首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Vocal Emotional Expressions in Mothers with and without a History of Major Depressive Disorder 有重度抑郁症史和无重度抑郁症史母亲的声乐情感表达
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-25 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00462-z
Emma Ilyaz, Xin Feng, Xiaoxue Fu, Eric E. Nelson, M. Morningstar
{"title":"Vocal Emotional Expressions in Mothers with and without a History of Major Depressive Disorder","authors":"Emma Ilyaz, Xin Feng, Xiaoxue Fu, Eric E. Nelson, M. Morningstar","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00462-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00462-z","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140658021","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An Experimental Investigation of Supportive Tactile Communication During Esteem Support Conversations 自尊心支持对话中的支持性触觉交流实验研究
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-09 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00461-0
Samantha J. Shebib, Josephine K. Boumis, Amanda Allard, Amanda J. Holmstrom, Adam J. Mason

The present study examines how supportive touch impacts evaluations of esteem support content containing high emotion-focused (HEF) or high problem-focused (HPF) messages during observed esteem support interactions. A 2 (verbal content; i.e., HEF or HPF) by 2 (nonverbal content; i.e., presence or absence of supportive tactile communication) experiment was conducted to test for main and interactional effects. Results revealed that HEF conditions were perceived to be more effective by observers at enhancing the recipient’s state self-esteem, state self-efficacy, and alleviating distress compared to HPF conditions. The supportive tactile communication conditions were perceived as better at enhancing state self-esteem and alleviating distress compared to the no supportive tactile communication conditions by observers. However, these main effects were qualified by significant two-way interactions between message content and nonverbal behavior on ratings of state self-esteem and distress alleviation, such that the addition of supportive tactile communication enhanced the effectiveness of HPF message content but not HEF content.

本研究探讨了在观察自尊支持互动过程中,支持性触觉如何影响对包含高情感关注(HEF)或高问题关注(HPF)信息的自尊支持内容的评价。我们进行了一个 2(语言内容;即 HEF 或 HPF)乘 2(非语言内容;即有无支持性触觉交流)的实验,以检验主要效应和互动效应。结果显示,与 HPF 条件相比,观察者认为 HEF 条件在增强受试者的状态自尊、状态自我效能感和减轻痛苦方面更有效。与无支持性触觉交流条件相比,观察者认为支持性触觉交流条件更能增强受试者的自尊心和减轻受试者的痛苦。然而,信息内容和非言语行为对状态自尊和痛苦缓解的评分之间存在显著的双向交互作用,从而限制了这些主效应,即添加支持性触觉交流增强了HPF信息内容的有效性,但没有增强HEF内容的有效性。
{"title":"An Experimental Investigation of Supportive Tactile Communication During Esteem Support Conversations","authors":"Samantha J. Shebib, Josephine K. Boumis, Amanda Allard, Amanda J. Holmstrom, Adam J. Mason","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00461-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00461-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The present study examines how supportive touch impacts evaluations of esteem support content containing high emotion-focused (HEF) or high problem-focused (HPF) messages during observed esteem support interactions. A 2 (verbal content; i.e., HEF or HPF) by 2 (nonverbal content; i.e., presence or absence of supportive tactile communication) experiment was conducted to test for main and interactional effects. Results revealed that HEF conditions were perceived to be more effective by observers at enhancing the recipient’s state self-esteem, state self-efficacy, and alleviating distress compared to HPF conditions. The supportive tactile communication conditions were perceived as better at enhancing state self-esteem and alleviating distress compared to the no supportive tactile communication conditions by observers. However, these main effects were qualified by significant two-way interactions between message content and nonverbal behavior on ratings of state self-esteem and distress alleviation, such that the addition of supportive tactile communication enhanced the effectiveness of HPF message content but not HEF content.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140562976","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Review of Automatic Lie Detection from Facial Features 从面部特征自动检测谎言综述
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-23 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00451-2
Hugues Delmas, Vincent Denault, Judee K. Burgoon, Norah E. Dunbar

