首页 > 最新文献

Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective最新文献

英文 中文
Correction to: Harm Avoidance and Mobility During Middle Childhood and Adolescence among Hadza Foragers. 修正:哈扎族觅食者在童年中期和青春期的伤害避免和流动性。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-03-01 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09403-x
Alyssa N Crittenden, Alan Farahani, Kristen N Herlosky, Trevor R Pollom, Ibrahim A Mabulla, Ian T Ruginski, Elizabeth Cashdan
{"title":"Correction to: Harm Avoidance and Mobility During Middle Childhood and Adolescence among Hadza Foragers.","authors":"Alyssa N Crittenden, Alan Farahani, Kristen N Herlosky, Trevor R Pollom, Ibrahim A Mabulla, Ian T Ruginski, Elizabeth Cashdan","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09403-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09403-x","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 1","pages":"177"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-021-09403-x","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38925228","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social Learning and Innovation in Adolescence : A Comparative Study of Aka and Chabu Hunter-Gatherers of Central and Eastern Africa. 青少年的社会学习与创新:中非和东非阿卡和查布狩猎采集者的比较研究。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-03-01 Epub Date: 2021-04-21 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09391-y
Bonnie Hewlett

This paper examines how innovative skills and knowledge are transmitted and acquired among adolescents in two hunter-gatherer communities, the Aka of southern Central African Republic and the Chabu of southwestern Ethiopia. Modes of transmission and processes of social learning are addressed. Innovation as well as social learning have been hypothesized to be key features of human cumulative culture, enhancing the fitness and survival of individuals in diverse environments. The innovation literature indicates adult males are more innovative than children and female adults and therefore predicts that adolescents will seek out adult males. Further, the mode of transmission should be oblique (i.e., learning from adults other than parents). Thus, learning of innovations should be oblique or horizontal rather than vertical, with adolescents paying particular attention to "successful" innovative individuals (prestige bias). The social learning literature indicates that complex skills or knowledge is likely to be learned through teaching, and therefore that teaching will be an important process in the transmission of innovations. In-depth and semi-structured interviews, informal observations, and systematic free-listing were used to evaluate these hypotheses. The study found that (1) cultural context patterned whether or not adolescents sought out adult male or female innovators; (2) oblique modes of transmission were mentioned with greater frequency than horizontal or vertical modes; (3) knowledge and skill bias was notable and explicitly linked by the adolescents to reproductive effort; and (4) teaching was biased toward same-sex individuals and was an important but not an exclusive means of transmitting complex skills and social knowledge.

本文考察了中非共和国南部的阿卡族和埃塞俄比亚西南部的查布族这两个狩猎采集社区的青少年如何传播和获得创新技能和知识。讨论了社会学习的传播模式和过程。创新和社会学习被假设为人类积累文化的关键特征,增强了个体在不同环境中的适应性和生存能力。创新文献表明,成年男性比儿童和成年女性更具创新能力,因此预测青少年会寻找成年男性。此外,传播方式应该是间接的(即从父母以外的成年人那里学习)。因此,创新的学习应该是倾斜的或水平的,而不是垂直的,青少年特别关注“成功的”创新个体(声望偏见)。社会学习文献表明,复杂的技能或知识很可能是通过教学来学习的,因此教学将是创新传播的重要过程。深入和半结构化访谈,非正式观察和系统的自由上市被用来评估这些假设。研究发现:(1)文化背景决定了青少年是否寻求成年男性或女性创新者;(2)斜向传播方式的频率高于水平或垂直传播方式;(3)青少年的知识和技能偏好与生育努力之间存在显著且明确的联系;(4)教学偏向于同性个体,是传递复杂技能和社会知识的重要手段,但不是唯一的手段。
{"title":"Social Learning and Innovation in Adolescence : A Comparative Study of Aka and Chabu Hunter-Gatherers of Central and Eastern Africa.","authors":"Bonnie Hewlett","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09391-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09391-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This paper examines how innovative skills and knowledge are transmitted and acquired among adolescents in two hunter-gatherer communities, the Aka of southern Central African Republic and the Chabu of southwestern Ethiopia. Modes of transmission and processes of social learning are addressed. Innovation as well as social learning have been hypothesized to be key features of human cumulative culture, enhancing the fitness and survival of individuals in diverse environments. The innovation literature indicates adult males are more innovative than children and female adults and therefore predicts that adolescents will seek out adult males. Further, the mode of transmission should be oblique (i.e., learning from adults other than parents). Thus, learning of innovations should be oblique or horizontal rather than vertical, with adolescents paying particular attention to \"successful\" innovative individuals (prestige bias). The social learning literature indicates that complex skills or knowledge is likely to be learned through teaching, and therefore that teaching will be an important process in the transmission of innovations. In-depth and semi-structured interviews, informal observations, and systematic free-listing were used to evaluate these hypotheses. The study found that (1) cultural context patterned whether or not adolescents sought out adult male or female innovators; (2) oblique modes of transmission were mentioned with greater frequency than horizontal or vertical modes; (3) knowledge and skill bias was notable and explicitly linked by the adolescents to reproductive effort; and (4) teaching was biased toward same-sex individuals and was an important but not an exclusive means of transmitting complex skills and social knowledge.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 1","pages":"239-278"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-021-09391-y","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38815801","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
The Life History of Learning Subsistence Skills among Hadza and BaYaka Foragers from Tanzania and the Republic of Congo. 坦桑尼亚和刚果共和国的哈扎人和巴雅卡人学习谋生技能的生活史。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-03-01 Epub Date: 2021-05-13 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09386-9
Sheina Lew-Levy, Erik J Ringen, Alyssa N Crittenden, Ibrahim A Mabulla, Tanya Broesch, Michelle A Kline

