首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
A Review of Automatic Lie Detection from Facial Features 从面部特征自动检测谎言综述
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-03-23 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00451-2
Hugues Delmas, Vincent Denault, Judee K. Burgoon, Norah E. Dunbar

The growth of machine learning and artificial intelligence has made it possible for automatic lie detection systems to emerge. These can be based on a variety of cues, such as facial features. However, there is a lack of knowledge about both the development and the accuracy of such systems. To address this lack, we conducted a review of studies that have investigated automatic lie detection systems by using facial features. Our analysis of twenty-eight eligible studies focused on four main categories: dataset features, facial features used, classifier features and publication features. Overall, the findings showed that automatic lie detection systems rely on diverse technologies, facial features, and measurements. They are mainly based on factual lies, regardless of the stakes involved. On average, these automatic systems were based on a dataset of 52 individuals and achieved an average accuracy ranging from 61.87% to 72.93% in distinguishing between truth-tellers and liars, depending on the types of classifiers used. However, although the leakage hypothesis was the most used explanatory framework, many studies did not provide sufficient theoretical justification for the choice of facial features and their measurements. Bridging the gap between psychology and the computational-engineering field should help to combine theoretical frameworks with technical advancements in this area.

随着机器学习和人工智能的发展,自动测谎系统应运而生。这些系统可以基于面部特征等各种线索。然而,人们对这类系统的开发和准确性都缺乏了解。为了解决这一问题,我们回顾了利用面部特征对自动测谎系统进行调查的研究。我们对 28 项符合条件的研究进行了分析,主要集中在四个方面:数据集特征、使用的面部特征、分类器特征和发布特征。总体而言,研究结果表明,自动测谎系统依赖于不同的技术、面部特征和测量方法。它们主要基于事实性谎言,而不考虑所涉及的利害关系。平均而言,这些自动系统以 52 个人的数据集为基础,在区分说真话者和说谎者方面达到了 61.87% 至 72.93% 的平均准确率,具体取决于所使用的分类器类型。然而,尽管泄漏假说是使用最多的解释框架,但许多研究并没有为面部特征的选择及其测量提供充分的理论依据。缩小心理学与计算工程领域之间的差距,有助于将理论框架与该领域的技术进步结合起来。
{"title":"A Review of Automatic Lie Detection from Facial Features","authors":"Hugues Delmas, Vincent Denault, Judee K. Burgoon, Norah E. Dunbar","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00451-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00451-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The growth of machine learning and artificial intelligence has made it possible for automatic lie detection systems to emerge. These can be based on a variety of cues, such as facial features. However, there is a lack of knowledge about both the development and the accuracy of such systems. To address this lack, we conducted a review of studies that have investigated automatic lie detection systems by using facial features. Our analysis of twenty-eight eligible studies focused on four main categories: dataset features, facial features used, classifier features and publication features. Overall, the findings showed that automatic lie detection systems rely on diverse technologies, facial features, and measurements. They are mainly based on factual lies, regardless of the stakes involved. On average, these automatic systems were based on a dataset of 52 individuals and achieved an average accuracy ranging from 61.87% to 72.93% in distinguishing between truth-tellers and liars, depending on the types of classifiers used. However, although the leakage hypothesis was the most used explanatory framework, many studies did not provide sufficient theoretical justification for the choice of facial features and their measurements. Bridging the gap between psychology and the computational-engineering field should help to combine theoretical frameworks with technical advancements in this area.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"196 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140202661","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Facial and Body Posture Emotion Identification in Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Young Adults 聋人和重听青少年的面部和身体姿势情绪识别
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-03-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00458-9
Brittany A. Blose, Lindsay S. Schenkel

The aim of the current study was to examine facial and body posture emotion recognition among deaf and hard-of-hearing (DHH) and hearing young adults. Participants were (N = 126) DHH (n = 48) and hearing (n = 78) college students who completed two emotion recognition tasks in which they were shown photographs of faces and body postures displaying different emotions of both high and low intensities and had to infer the emotion being displayed. Compared to hearing participants, DHH participants performed worse on the body postures emotion task for both high and low intensities. They also performed more poorly on the facial emotion task, but only for low-intensity emotional facial expressions. On both tasks, DHH participants whose primary mode of communication was Signed English performed significantly more poorly than those whose primary mode was American Sign Language (ASL) or spoken English. Moreover, DHH participants who communicated using ASL performed similarly to hearing participants. This suggests that difficulties in affect recognition among DHH individuals occur when processing both facial and body postures that are more subtle and reflective of real-life displays of emotion. Importantly, this also suggests that ASL as a primary form of communication in this population may serve as a protective factor against emotion recognition difficulties, which could, in part, be due to the complex nature of this language and its requirement to perceive meaning through facial and postural expressions with a wide visual lens.