The growth of machine learning and artificial intelligence has made it possible for automatic lie detection systems to emerge. These can be based on a variety of cues, such as facial features. However, there is a lack of knowledge about both the development and the accuracy of such systems. To address this lack, we conducted a review of studies that have investigated automatic lie detection systems by using facial features. Our analysis of twenty-eight eligible studies focused on four main categories: dataset features, facial features used, classifier features and publication features. Overall, the findings showed that automatic lie detection systems rely on diverse technologies, facial features, and measurements. They are mainly based on factual lies, regardless of the stakes involved. On average, these automatic systems were based on a dataset of 52 individuals and achieved an average accuracy ranging from 61.87% to 72.93% in distinguishing between truth-tellers and liars, depending on the types of classifiers used. However, although the leakage hypothesis was the most used explanatory framework, many studies did not provide sufficient theoretical justification for the choice of facial features and their measurements. Bridging the gap between psychology and the computational-engineering field should help to combine theoretical frameworks with technical advancements in this area.

随着机器学习和人工智能的发展,自动测谎系统应运而生。这些系统可以基于面部特征等各种线索。然而,人们对这类系统的开发和准确性都缺乏了解。为了解决这一问题,我们回顾了利用面部特征对自动测谎系统进行调查的研究。我们对 28 项符合条件的研究进行了分析,主要集中在四个方面:数据集特征、使用的面部特征、分类器特征和发布特征。总体而言,研究结果表明,自动测谎系统依赖于不同的技术、面部特征和测量方法。它们主要基于事实性谎言,而不考虑所涉及的利害关系。平均而言,这些自动系统以 52 个人的数据集为基础,在区分说真话者和说谎者方面达到了 61.87% 至 72.93% 的平均准确率,具体取决于所使用的分类器类型。然而,尽管泄漏假说是使用最多的解释框架,但许多研究并没有为面部特征的选择及其测量提供充分的理论依据。缩小心理学与计算工程领域之间的差距,有助于将理论框架与该领域的技术进步结合起来。
{"title":"A Review of Automatic Lie Detection from Facial Features","authors":"Hugues Delmas, Vincent Denault, Judee K. Burgoon, Norah E. Dunbar","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00451-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00451-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The growth of machine learning and artificial intelligence has made it possible for automatic lie detection systems to emerge. These can be based on a variety of cues, such as facial features. However, there is a lack of knowledge about both the development and the accuracy of such systems. To address this lack, we conducted a review of studies that have investigated automatic lie detection systems by using facial features. Our analysis of twenty-eight eligible studies focused on four main categories: dataset features, facial features used, classifier features and publication features. Overall, the findings showed that automatic lie detection systems rely on diverse technologies, facial features, and measurements. They are mainly based on factual lies, regardless of the stakes involved. On average, these automatic systems were based on a dataset of 52 individuals and achieved an average accuracy ranging from 61.87% to 72.93% in distinguishing between truth-tellers and liars, depending on the types of classifiers used. However, although the leakage hypothesis was the most used explanatory framework, many studies did not provide sufficient theoretical justification for the choice of facial features and their measurements. Bridging the gap between psychology and the computational-engineering field should help to combine theoretical frameworks with technical advancements in this area.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140202661","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Facial and Body Posture Emotion Identification in Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Young Adults 聋人和重听青少年的面部和身体姿势情绪识别
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00458-9
Brittany A. Blose, Lindsay S. Schenkel

The aim of the current study was to examine facial and body posture emotion recognition among deaf and hard-of-hearing (DHH) and hearing young adults. Participants were (N = 126) DHH (n = 48) and hearing (n = 78) college students who completed two emotion recognition tasks in which they were shown photographs of faces and body postures displaying different emotions of both high and low intensities and had to infer the emotion being displayed. Compared to hearing participants, DHH participants performed worse on the body postures emotion task for both high and low intensities. They also performed more poorly on the facial emotion task, but only for low-intensity emotional facial expressions. On both tasks, DHH participants whose primary mode of communication was Signed English performed significantly more poorly than those whose primary mode was American Sign Language (ASL) or spoken English. Moreover, DHH participants who communicated using ASL performed similarly to hearing participants. This suggests that difficulties in affect recognition among DHH individuals occur when processing both facial and body postures that are more subtle and reflective of real-life displays of emotion. Importantly, this also suggests that ASL as a primary form of communication in this population may serve as a protective factor against emotion recognition difficulties, which could, in part, be due to the complex nature of this language and its requirement to perceive meaning through facial and postural expressions with a wide visual lens.