Aspects of human life history and cognition, such as our long childhoods and extensive use of teaching, theoretically evolved to facilitate the acquisition of complex tasks. The present paper empirically examines the relationship between subsistence task difficulty and age of acquisition, rates of teaching, and rates of oblique transmission among Hadza and BaYaka foragers from Tanzania and the Republic of Congo. We further examine cross-cultural variation in how and from whom learning occurred. Learning patterns and community perceptions of task difficulty were assessed through interviews. We found no relationship between task difficulty, age of acquisition, and oblique transmission, and a weak but positive relationship between task difficulty and rates of teaching. While same-sex transmission was normative in both societies, tasks ranked as more difficult were more likely to be transmitted by men among the BaYaka, but not among the Hadza, potentially reflecting cross-cultural differences in the sexual division of subsistence and teaching labor. Further, the BaYaka were more likely to report learning via teaching, and less likely to report learning via observation, than the Hadza, possibly owing to differences in socialization practices.

人类生活史和认知的一些方面,如漫长的童年和广泛使用教学,从理论上讲是为了促进复杂任务的习得而进化的。本文以经验为基础,研究了坦桑尼亚和刚果共和国的哈扎人和巴雅卡人的生存任务难度与习得年龄、教学率和斜传率之间的关系。我们还进一步研究了学习方式和学习对象的跨文化差异。我们通过访谈评估了学习模式和社区对任务难度的看法。我们发现,任务难度、习得年龄和顺向传播之间没有关系,任务难度和传授率之间的关系微弱但呈正相关。虽然同性传授在两个社会中都是正常的,但在巴雅卡人中,难度较高的任务更有可能由男性传授,而在哈扎人中则不然,这可能反映了生计和教学劳动在性别分工上的跨文化差异。此外,与哈扎人相比,巴雅卡人更倾向于通过教学来学习,而较少通过观察来学习,这可能是由于社会化实践的差异造成的。
{"title":"The Life History of Learning Subsistence Skills among Hadza and BaYaka Foragers from Tanzania and the Republic of Congo.","authors":"Sheina Lew-Levy, Erik J Ringen, Alyssa N Crittenden, Ibrahim A Mabulla, Tanya Broesch, Michelle A Kline","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09386-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s12110-021-09386-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Aspects of human life history and cognition, such as our long childhoods and extensive use of teaching, theoretically evolved to facilitate the acquisition of complex tasks. The present paper empirically examines the relationship between subsistence task difficulty and age of acquisition, rates of teaching, and rates of oblique transmission among Hadza and BaYaka foragers from Tanzania and the Republic of Congo. We further examine cross-cultural variation in how and from whom learning occurred. Learning patterns and community perceptions of task difficulty were assessed through interviews. We found no relationship between task difficulty, age of acquisition, and oblique transmission, and a weak but positive relationship between task difficulty and rates of teaching. While same-sex transmission was normative in both societies, tasks ranked as more difficult were more likely to be transmitted by men among the BaYaka, but not among the Hadza, potentially reflecting cross-cultural differences in the sexual division of subsistence and teaching labor. Further, the BaYaka were more likely to report learning via teaching, and less likely to report learning via observation, than the Hadza, possibly owing to differences in socialization practices.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 1","pages":"16-47"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8208923/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38975291","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Co-occurrence of Ostensive Communication and Generalizable Knowledge in Forager Storytelling : Cross-Cultural Evidence of Teaching in Forager Societies. 在采集者讲故事中,显性交际和可概括知识的共存:采集者社会教学的跨文化证据。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-03-01 Epub Date: 2021-04-12 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09385-w
Michelle Scalise Sugiyama

