149)中,微妙模仿(与不模仿相比)的参与者更喜欢同盟者,而强烈模仿(与微妙模仿相比)的参与者更不喜欢和信任同盟者。在实验2中,强烈(相对于微妙)模仿的参与者更不容易受到第一提议锚的影响。在线实验3 (N = 180)证实了过度模仿效应:当参与者意识到模仿时,无论实验条件如何,它都会对喜欢和信任产生不利影响。实验1和实验3没有发现过度模仿对锚定敏感性的影响。这些发现表明:(a)足够微妙的模仿对人际关系的结果有积极影响,(b)太多的模仿适得其反。
{"title":"The Too-Much-Mimicry Effect: Strong (vs. Subtle) Mimicry Impairs Liking and Trust in Distributive Negotiations","authors":"Janet Wessler, David D. Loschelder, Johannes C. Fendel, Malte Friese","doi":"10.1007/s10919-023-00446-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00446-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We examined whether mimicking an interaction partner is universally advantageous or, provided the mimicry is particularly strong, whether it has detrimental impacts on interpersonal and negotiation outcomes. Participants interacted with a confederate who engaged in no, subtle, or strong mimicry and then negotiated. In laboratory Experiment 1 (<i>N</i> = 71) and Experiment 2 (<i>N</i> = 149), subtly (vs. not) mimicked participants liked the confederate more, while strongly (vs. subtly) mimicked participants liked and trusted less. In Experiment 2, strongly (vs. subtly) mimicked participants were less susceptible to the first-offer anchor. The online Experiment 3 (<i>N</i> = 180) corroborated the too-much-mimicry effect: When participants became aware of mimicry, it exerted detrimental effects on liking and trust irrespective of the experimental condition. Experiment 1 and Experiment 3 found no too-much-mimicry effect on anchoring susceptibility. These findings show that (a) sufficiently subtle mimicry positively influences interpersonal outcomes and (b) too much mimicry backfires.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138527451","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Follow the Leader: Parent- and Child-led Synchrony in Competitive and Cooperative play 跟随领导者:竞争与合作游戏中父母与孩子主导的同步
3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-023-00445-6
Jessica Yarmolovsky, Ronny Geva
Abstract Social interactions involve both cooperation to achieve a shared goal and competition over shared resources and rewards. The ability to engage in inter-personal coordination is an important measure of socio-emotional and cognitive well-being. Both cooperation and competition require interpersonal coordination, however with different motivational backgrounds. Competition is defined by a higher level of extrinsic motivation, while cooperation is related to more intrinsic motivation. In the context of the parent-child dyad, each individual has different motivations and contributions to the dyad. The parent’s and child’s sense of competitiveness and contribution to inter-personal synchrony will presumably differ from each other and adapt to one another. The current research employed Motion Energy Analysis, an objective measure of coordination of movements between individuals, to measure motor in-phase and anti-phase synchrony during parent-child cooperative and competitive play, with a focus on parent and child-led synchrony. Findings highlight that parents rate themselves as less competitive than their children rate themselves; with no such difference noted in cooperation. Further, parent-led motor synchrony is defined more by in-phase coordination in competition, especially when the interaction is novel. Alternatively, child-led motor synchrony is more anti-phase during competition. In cooperation parents and children lead synchrony to the same extent and in the same phase. Current findings highlight that parent’s and children uniquely adjust their leading behaviors in synchrony in competition, presumably adjusting their behavior to accommodate a complex situation. Given the importance of cooperative and competitive interactions to overall social well-being, and the parent’s role of modeling behaviors for their child, findings may direct future guidance and treatment plans that will promote social development.
社会互动既包括为实现共同目标而进行的合作,也包括为共享资源和奖励而进行的竞争。参与人际协调的能力是衡量社会情感和认知健康的重要指标。合作和竞争都需要人际协调,但动机背景不同。竞争是由更高层次的外在动机决定的,而合作则与更多的内在动机有关。在亲子两分制的背景下,每个人都有不同的动机和对两分制的贡献。父母和孩子的竞争意识和对人际同步的贡献可能会彼此不同,并相互适应。本研究采用运动能量分析这一衡量个体间运动协调性的客观指标来衡量亲子合作和竞争游戏中运动的同相和反相同步,重点关注父母和儿童主导的同步。研究结果强调,父母对自己的竞争力的评价不如孩子对自己的评价;在合作中没有这种差别。此外,父母主导的运动同步更多地由竞争中的同相协调来定义,特别是当互动是新颖的。或者,儿童主导的运动同步在比赛中更反相。在合作中,父母和孩子领导同步性的程度和阶段相同。目前的研究结果强调,父母和孩子在竞争中会以独特的方式同步调整他们的主导行为,这可能是为了适应复杂的情况而调整他们的行为。鉴于合作和竞争互动对整体社会福祉的重要性,以及父母为孩子树立榜样行为的作用,研究结果可能会指导未来的指导和治疗计划,从而促进社会发展。
{"title":"Follow the Leader: Parent- and Child-led Synchrony in Competitive and Cooperative play","authors":"Jessica Yarmolovsky, Ronny Geva","doi":"10.1007/s10919-023-00445-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00445-6","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Social interactions involve both cooperation to achieve a shared goal and competition over shared resources and rewards. The ability to engage in inter-personal coordination is an important measure of socio-emotional and cognitive well-being. Both cooperation and competition require interpersonal coordination, however with different motivational backgrounds. Competition is defined by a higher level of extrinsic motivation, while cooperation is related to more intrinsic motivation. In the context of the parent-child dyad, each individual has different motivations and contributions to the dyad. The parent’s and child’s sense of competitiveness and contribution to inter-personal synchrony will presumably differ from each other and adapt to one another. The current research employed Motion Energy Analysis, an objective measure of coordination of movements between individuals, to measure motor in-phase and anti-phase synchrony during parent-child cooperative and competitive play, with a focus on parent and child-led synchrony. Findings highlight that parents rate themselves as less competitive than their children rate themselves; with no such difference noted in cooperation. Further, parent-led motor synchrony is defined more by in-phase coordination in competition, especially when the interaction is novel. Alternatively, child-led motor synchrony is more anti-phase during competition. In cooperation parents and children lead synchrony to the same extent and in the same phase. Current findings highlight that parent’s and children uniquely adjust their leading behaviors in synchrony in competition, presumably adjusting their behavior to accommodate a complex situation. Given the importance of cooperative and competitive interactions to overall social well-being, and the parent’s role of modeling behaviors for their child, findings may direct future guidance and treatment plans that will promote social development.","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135774657","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Maternal Touch as a Channel of Communication at Age Four Months: Variations by Infant Gender and Maternal Depression 母亲触摸作为四个月大婴儿的沟通渠道:婴儿性别和母亲抑郁的差异
3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-24 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-023-00442-9
Shanee Stepakoff, Beatrice Beebe
{"title":"Maternal Touch as a Channel of Communication at Age Four Months: Variations by Infant Gender and Maternal Depression","authors":"Shanee Stepakoff, Beatrice Beebe","doi":"10.1007/s10919-023-00442-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00442-9","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135268345","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Nonverbal Expressivity, Physical Attractiveness, and Liking: First Impression to Established Relationship 非语言表达、身体吸引力和喜欢:建立关系的第一印象
3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-023-00444-7
Amber A. Fultz, Morgan D. Stosic, Frank J. Bernieri
{"title":"Nonverbal Expressivity, Physical Attractiveness, and Liking: First Impression to Established Relationship","authors":"Amber A. Fultz, Morgan D. Stosic, Frank J. Bernieri","doi":"10.1007/s10919-023-00444-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00444-7","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135645027","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1