本研究旨在考察聋人、重听人(DHH)和听力正常的年轻人对面部和身体姿势的情绪识别能力。参加者是(人数=126)DHH(人数=48)和听力(人数=78)大学生,他们完成了两项情绪识别任务,在这些任务中,他们看到了显示高强度和低强度不同情绪的面部和身体姿势的照片,并必须推断所显示的情绪。与健听受试者相比,在高强度和低强度的身体姿势情绪任务中,聋哑受试者的表现都较差。他们在面部情绪任务中的表现也较差,但仅限于低强度情绪面部表情。在这两项任务中,以手语英语为主要交流方式的 DHH 参与者的表现明显比以美国手语(ASL)或英语口语为主要交流方式的 DHH 参与者差。此外,使用美国手语交流的 DHH 参与者的表现与听力参与者相似。这表明,在处理面部和身体姿势时,DHH 人的情感识别会出现困难,而面部和身体姿势更微妙,更能反映现实生活中的情感表现。重要的是,这也表明 ASL 作为该人群的主要交流形式可能会成为避免情感识别困难的保护因素,部分原因可能是这种语言的复杂性及其要求通过面部和姿势表达以广阔的视觉视角来感知意义。
{"title":"Facial and Body Posture Emotion Identification in Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Young Adults","authors":"Brittany A. Blose, Lindsay S. Schenkel","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00458-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00458-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The aim of the current study was to examine facial and body posture emotion recognition among deaf and hard-of-hearing (DHH) and hearing young adults. Participants were (<i>N</i> = 126) DHH (<i>n</i> = 48) and hearing (<i>n</i> = 78) college students who completed two emotion recognition tasks in which they were shown photographs of faces and body postures displaying different emotions of both high and low intensities and had to infer the emotion being displayed. Compared to hearing participants, DHH participants performed worse on the body postures emotion task for both high and low intensities. They also performed more poorly on the facial emotion task, but only for low-intensity emotional facial expressions. On both tasks, DHH participants whose primary mode of communication was Signed English performed significantly more poorly than those whose primary mode was American Sign Language (ASL) or spoken English. Moreover, DHH participants who communicated using ASL performed similarly to hearing participants. This suggests that difficulties in affect recognition among DHH individuals occur when processing both facial and body postures that are more subtle and reflective of real-life displays of emotion. Importantly, this also suggests that ASL as a primary form of communication in this population may serve as a protective factor against emotion recognition difficulties, which could, in part, be due to the complex nature of this language and its requirement to perceive meaning through facial and postural expressions with a wide visual lens.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140146790","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Tutorial for Deception Detection Analysis or: How I Learned to Stop Aggregating Veracity Judgments and Embraced Signal Detection Theory Mixed Models 欺骗检测分析教程或:我是如何学会停止汇总真实性判断并接受信号检测理论混合模型的?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00456-x
Mircea Zloteanu, Matti Vuorre

Historically, deception detection research has relied on factorial analyses of response accuracy to make inferences. However, this practice overlooks important sources of variability resulting in potentially misleading estimates and may conflate response bias with participants’ underlying sensitivity to detect lies from truths. We showcase an alternative approach using a signal detection theory (SDT) with generalized linear mixed models framework to address these limitations. This SDT approach incorporates individual differences from both judges and senders, which are a principal source of spurious findings in deception research. By avoiding data transformations and aggregations, this methodology outperforms traditional methods and provides more informative and reliable effect estimates. This well-established framework offers researchers a powerful tool for analyzing deception data and advances our understanding of veracity judgments. All code and data are openly available.