Teaching is hypothesized to be a species-typical behavior in humans that contributed to the emergence of cumulative culture. Several within-culture studies indicate that foragers depend heavily on social learning to acquire practical skills and knowledge, but it is unknown whether teaching is universal across forager populations. Teaching can be defined ethologically as the modification of behavior by an expert in the presence of a novice, such that the expert incurs a cost and the novice acquires skills/knowledge more efficiently or that it would not acquire otherwise. One behavioral modification hypothesized to be an adaptation for teaching is ostensive communication-exaggerations of prosody and gesture that signal intent to transmit generalizable knowledge and indicate the intended receiver. On this view, the use of ostensive communication in conjunction with the transmission of generalizable knowledge constitutes evidence of teaching. Oral storytelling appears to meet these criteria: Indigenous peoples regard their traditions as important sources of ecological and social knowledge, and oral storytelling is widely reported to employ paralinguistic communication. To test this hypothesis, descriptions of performed narrative in forager societies were coded for the use of 14 ostensive-communicative behaviors and the presence of generalizable knowledge. Although biased toward North America, the study sample comprised 53 forager cultures spanning five continents, 34 language families, and diverse biomes. All cultures evinced the predicted behaviors. Results suggest that foragers use storytelling as a mode of instruction, thus providing cross-cultural evidence of teaching in forager populations.

教学被认为是人类的一种典型行为,有助于积累文化的出现。几项文化内部研究表明,觅食者在很大程度上依赖于社会学习来获得实用技能和知识,但尚不清楚教学是否在觅食者群体中普遍存在。从行为学上讲,教学可以定义为专家在新手面前对行为的修改,这样专家就会付出代价,新手就能更有效地获得技能/知识,否则就无法获得。一种被假设为适应教学的行为改变是明示交际——夸张的韵律和手势,表明意图传递可概括的知识,并指出预期的接收者。根据这一观点,使用明示交际与传播可概括的知识构成教学的证据。口头讲故事似乎符合这些标准:土著人民认为他们的传统是生态和社会知识的重要来源,口头讲故事被广泛报道为使用副语言交流。为了验证这一假设,对采集社会中表演叙事的描述进行了编码,使用14种明示交际行为和可概括知识的存在。虽然偏向于北美,但研究样本包括跨越五大洲的53种觅食文化,34个语系和不同的生物群落。所有的文化都证明了预测的行为。研究结果表明,采集者使用讲故事作为一种教学模式,从而为采集者群体的教学提供了跨文化证据。
{"title":"Co-occurrence of Ostensive Communication and Generalizable Knowledge in Forager Storytelling : Cross-Cultural Evidence of Teaching in Forager Societies.","authors":"Michelle Scalise Sugiyama","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09385-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09385-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Teaching is hypothesized to be a species-typical behavior in humans that contributed to the emergence of cumulative culture. Several within-culture studies indicate that foragers depend heavily on social learning to acquire practical skills and knowledge, but it is unknown whether teaching is universal across forager populations. Teaching can be defined ethologically as the modification of behavior by an expert in the presence of a novice, such that the expert incurs a cost and the novice acquires skills/knowledge more efficiently or that it would not acquire otherwise. One behavioral modification hypothesized to be an adaptation for teaching is ostensive communication-exaggerations of prosody and gesture that signal intent to transmit generalizable knowledge and indicate the intended receiver. On this view, the use of ostensive communication in conjunction with the transmission of generalizable knowledge constitutes evidence of teaching. Oral storytelling appears to meet these criteria: Indigenous peoples regard their traditions as important sources of ecological and social knowledge, and oral storytelling is widely reported to employ paralinguistic communication. To test this hypothesis, descriptions of performed narrative in forager societies were coded for the use of 14 ostensive-communicative behaviors and the presence of generalizable knowledge. Although biased toward North America, the study sample comprised 53 forager cultures spanning five continents, 34 language families, and diverse biomes. All cultures evinced the predicted behaviors. Results suggest that foragers use storytelling as a mode of instruction, thus providing cross-cultural evidence of teaching in forager populations.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 1","pages":"279-300"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-021-09385-w","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25584205","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Cultural Change Reduces Gender Differences in Mobility and Spatial Ability among Seminomadic Pastoralist-Forager Children in Northern Namibia. 文化变迁降低了纳米比亚北部半游牧游牧儿童流动性和空间能力的性别差异。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-03-01 Epub Date: 2021-04-22 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09388-7
Helen E Davis, Jonathan Stack, Elizabeth Cashdan