本研究旨在考察聋人、重听人(DHH)和听力正常的年轻人对面部和身体姿势的情绪识别能力。参加者是(人数=126)DHH(人数=48)和听力(人数=78)大学生,他们完成了两项情绪识别任务,在这些任务中,他们看到了显示高强度和低强度不同情绪的面部和身体姿势的照片,并必须推断所显示的情绪。与健听受试者相比,在高强度和低强度的身体姿势情绪任务中,聋哑受试者的表现都较差。他们在面部情绪任务中的表现也较差,但仅限于低强度情绪面部表情。在这两项任务中,以手语英语为主要交流方式的 DHH 参与者的表现明显比以美国手语(ASL)或英语口语为主要交流方式的 DHH 参与者差。此外,使用美国手语交流的 DHH 参与者的表现与听力参与者相似。这表明,在处理面部和身体姿势时,DHH 人的情感识别会出现困难,而面部和身体姿势更微妙,更能反映现实生活中的情感表现。重要的是,这也表明 ASL 作为该人群的主要交流形式可能会成为避免情感识别困难的保护因素,部分原因可能是这种语言的复杂性及其要求通过面部和姿势表达以广阔的视觉视角来感知意义。
{"title":"Facial and Body Posture Emotion Identification in Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Young Adults","authors":"Brittany A. Blose, Lindsay S. Schenkel","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00458-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00458-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The aim of the current study was to examine facial and body posture emotion recognition among deaf and hard-of-hearing (DHH) and hearing young adults. Participants were (<i>N</i> = 126) DHH (<i>n</i> = 48) and hearing (<i>n</i> = 78) college students who completed two emotion recognition tasks in which they were shown photographs of faces and body postures displaying different emotions of both high and low intensities and had to infer the emotion being displayed. Compared to hearing participants, DHH participants performed worse on the body postures emotion task for both high and low intensities. They also performed more poorly on the facial emotion task, but only for low-intensity emotional facial expressions. On both tasks, DHH participants whose primary mode of communication was Signed English performed significantly more poorly than those whose primary mode was American Sign Language (ASL) or spoken English. Moreover, DHH participants who communicated using ASL performed similarly to hearing participants. This suggests that difficulties in affect recognition among DHH individuals occur when processing both facial and body postures that are more subtle and reflective of real-life displays of emotion. Importantly, this also suggests that ASL as a primary form of communication in this population may serve as a protective factor against emotion recognition difficulties, which could, in part, be due to the complex nature of this language and its requirement to perceive meaning through facial and postural expressions with a wide visual lens.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"149 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140146790","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Tutorial for Deception Detection Analysis or: How I Learned to Stop Aggregating Veracity Judgments and Embraced Signal Detection Theory Mixed Models 欺骗检测分析教程或:我是如何学会停止汇总真实性判断并接受信号检测理论混合模型的?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00456-x
Mircea Zloteanu, Matti Vuorre

Historically, deception detection research has relied on factorial analyses of response accuracy to make inferences. However, this practice overlooks important sources of variability resulting in potentially misleading estimates and may conflate response bias with participants’ underlying sensitivity to detect lies from truths. We showcase an alternative approach using a signal detection theory (SDT) with generalized linear mixed models framework to address these limitations. This SDT approach incorporates individual differences from both judges and senders, which are a principal source of spurious findings in deception research. By avoiding data transformations and aggregations, this methodology outperforms traditional methods and provides more informative and reliable effect estimates. This well-established framework offers researchers a powerful tool for analyzing deception data and advances our understanding of veracity judgments. All code and data are openly available.