欺骗检测研究历来依赖于对反应准确性的因子分析来进行推断。然而,这种做法忽略了重要的变异性来源,导致了潜在的误导性估计,并可能将反应偏差与参与者从真话中识别谎言的潜在敏感性混为一谈。我们展示了一种采用信号检测理论(SDT)和广义线性混合模型框架的替代方法,以解决这些局限性。这种 SDT 方法包含了评判者和发送者的个体差异,而个体差异是欺骗研究中虚假结论的主要来源。通过避免数据转换和汇总,这种方法优于传统方法,能提供更多信息和更可靠的效果估计。这个成熟的框架为研究人员提供了分析欺骗数据的强大工具,并加深了我们对真实性判断的理解。所有代码和数据均可公开获取。
{"title":"A Tutorial for Deception Detection Analysis or: How I Learned to Stop Aggregating Veracity Judgments and Embraced Signal Detection Theory Mixed Models","authors":"Mircea Zloteanu, Matti Vuorre","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00456-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00456-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Historically, deception detection research has relied on factorial analyses of response accuracy to make inferences. However, this practice overlooks important sources of variability resulting in potentially misleading estimates and may conflate response bias with participants’ underlying sensitivity to detect lies from truths. We showcase an alternative approach using a signal detection theory (SDT) with generalized linear mixed models framework to address these limitations. This SDT approach incorporates individual differences from both judges and senders, which are a principal source of spurious findings in deception research. By avoiding data transformations and aggregations, this methodology outperforms traditional methods and provides more informative and reliable effect estimates. This well-established framework offers researchers a powerful tool for analyzing deception data and advances our understanding of veracity judgments. All code and data are openly available.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140017387","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Introduction to the Special Issue on Innovations in Nonverbal Deception Research: Promising Avenues for Advancing the Field 非言语欺骗研究创新特刊导言:推动该领域发展的可行途径
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-28 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00457-w
Sally D. Farley

Ekman and Friesen’s (1969) seminal theoretical paper on the leakage hierarchy sparked decades of research on the relationship between nonverbal cues and deception. Yet skepticism over the strength and reliability of behavioral cues to deception has been building over the years (DePaulo et al., 2003; Patterson et al., 2023; Vrij et al., 2019). However, the last two decades have seen dramatic growth in research paradigms, interviewing techniques, integration of technology, automated coding methods, and facial research, suggesting a need for reexamination of the current state of the field. This special issue includes theoretical and empirical papers that advance our understanding of the link between nonverbal cues and deception. This collection of papers suggests there is cause for some optimism in the field of nonverbal deception detection and signals some fruitful avenues for future research. Specifically, deception research in ecologically valid, high-stakes lie-detection situations using a multi-modal approach has good promise for differentiating truth-tellers from liars.

Ekman 和 Friesen(1969 年)关于泄密等级的开创性理论论文引发了数十年关于非语言线索与欺骗之间关系的研究。然而,多年来,人们一直对欺骗行为线索的强度和可靠性持怀疑态度(DePaulo 等人,2003 年;Patterson 等人,2023 年;Vrij 等人,2019 年)。然而,在过去的二十年里,研究范式、访谈技术、技术整合、自动编码方法和面部研究都有了巨大的发展,这表明有必要重新审视该领域的现状。本特刊收录的理论和实证论文有助于我们进一步了解非语言线索与欺骗之间的联系。这组论文表明,我们有理由对非语言欺骗检测领域持乐观态度,并预示着未来研究的一些富有成效的途径。具体来说,在生态有效、高风险的谎言检测情境中使用多模态方法进行欺骗研究,对于区分说真话者和说谎者大有可为。
{"title":"Introduction to the Special Issue on Innovations in Nonverbal Deception Research: Promising Avenues for Advancing the Field","authors":"Sally D. Farley","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00457-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00457-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Ekman and Friesen’s (1969) seminal theoretical paper on the leakage hierarchy sparked decades of research on the relationship between nonverbal cues and deception. Yet skepticism over the strength and reliability of behavioral cues to deception has been building over the years (DePaulo et al., 2003; Patterson et al., 2023; Vrij et al., 2019). However, the last two decades have seen dramatic growth in research paradigms, interviewing techniques, integration of technology, automated coding methods, and facial research, suggesting a need for reexamination of the current state of the field. This special issue includes theoretical and empirical papers that advance our understanding of the link between nonverbal cues and deception. This collection of papers suggests there is cause for some optimism in the field of nonverbal deception detection and signals some fruitful avenues for future research. Specifically, deception research in ecologically valid, high-stakes lie-detection situations using a multi-modal approach has good promise for differentiating truth-tellers from liars.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140004017","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Touch as a Stress Buffer? Gender Differences in Subjective and Physiological Responses to Partner and Stranger Touch 触摸是压力缓冲器?对伴侣和陌生人触摸的主观和生理反应的性别差异
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-17 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00455-y
Anik Debrot, Jennifer E. Stellar, Elise Dan-Glauser, Petra L. Klumb