A fundamental cognitive function found across a wide range of species and necessary for survival is the ability to navigate complex environments. It has been suggested that mobility may play an important role in the development of spatial skills. Despite evolutionary arguments offering logical explanations for why sex/gender differences in spatial abilities and mobility might exist, thus far there has been limited sampling from nonindustrialized and subsistence-based societies. This lack of sampling diversity has left many unanswered questions regarding the effects that environmental variation and cultural norms may have in shaping mobility patterns during childhood and the development of spatial competencies that may be associated with it. Here we examine variation in mobility (through GPS tracking and interviews), performance on large-scale spatial skills (i.e., navigational ability), and performance on small-scale spatial skills (e.g., mental rotation task, Corsi blocks task, and water-level task) among Twa forager/pastoralist children whose daily lives have been dramatically altered since settlement and the introduction of government-funded boarding schools. Unlike in previous findings among Twa adults, boys and girls (N = 88; aged 6-18) show similar patterns of travel on all measures of mobility. We also find no significant differences in spatial task performance by gender for large- or small-scale spatial skills. Further, children performed as well as adults did on mental rotation, and they outperformed adults on the water-level task. We discuss how children's early learning environments may influence the development of both large- and small-scale spatial skills.

许多物种都有一种基本的认知功能,这是生存所必需的,那就是驾驭复杂环境的能力。有人认为,移动性可能在空间技能的发展中起着重要作用。尽管进化论为空间能力和流动性的性别差异提供了合乎逻辑的解释,但迄今为止,来自非工业化和以生存为基础的社会的样本有限。这种样本多样性的缺乏留下了许多悬而未决的问题,如环境变化和文化规范在塑造儿童流动模式和可能与之相关的空间能力发展方面可能产生的影响。在这里,我们研究了自定居和引入政府资助的寄宿学校以来,日常生活发生了巨大变化的Twa觅食/游牧儿童在流动性(通过GPS跟踪和访谈)、大规模空间技能(如导航能力)和小规模空间技能(如心理旋转任务、科西积木任务和水位任务)方面的变化。与之前在两名成年人中发现的不同,男孩和女孩(N = 88;年龄在6-18岁之间的年轻人,在各种出行方式上都表现出相似的出行模式。我们还发现,在空间任务表现上,男女在大型或小型空间技能上没有显著差异。此外,儿童在心理旋转上的表现和成年人一样好,他们在水位任务上的表现优于成年人。我们讨论了儿童的早期学习环境如何影响大型和小型空间技能的发展。
{"title":"Cultural Change Reduces Gender Differences in Mobility and Spatial Ability among Seminomadic Pastoralist-Forager Children in Northern Namibia.","authors":"Helen E Davis,&nbsp;Jonathan Stack,&nbsp;Elizabeth Cashdan","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09388-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09388-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A fundamental cognitive function found across a wide range of species and necessary for survival is the ability to navigate complex environments. It has been suggested that mobility may play an important role in the development of spatial skills. Despite evolutionary arguments offering logical explanations for why sex/gender differences in spatial abilities and mobility might exist, thus far there has been limited sampling from nonindustrialized and subsistence-based societies. This lack of sampling diversity has left many unanswered questions regarding the effects that environmental variation and cultural norms may have in shaping mobility patterns during childhood and the development of spatial competencies that may be associated with it. Here we examine variation in mobility (through GPS tracking and interviews), performance on large-scale spatial skills (i.e., navigational ability), and performance on small-scale spatial skills (e.g., mental rotation task, Corsi blocks task, and water-level task) among Twa forager/pastoralist children whose daily lives have been dramatically altered since settlement and the introduction of government-funded boarding schools. Unlike in previous findings among Twa adults, boys and girls (N = 88; aged 6-18) show similar patterns of travel on all measures of mobility. We also find no significant differences in spatial task performance by gender for large- or small-scale spatial skills. Further, children performed as well as adults did on mental rotation, and they outperformed adults on the water-level task. We discuss how children's early learning environments may influence the development of both large- and small-scale spatial skills.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 1","pages":"178-206"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-021-09388-7","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38897696","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Age-Appropriate Wisdom? : Ethnobiological Knowledge Ontogeny in Pastoralist Mexican Choyeros. 适合孩子年龄的智慧吗?:墨西哥游牧部落个体发生的民族生物学知识。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-03-01 Epub Date: 2021-04-23 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09387-8
Eric Schniter, Shane J Macfarlan, Juan J Garcia, Gorgonio Ruiz-Campos, Diego Guevara Beltran, Brenda B Bowen, Jory C Lerback