欺骗检测研究历来依赖于对反应准确性的因子分析来进行推断。然而,这种做法忽略了重要的变异性来源,导致了潜在的误导性估计,并可能将反应偏差与参与者从真话中识别谎言的潜在敏感性混为一谈。我们展示了一种采用信号检测理论(SDT)和广义线性混合模型框架的替代方法,以解决这些局限性。这种 SDT 方法包含了评判者和发送者的个体差异,而个体差异是欺骗研究中虚假结论的主要来源。通过避免数据转换和汇总,这种方法优于传统方法,能提供更多信息和更可靠的效果估计。这个成熟的框架为研究人员提供了分析欺骗数据的强大工具,并加深了我们对真实性判断的理解。所有代码和数据均可公开获取。
{"title":"A Tutorial for Deception Detection Analysis or: How I Learned to Stop Aggregating Veracity Judgments and Embraced Signal Detection Theory Mixed Models","authors":"Mircea Zloteanu, Matti Vuorre","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00456-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00456-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Historically, deception detection research has relied on factorial analyses of response accuracy to make inferences. However, this practice overlooks important sources of variability resulting in potentially misleading estimates and may conflate response bias with participants’ underlying sensitivity to detect lies from truths. We showcase an alternative approach using a signal detection theory (SDT) with generalized linear mixed models framework to address these limitations. This SDT approach incorporates individual differences from both judges and senders, which are a principal source of spurious findings in deception research. By avoiding data transformations and aggregations, this methodology outperforms traditional methods and provides more informative and reliable effect estimates. This well-established framework offers researchers a powerful tool for analyzing deception data and advances our understanding of veracity judgments. All code and data are openly available.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"53 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140017387","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Introduction to the Special Issue on Innovations in Nonverbal Deception Research: Promising Avenues for Advancing the Field 非言语欺骗研究创新特刊导言:推动该领域发展的可行途径
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-02-28 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00457-w
Sally D. Farley

Ekman and Friesen’s (1969) seminal theoretical paper on the leakage hierarchy sparked decades of research on the relationship between nonverbal cues and deception. Yet skepticism over the strength and reliability of behavioral cues to deception has been building over the years (DePaulo et al., 2003; Patterson et al., 2023; Vrij et al., 2019). However, the last two decades have seen dramatic growth in research paradigms, interviewing techniques, integration of technology, automated coding methods, and facial research, suggesting a need for reexamination of the current state of the field. This special issue includes theoretical and empirical papers that advance our understanding of the link between nonverbal cues and deception. This collection of papers suggests there is cause for some optimism in the field of nonverbal deception detection and signals some fruitful avenues for future research. Specifically, deception research in ecologically valid, high-stakes lie-detection situations using a multi-modal approach has good promise for differentiating truth-tellers from liars.

Ekman 和 Friesen(1969 年)关于泄密等级的开创性理论论文引发了数十年关于非语言线索与欺骗之间关系的研究。然而,多年来,人们一直对欺骗行为线索的强度和可靠性持怀疑态度(DePaulo 等人,2003 年;Patterson 等人,2023 年;Vrij 等人,2019 年)。然而,在过去的二十年里,研究范式、访谈技术、技术整合、自动编码方法和面部研究都有了巨大的发展,这表明有必要重新审视该领域的现状。本特刊收录的理论和实证论文有助于我们进一步了解非语言线索与欺骗之间的联系。这组论文表明,我们有理由对非语言欺骗检测领域持乐观态度,并预示着未来研究的一些富有成效的途径。具体来说,在生态有效、高风险的谎言检测情境中使用多模态方法进行欺骗研究,对于区分说真话者和说谎者大有可为。
{"title":"Introduction to the Special Issue on Innovations in Nonverbal Deception Research: Promising Avenues for Advancing the Field","authors":"Sally D. Farley","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00457-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00457-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Ekman and Friesen’s (1969) seminal theoretical paper on the leakage hierarchy sparked decades of research on the relationship between nonverbal cues and deception. Yet skepticism over the strength and reliability of behavioral cues to deception has been building over the years (DePaulo et al., 2003; Patterson et al., 2023; Vrij et al., 2019). However, the last two decades have seen dramatic growth in research paradigms, interviewing techniques, integration of technology, automated coding methods, and facial research, suggesting a need for reexamination of the current state of the field. This special issue includes theoretical and empirical papers that advance our understanding of the link between nonverbal cues and deception. This collection of papers suggests there is cause for some optimism in the field of nonverbal deception detection and signals some fruitful avenues for future research. Specifically, deception research in ecologically valid, high-stakes lie-detection situations using a multi-modal approach has good promise for differentiating truth-tellers from liars.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"23 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140004017","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Touch as a Stress Buffer? Gender Differences in Subjective and Physiological Responses to Partner and Stranger Touch 触摸是压力缓冲器?对伴侣和陌生人触摸的主观和生理反应的性别差异
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-02-17 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00455-y
Anik Debrot, Jennifer E. Stellar, Elise Dan-Glauser, Petra L. Klumb