Interpersonal touch buffers against stress under challenging conditions, but this effect depends on familiarity. People benefit from receiving touch from their romantic partners, but the results are less consistent in the context of receiving touch from an opposite-gender stranger. We propose that there may be important gender differences in how people respond to touch from opposite-gender strangers. Specifically, we propose that touch from an opposite-gender stranger may only have stress-buffering effects for men, not women. Stress was induced as participants took part in an emotion recognition task in which they received false failure feedback while being touched by a romantic partner or stranger. We measured subjective and physiological markers of stress (i.e., reduced heart rate variability) throughout the experiment. Neither stranger’s nor partner’s touch had any effect on subjective or physiological markers of stress for men. Women, however, subjectively experienced a stress-buffering effect of partner and stranger touch, but showed increased physiological markers of stress when receiving touch from an opposite-gender stranger. These results highlight the importance of considering gender when investigating touch as a stress buffer.

在充满挑战的条件下,人际接触可以缓冲压力,但这种效果取决于熟悉程度。人们从恋爱伴侣的抚摸中获益,但在接受异性陌生人的抚摸时,结果就不那么一致了。我们认为,人们在对异性陌生人的触摸做出反应时可能存在重要的性别差异。具体来说,我们认为异性陌生人的触摸可能只对男性有缓冲压力的作用,对女性则没有。参与者在参加一项情绪识别任务时会受到压力的诱导,在这项任务中,他们在受到浪漫伴侣或陌生人的触摸时会收到错误的失败反馈。在整个实验过程中,我们测量了压力的主观和生理指标(即心率变异性降低)。对于男性来说,陌生人或伴侣的触摸都不会对主观或生理压力指标产生任何影响。然而,女性在主观上体验到了伴侣和陌生人抚摸的压力缓冲效应,但在接受异性陌生人的抚摸时却表现出了更高的压力生理指标。这些结果凸显了在研究触摸作为压力缓冲时考虑性别因素的重要性。
{"title":"Touch as a Stress Buffer? Gender Differences in Subjective and Physiological Responses to Partner and Stranger Touch","authors":"Anik Debrot, Jennifer E. Stellar, Elise Dan-Glauser, Petra L. Klumb","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00455-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00455-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Interpersonal touch buffers against stress under challenging conditions, but this effect depends on familiarity. People benefit from receiving touch from their romantic partners, but the results are less consistent in the context of receiving touch from an opposite-gender stranger. We propose that there may be important gender differences in how people respond to touch from opposite-gender strangers. Specifically, we propose that touch from an opposite-gender stranger may only have stress-buffering effects for men, not women. Stress was induced as participants took part in an emotion recognition task in which they received false failure feedback while being touched by a romantic partner or stranger. We measured subjective and physiological markers of stress (i.e., reduced heart rate variability) throughout the experiment. Neither stranger’s nor partner’s touch had any effect on subjective or physiological markers of stress for men. Women, however, subjectively experienced a stress-buffering effect of partner and stranger touch, but showed increased physiological markers of stress when receiving touch from an opposite-gender stranger. These results highlight the importance of considering gender when investigating touch as a stress buffer.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139903105","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Decoding Angry and Disgusted Faces Across Cultures: Facial Prototypes and Software Matter 跨文化解码愤怒和厌恶的表情:面部原型和软件问题
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-14 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00453-0
Xia Fang, Kerry Kawakami
{"title":"Decoding Angry and Disgusted Faces Across Cultures: Facial Prototypes and Software Matter","authors":"Xia Fang, Kerry Kawakami","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00453-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00453-0","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139776968","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Decoding Angry and Disgusted Faces Across Cultures: Facial Prototypes and Software Matter 跨文化解码愤怒和厌恶的表情:面部原型和软件问题
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-14 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00453-0
Xia Fang, Kerry Kawakami
{"title":"Decoding Angry and Disgusted Faces Across Cultures: Facial Prototypes and Software Matter","authors":"Xia Fang, Kerry Kawakami","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00453-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00453-0","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139836722","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cross-cultural Differences in Using Nonverbal Behaviors to Identify Indirect Replies 利用非语言行为识别间接回复的跨文化差异
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00454-z
Hio Tong Pang, Xiaolin Zhou, Mingyuan Chu
{"title":"Cross-cultural Differences in Using Nonverbal Behaviors to Identify Indirect Replies","authors":"Hio Tong Pang, Xiaolin Zhou, Mingyuan Chu","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00454-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00454-z","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139801683","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1