We investigate whether age profiles of ethnobiological knowledge development are consistent with predictions derived from life history theory about the timing of productivity and reproduction. Life history models predict complementary knowledge profiles developing across the lifespan for women and men as they experience changes in embodied capital and the needs of dependent offspring. We evaluate these predictions using an ethnobiological knowledge assessment tool developed for an off-grid pastoralist population known as Choyeros, from Baja California Sur, Mexico. Our results indicate that while individuals acquire knowledge of most dangerous items and edible resources by early adulthood, knowledge of plants and animals relevant to the age and sex divided labor domains and ecologies (e.g., women's house gardens, men's herding activities in the wilderness) continues to develop into middle adulthood but to different degrees and at different rates for men and women. As the demands of offspring on parents accumulate with age, reproductive-aged adults continue to develop their knowledge to meet their children's needs. After controlling for vision, our analysis indicates that many post-reproductive adults show the greatest ethnobiological knowledge. These findings extend our understanding of the evolved human life history by illustrating how changes in embodied capital and the needs of dependent offspring predict the development of men's and women's ethnobiological knowledge across the lifespan.

我们研究了民族生物学知识发展的年龄分布是否与生活史理论中关于生产力和繁殖时间的预测相一致。生活史模型预测,随着女性和男性在具体资本和依赖后代的需求方面的变化,他们在整个生命周期中会形成互补的知识概况。我们使用一种民族生物学知识评估工具对这些预测进行了评估,该工具是为墨西哥下加利福尼亚州南部一个被称为Choyeros的离网牧民群体开发的。我们的研究结果表明,虽然个体在成年早期获得了最危险物品和可食用资源的知识,但与年龄和性别划分的劳动领域和生态相关的动植物知识(例如,女性的家庭花园,男性在荒野中的放牧活动)在成年中期继续发展,但男女的程度和速度不同。由于子女对父母的需求随着年龄的增长而增加,育龄成年人继续发展自己的知识以满足子女的需求。在控制视力后,我们的分析表明,许多生育后的成年人表现出最大的民族生物学知识。这些发现扩展了我们对进化的人类生活史的理解,说明了具体化资本的变化和依赖后代的需求如何预测男性和女性在整个生命周期中民族生物学知识的发展。
{"title":"Age-Appropriate Wisdom? : Ethnobiological Knowledge Ontogeny in Pastoralist Mexican Choyeros.","authors":"Eric Schniter,&nbsp;Shane J Macfarlan,&nbsp;Juan J Garcia,&nbsp;Gorgonio Ruiz-Campos,&nbsp;Diego Guevara Beltran,&nbsp;Brenda B Bowen,&nbsp;Jory C Lerback","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09387-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09387-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We investigate whether age profiles of ethnobiological knowledge development are consistent with predictions derived from life history theory about the timing of productivity and reproduction. Life history models predict complementary knowledge profiles developing across the lifespan for women and men as they experience changes in embodied capital and the needs of dependent offspring. We evaluate these predictions using an ethnobiological knowledge assessment tool developed for an off-grid pastoralist population known as Choyeros, from Baja California Sur, Mexico. Our results indicate that while individuals acquire knowledge of most dangerous items and edible resources by early adulthood, knowledge of plants and animals relevant to the age and sex divided labor domains and ecologies (e.g., women's house gardens, men's herding activities in the wilderness) continues to develop into middle adulthood but to different degrees and at different rates for men and women. As the demands of offspring on parents accumulate with age, reproductive-aged adults continue to develop their knowledge to meet their children's needs. After controlling for vision, our analysis indicates that many post-reproductive adults show the greatest ethnobiological knowledge. These findings extend our understanding of the evolved human life history by illustrating how changes in embodied capital and the needs of dependent offspring predict the development of men's and women's ethnobiological knowledge across the lifespan.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 1","pages":"48-83"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-021-09387-8","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38901520","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Early Plant Learning in Fiji. 斐济的早期植物学习。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-03-01 Epub Date: 2021-03-26 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09389-6
Rita Anne McNamara, Annie E Wertz