Interpersonal touch buffers against stress under challenging conditions, but this effect depends on familiarity. People benefit from receiving touch from their romantic partners, but the results are less consistent in the context of receiving touch from an opposite-gender stranger. We propose that there may be important gender differences in how people respond to touch from opposite-gender strangers. Specifically, we propose that touch from an opposite-gender stranger may only have stress-buffering effects for men, not women. Stress was induced as participants took part in an emotion recognition task in which they received false failure feedback while being touched by a romantic partner or stranger. We measured subjective and physiological markers of stress (i.e., reduced heart rate variability) throughout the experiment. Neither stranger’s nor partner’s touch had any effect on subjective or physiological markers of stress for men. Women, however, subjectively experienced a stress-buffering effect of partner and stranger touch, but showed increased physiological markers of stress when receiving touch from an opposite-gender stranger. These results highlight the importance of considering gender when investigating touch as a stress buffer.

在充满挑战的条件下,人际接触可以缓冲压力,但这种效果取决于熟悉程度。人们从恋爱伴侣的抚摸中获益,但在接受异性陌生人的抚摸时,结果就不那么一致了。我们认为,人们在对异性陌生人的触摸做出反应时可能存在重要的性别差异。具体来说,我们认为异性陌生人的触摸可能只对男性有缓冲压力的作用,对女性则没有。参与者在参加一项情绪识别任务时会受到压力的诱导,在这项任务中,他们在受到浪漫伴侣或陌生人的触摸时会收到错误的失败反馈。在整个实验过程中,我们测量了压力的主观和生理指标(即心率变异性降低)。对于男性来说,陌生人或伴侣的触摸都不会对主观或生理压力指标产生任何影响。然而,女性在主观上体验到了伴侣和陌生人抚摸的压力缓冲效应,但在接受异性陌生人的抚摸时却表现出了更高的压力生理指标。这些结果凸显了在研究触摸作为压力缓冲时考虑性别因素的重要性。
{"title":"Touch as a Stress Buffer? Gender Differences in Subjective and Physiological Responses to Partner and Stranger Touch","authors":"Anik Debrot, Jennifer E. Stellar, Elise Dan-Glauser, Petra L. Klumb","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00455-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00455-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Interpersonal touch buffers against stress under challenging conditions, but this effect depends on familiarity. People benefit from receiving touch from their romantic partners, but the results are less consistent in the context of receiving touch from an opposite-gender stranger. We propose that there may be important gender differences in how people respond to touch from opposite-gender strangers. Specifically, we propose that touch from an opposite-gender stranger may only have stress-buffering effects for men, not women. Stress was induced as participants took part in an emotion recognition task in which they received false failure feedback while being touched by a romantic partner or stranger. We measured subjective and physiological markers of stress (i.e., reduced heart rate variability) throughout the experiment. Neither stranger’s nor partner’s touch had any effect on subjective or physiological markers of stress for men. Women, however, subjectively experienced a stress-buffering effect of partner and stranger touch, but showed increased physiological markers of stress when receiving touch from an opposite-gender stranger. These results highlight the importance of considering gender when investigating touch as a stress buffer.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"7 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139903105","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
To Nod or Not to Nod: How Does Interviewer Nonverbal Behavior Affect Rapport Perceptions and Recall in Truth Tellers and Lie Tellers? 点头还是不点头:采访者的非语言行为如何影响说实话者和说谎者的关系感知和回忆?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-31 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-024-00452-1
Haneen Deeb, Sharon Leal, Aldert Vrij, Samantha Mann, Oliwia Dabrowna