Recent work with infants suggests that plant foraging throughout evolutionary history has shaped the design of the human mind. Infants in Germany and the US avoid touching plants and engage in more social looking toward adults before touching them. This combination of behavioral avoidance and social looking strategies enables safe and rapid social learning about plant properties within the first two years of life. Here, we explore how growing up in a context that requires frequent interaction with plants shapes children's responses with the participation of communities in rural Fiji. We conducted two interviews with adults and a behavioral study with children. The adult interviews map the plant learning landscape in these communities and provide context for the child study. The child study used a time-to-touch paradigm to examine whether 6- to 48-month-olds (N = 33) in participating communities exhibit avoidance behaviors and social looking patterns that are similar to, or different from, those of German and American infants. Our adult interview results confirmed that knowledge about daily and medicinal uses of plants is widely known throughout the communities, and children are given many opportunities to informally learn about plants. The results of the child behavioral study suggest that young Fijian children, like German and American infants, are reluctant to reach for novel artificial plants and are fastest to interact with familiar household items and shells. In contrast to German and American infants, Fijian children also quickly reached for familiar real plants and did not engage in differential social looking before touching them. These results suggest that cultural contexts flexibly shape the development of plant-relevant cognitive design.

最近对婴儿的研究表明,在进化史上,植物觅食塑造了人类思维的设计。德国和美国的婴儿避免触摸植物,在触摸它们之前,他们会对成年人进行更多的社交性观察。这种行为回避和社交寻找策略的结合,使它们能够在生命的头两年内安全、快速地对植物特性进行社交学习。在这里,我们探讨了在斐济农村社区中,在需要经常与植物互动的环境中长大的儿童是如何通过社区参与来塑造他们的反应的。我们对成年人进行了两次访谈,并对儿童进行了行为研究。成人访谈描绘了这些社区的植物学习景观,并为儿童研究提供了背景。这项儿童研究使用了触摸时间范式来检验参与社区的6- 48个月大的婴儿(N = 33)是否表现出与德国和美国婴儿相似或不同的回避行为和社会注视模式。我们对成年人的采访结果证实,植物的日常和药用知识在整个社区都广为人知,儿童也有很多非正式学习植物的机会。儿童行为研究的结果表明,斐济的幼儿,像德国和美国的婴儿一样,不愿意伸手去拿新奇的人造植物,而与熟悉的家居用品和贝壳互动的速度最快。与德国和美国的婴儿相比,斐济的孩子也会很快伸手去拿熟悉的真实植物,并且在触摸它们之前不会进行不同的社会观察。这些结果表明,文化背景灵活地塑造了植物相关认知设计的发展。
{"title":"Early Plant Learning in Fiji.","authors":"Rita Anne McNamara,&nbsp;Annie E Wertz","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09389-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09389-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent work with infants suggests that plant foraging throughout evolutionary history has shaped the design of the human mind. Infants in Germany and the US avoid touching plants and engage in more social looking toward adults before touching them. This combination of behavioral avoidance and social looking strategies enables safe and rapid social learning about plant properties within the first two years of life. Here, we explore how growing up in a context that requires frequent interaction with plants shapes children's responses with the participation of communities in rural Fiji. We conducted two interviews with adults and a behavioral study with children. The adult interviews map the plant learning landscape in these communities and provide context for the child study. The child study used a time-to-touch paradigm to examine whether 6- to 48-month-olds (N = 33) in participating communities exhibit avoidance behaviors and social looking patterns that are similar to, or different from, those of German and American infants. Our adult interview results confirmed that knowledge about daily and medicinal uses of plants is widely known throughout the communities, and children are given many opportunities to informally learn about plants. The results of the child behavioral study suggest that young Fijian children, like German and American infants, are reluctant to reach for novel artificial plants and are fastest to interact with familiar household items and shells. In contrast to German and American infants, Fijian children also quickly reached for familiar real plants and did not engage in differential social looking before touching them. These results suggest that cultural contexts flexibly shape the development of plant-relevant cognitive design.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 1","pages":"115-149"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-021-09389-6","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25519276","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Dispositional Fear and Political Attitudes. 性格恐惧和政治态度。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2020-12-01 Epub Date: 2020-12-03 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-020-09378-1
Peter K Hatemi, Rose McDermott