Researchers have often claimed that the interviewer’s nonverbal behavior such as nodding facilitates rapport building, the number of recalled details, and verbal veracity cues. However, there is no experiment to-date that isolated the effects of nodding in information gathering interviews. We thus examined the effects of interviewer’s nodding behavior on rapport perceptions and on the number and accuracy of total details provided by truth tellers and lie tellers. Participants (N = 150) watched a video recording and then reported it truthfully or falsely to an interviewer. The interviewer showed demeanor that was either supportive with nodding, supportive without nodding, or neutral. Truth tellers reported more total details than lie tellers and these effects were similar across demeanor conditions. No significant effects emerged for rapport perceptions and accuracy of total details. These results suggest that the interviewer’s nodding behavior does not affect rapport perceptions and details provided by truth tellers and lie tellers.

研究人员经常声称,访谈者的非语言行为(如点头)有助于建立融洽的关系、回忆细节的数量以及言语真实性线索。然而,迄今为止,还没有一项实验能将点头在信息收集访谈中的效果独立出来。因此,我们研究了访谈者点头行为对融洽感的影响,以及对说真话者和说谎话者所提供的全部细节的数量和准确性的影响。参与者(N = 150)观看了一段视频录像,然后向面试官如实或不如实地汇报。面试官表现出点头支持、不点头支持或中立的态度。说真话的人比说假话的人报告了更多的细节,这些效果在不同的态度条件下是相似的。融洽感和总细节的准确性没有明显的影响。这些结果表明,面试官的点头行为不会影响说实话者和说谎话者的融洽感和提供的细节。
{"title":"To Nod or Not to Nod: How Does Interviewer Nonverbal Behavior Affect Rapport Perceptions and Recall in Truth Tellers and Lie Tellers?","authors":"Haneen Deeb, Sharon Leal, Aldert Vrij, Samantha Mann, Oliwia Dabrowna","doi":"10.1007/s10919-024-00452-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-024-00452-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Researchers have often claimed that the interviewer’s nonverbal behavior such as nodding facilitates rapport building, the number of recalled details, and verbal veracity cues. However, there is no experiment to-date that isolated the effects of nodding in information gathering interviews. We thus examined the effects of interviewer’s nodding behavior on rapport perceptions and on the number and accuracy of total details provided by truth tellers and lie tellers. Participants (<i>N</i> = 150) watched a video recording and then reported it truthfully or falsely to an interviewer. The interviewer showed demeanor that was either supportive with nodding, supportive without nodding, or neutral. Truth tellers reported more total details than lie tellers and these effects were similar across demeanor conditions. No significant effects emerged for rapport perceptions and accuracy of total details. These results suggest that the interviewer’s nodding behavior does not affect rapport perceptions and details provided by truth tellers and lie tellers.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"152 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139647724","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mining Bodily Cues to Deception 挖掘欺骗的身体线索
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-16 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-023-00450-9

Abstract

A significant body of research has investigated potential correlates of deception and bodily behavior. The vast majority of these studies consider discrete, subjectively coded bodily movements such as specific hand or head gestures. Such studies fail to consider quantitative aspects of body movement such as the precise movement direction, magnitude and timing. In this paper, we employ an innovative data mining approach to systematically study bodily correlates of deception. We re-analyze motion capture data from a previously published deception study, and experiment with different data coding options. We report how deception detection rates are affected by variables such as body part, the coding of the pose and movement, the length of the observation, and the amount of measurement noise. Our results demonstrate the feasibility of a data mining approach, with detection rates above 65%, significantly outperforming human judgement (52.80%). Owing to the systematic analysis, our analyses allow for an understanding of the importance of various coding factor. Moreover, we can reconcile seemingly discrepant findings in previous research. Our approach highlights the merits of data-driven research to support the validation and development of deception theory.

摘要 大量研究调查了欺骗和身体行为的潜在相关性。这些研究绝大多数考虑的是离散的、主观编码的肢体动作,如特定的手势或头部动作。这些研究没有考虑身体动作的定量方面,如精确的动作方向、幅度和时间。在本文中,我们采用了一种创新的数据挖掘方法来系统地研究欺骗行为的身体相关因素。我们重新分析了之前发表的一项欺骗研究中的动作捕捉数据,并试验了不同的数据编码选项。我们报告了欺骗检测率如何受到身体部位、姿势和动作编码、观察长度以及测量噪音量等变量的影响。我们的结果证明了数据挖掘方法的可行性,其检测率超过 65%,明显优于人类判断(52.80%)。通过系统分析,我们可以了解各种编码因素的重要性。此外,我们还能调和以往研究中看似不一致的发现。我们的方法凸显了数据驱动研究在支持欺骗理论的验证和发展方面的优势。
{"title":"Mining Bodily Cues to Deception","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s10919-023-00450-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00450-9","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>A significant body of research has investigated potential correlates of deception and bodily behavior. The vast majority of these studies consider discrete, subjectively coded bodily movements such as specific hand or head gestures. Such studies fail to consider quantitative aspects of body movement such as the precise movement direction, magnitude and timing. In this paper, we employ an innovative data mining approach to systematically study bodily correlates of deception. We re-analyze motion capture data from a previously published deception study, and experiment with different data coding options. We report how deception detection rates are affected by variables such as body part, the coding of the pose and movement, the length of the observation, and the amount of measurement noise. Our results demonstrate the feasibility of a data mining approach, with detection rates above 65%, significantly outperforming human judgement (52.80%). Owing to the systematic analysis, our analyses allow for an understanding of the importance of various coding factor. Moreover, we can reconcile seemingly discrepant findings in previous research. Our approach highlights the merits of data-driven research to support the validation and development of deception theory.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"99 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139500557","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Dynamic Disadvantage? Social Perceptions of Dynamic Morphed Emotions Differ from Videos and Photos 动态劣势?动态变形情绪的社会认知与视频和照片不同
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-13 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-023-00448-3
Casey Becker, Russell Conduit, Philippe A. Chouinard, Robin Laycock

Dynamic face stimuli are increasingly used in face perception research, as increasing evidence shows they are perceived differently from static photographs. One popular method for creating dynamic faces is the dynamic morph, which can animate the transition between expressions by blending two photographs together. Although morphs offer increased experimental control, their unnatural motion differs from the biological facial motion captured in video recordings. This study aimed to compare ratings of emotion intensity and genuineness in video recordings, dynamic morphs, and static photographs of happy, sad, fearful, and angry expressions. We found that video recordings were perceived to have greater emotional intensity than dynamic morphs, and video recordings of happy expressions were perceived as more genuine compared to happy dynamic morphs. Unexpectedly, static photographs and video recordings had similar ratings for genuineness and intensity. Overall, these results suggest that dynamic morphs may be an inappropriate substitute for video recordings, as they may elicit misleading dynamic effects.

在人脸感知研究中,越来越多地使用动态人脸刺激,因为越来越多的证据表明,人们对动态人脸的感知与静态照片不同。创建动态人脸的一种流行方法是动态变形,它可以通过将两张照片混合在一起,使表情之间的过渡变得生动。虽然变形提供了更多的实验控制,但其不自然的运动与视频记录中捕捉到的生物面部运动不同。本研究旨在比较视频记录、动态变形和静态照片中快乐、悲伤、恐惧和愤怒表情的情绪强度和真实度。我们发现,与动态变形相比,视频录像被认为具有更强的情绪强度;与快乐的动态变形相比,快乐表情的视频录像被认为更真实。出乎意料的是,静态照片和视频录像在真实性和强度方面的评分相似。总之,这些结果表明,动态变形可能不适合替代视频记录,因为它们可能会引起误导性的动态效果。
{"title":"A Dynamic Disadvantage? Social Perceptions of Dynamic Morphed Emotions Differ from Videos and Photos","authors":"Casey Becker, Russell Conduit, Philippe A. Chouinard, Robin Laycock","doi":"10.1007/s10919-023-00448-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00448-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Dynamic face stimuli are increasingly used in face perception research, as increasing evidence shows they are perceived differently from static photographs. One popular method for creating dynamic faces is the dynamic morph, which can animate the transition between expressions by blending two photographs together. Although morphs offer increased experimental control, their unnatural motion differs from the biological facial motion captured in video recordings. This study aimed to compare ratings of emotion intensity and genuineness in video recordings, dynamic morphs, and static photographs of happy, sad, fearful, and angry expressions. We found that video recordings were perceived to have greater emotional intensity than dynamic morphs, and video recordings of happy expressions were perceived as more genuine compared to happy dynamic morphs. Unexpectedly, static photographs and video recordings had similar ratings for genuineness and intensity. Overall, these results suggest that dynamic morphs may be an inappropriate substitute for video recordings, as they may elicit misleading dynamic effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"61 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139465063","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Impaired Emotional Mimicry Responses Towards Objectified Women 对被物化女性的情感模仿反应受损
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-09 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-023-00449-2
Daniela Ruzzante, Jeroen Vaes

Sexual objectification mostly targets women and occurs whenever they are treated as bodies for the use or consumption of others and stripped of their full humanity. While research has mostly focused on sexual harassment and aggression as the main behavioral consequence of sexual objectification, only a few studies have tried to focus on more subtle consequences towards sexually objectified targets. Spontaneous mimicry is an implicit behavior that influences our social interactions in general. It involves the imitation of other people’s postures, gestures and emotions and allows one to understand other’s emotions and intentions. Therefore, impairments in mimicry behavior are bound to have potentially damaging consequences in everyday social interactions for women who fall victim of sexual objectification. In two studies, using electromyography we measured participants’ mimicry behavior towards objectified and non-objectified women who expressed happiness and anger. Results indicated that both male and female participants attributed less mental and human traits and showed less mimicry behavior towards sexually objectified rather than non-objectified women especially when they expressed happiness. Given the fundamental role of mimicry in creating successful everyday interpersonal interactions, the results of this research advance our understanding on the more subtle, but daily consequences of sexual objectification.

性物化主要针对妇女,只要她们被当作供他人使用或消费的躯体,被剥夺了全部人性,就会发生性物化。研究主要集中在性骚扰和性侵犯上,将其作为性物化的主要行为后果,只有少数研究试图关注对性物化目标造成的更微妙的后果。自发模仿是一种影响我们社会交往的隐性行为。它包括模仿他人的姿势、手势和情绪,使人能够理解他人的情绪和意图。因此,模仿行为的障碍必然会在日常社会交往中对遭受性物化的女性产生潜在的破坏性后果。在两项研究中,我们使用肌电图测量了参与者对被物化和未被物化的女性表达快乐和愤怒时的模仿行为。结果表明,男性和女性参与者对性客体化女性而非非客体化女性的心理和人性特征的归因较少,表现出的模仿行为也较少,尤其是当她们表达快乐时。鉴于模仿在创造成功的日常人际交往中的基本作用,这项研究的结果有助于我们了解性物化更微妙但更日常的后果。
{"title":"Impaired Emotional Mimicry Responses Towards Objectified Women","authors":"Daniela Ruzzante, Jeroen Vaes","doi":"10.1007/s10919-023-00449-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00449-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Sexual objectification mostly targets women and occurs whenever they are treated as bodies for the use or consumption of others and stripped of their full humanity. While research has mostly focused on sexual harassment and aggression as the main behavioral consequence of sexual objectification, only a few studies have tried to focus on more subtle consequences towards sexually objectified targets. Spontaneous mimicry is an implicit behavior that influences our social interactions in general. It involves the imitation of other people’s postures, gestures and emotions and allows one to understand other’s emotions and intentions. Therefore, impairments in mimicry behavior are bound to have potentially damaging consequences in everyday social interactions for women who fall victim of sexual objectification. In two studies, using electromyography we measured participants’ mimicry behavior towards objectified and non-objectified women who expressed happiness and anger. Results indicated that both male and female participants attributed less mental and human traits and showed less mimicry behavior towards sexually objectified rather than non-objectified women especially when they expressed happiness. Given the fundamental role of mimicry in creating successful everyday interpersonal interactions, the results of this research advance our understanding on the more subtle, but daily consequences of sexual objectification.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"50 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139410111","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Effect of Synchrony of Happiness on Facial Expression of Negative Emotion When Lying 快乐的同步性对说谎时面部负面情绪表达的影响
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-12-16 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-023-00447-4

Abstract

Meta-analyses have not shown emotions to be significant predictors of deception. Criticisms of this conclusion argued that individuals must be engaged with each other in higher stake situations for such emotions to manifest, and that these emotions must be evaluated in their verbal context (Frank and Svetieva in J Appl Res Memory Cognit 1:131–133, 10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.04.006, 2012). This study examined behavioral synchrony as a marker of engagement in higher stakes truthful and deceptive interactions, and then compared the differences in facial expressions of fear, contempt, disgust, anger, and sadness not consistent with the verbal content. Forty-eight pairs of participants were randomly assigned to interviewer and interviewee, and the interviewee was assigned to steal either a watch or a ring and to lie about the item they stole, and tell the truth about the other, under conditions of higher stakes of up to $30 rewards for successful deception, and $0 plus having to write a 15-min essay for unsuccessful deception. The interviews were coded for expression of emotions using EMFACS (Friesen and Ekman in EMFACS-7; emotional facial action coding system, 1984). Synchrony was demonstrated by the pairs of participants expressing overlapping instances of happiness (AU6 + 12). A 3 (low, moderate, high synchrony) × 2 (truth, lie) mixed-design ANOVA found that negative facial expressions of emotion were a significant predictor of deception, but only when they were not consistent with the verbal content, in the moderate and high synchrony conditions. This finding is consistent with data and theorizing that shows that with higher stakes, or with higher engagement, emotions can be a predictor of deception.

摘要 Meta 分析表明,情绪并不是预测欺骗的重要因素。对这一结论的批评意见认为,个体必须在更高利害关系的情境中相互接触才能表现出这种情绪,而且这些情绪必须在言语语境中进行评估(Frank 和 Svetieva,J Appl Res Memory Cognit 1:131-133, 10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.04.006, 2012)。本研究将行为同步性作为参与风险较高的真实和欺骗性互动的标志,然后比较了与言语内容不一致的恐惧、蔑视、厌恶、愤怒和悲伤等面部表情的差异。48 对参与者被随机分配为访谈者和被访谈者,被访谈者被分配去偷手表或戒指,并对所偷物品撒谎,对另一方则说实话,成功的欺骗行为可获得高达 30 美元的奖励,而不成功的欺骗行为则需要支付 0 美元并撰写一篇 15 分钟的文章。使用 EMFACS(Friesen 和 Ekman,EMFACS-7;情绪面部动作编码系统,1984 年)对访谈的情绪表达进行编码。同步性表现在参与者表达了重叠的快乐(AU6 + 12)。通过 3(低同步、中同步、高同步)×2(真实、谎言)混合设计方差分析发现,在中同步和高同步条件下,负面面部情绪表达对欺骗有显著的预测作用,但仅限于与言语内容不一致的情况。这一发现与数据和理论研究一致,即在风险较高或参与度较高的情况下,情绪可以预测欺骗行为。
{"title":"The Effect of Synchrony of Happiness on Facial Expression of Negative Emotion When Lying","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s10919-023-00447-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-023-00447-4","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Meta-analyses have not shown emotions to be significant predictors of deception. Criticisms of this conclusion argued that individuals must be engaged with each other in higher stake situations for such emotions to manifest, and that these emotions must be evaluated in their verbal context (Frank and Svetieva in J Appl Res Memory Cognit 1:131–133, 10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.04.006, 2012). This study examined behavioral synchrony as a marker of engagement in higher stakes truthful and deceptive interactions, and then compared the differences in facial expressions of fear, contempt, disgust, anger, and sadness not consistent with the verbal content. Forty-eight pairs of participants were randomly assigned to interviewer and interviewee, and the interviewee was assigned to steal either a watch or a ring and to lie about the item they stole, and tell the truth about the other, under conditions of higher stakes of up to $30 rewards for successful deception, and $0 plus having to write a 15-min essay for unsuccessful deception. The interviews were coded for expression of emotions using EMFACS (Friesen and Ekman in EMFACS-7; emotional facial action coding system, 1984). Synchrony was demonstrated by the pairs of participants expressing overlapping instances of happiness (AU6 + 12). A 3 (low, moderate, high synchrony) × 2 (truth, lie) mixed-design ANOVA found that negative facial expressions of emotion were a significant predictor of deception, but only when they were not consistent with the verbal content, in the moderate and high synchrony conditions. This finding is consistent with data and theorizing that shows that with higher stakes, or with higher engagement, emotions can be a predictor of deception.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"239 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138683259","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1