Previous work proposes that dispositional fear exists predominantly among political conservatives, generating the appearance that fears align strictly along party lines. This view obscures evolutionary dynamics because fear evolved to protect against myriad threats, not merely those in the political realm. We suggest prior work in this area has been biased by selection on the dependent variable, resulting from an examination of exclusively politically oriented fears that privilege conservative values. Because the adaptation regulating fear should be based upon both universal and ancestral-specific selection pressures combined with developmental and individual differences, the elicitation of it should prove variable across the ideological continuum dependent upon specific combinations of fear and value domains. In a sample of ~ 1,600 Australians assessed with a subset of the Fear Survey Schedule II, we find fears not infused with political content are differentially influential across the political spectrum. Specifically, those who are more fearful of sharp objects, graveyards, and urinating in public are more socially conservative and less supportive of gay rights. Those who are more fearful of death are more supportive of gay rights. Those who are more fearful of suffocating and swimming alone are more concerned about emissions controls and immigration, while those who are more fearful of thunderstorms are also more anti-immigration. Contrary to existing research, both liberals and conservatives are more fearful of different circumstances, and the role of dispositional fears are attitude-specific.

先前的研究表明,性格恐惧主要存在于政治保守派中,造成了恐惧严格按照政党路线排列的表象。这种观点模糊了进化动力学,因为恐惧的进化是为了抵御无数的威胁,而不仅仅是政治领域的威胁。我们认为,之前在这一领域的工作由于对因变量的选择而存在偏见,这是由于对特权保守价值观的完全政治导向的恐惧的检查造成的。因为调节恐惧的适应应该基于普遍的和祖先特有的选择压力,结合发展和个体差异,它的激发应该在意识形态连续体中被证明是可变的,依赖于恐惧和价值领域的特定组合。在约1600名澳大利亚人的样本中,我们用恐惧调查附表二的一个子集进行了评估,我们发现,没有注入政治内容的恐惧在政治范围内的影响是不同的。具体来说,那些更害怕尖锐物品、墓地和在公共场所小便的人在社会上更保守,更不支持同性恋权利。那些更害怕死亡的人更支持同性恋权利。那些更害怕窒息和独自游泳的人更关心排放控制和移民,而那些更害怕雷暴的人也更反对移民。与现有的研究相反,自由主义者和保守主义者都对不同的环境更害怕,而且性格恐惧的作用是态度特定的。
{"title":"Dispositional Fear and Political Attitudes.","authors":"Peter K Hatemi,&nbsp;Rose McDermott","doi":"10.1007/s12110-020-09378-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-020-09378-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous work proposes that dispositional fear exists predominantly among political conservatives, generating the appearance that fears align strictly along party lines. This view obscures evolutionary dynamics because fear evolved to protect against myriad threats, not merely those in the political realm. We suggest prior work in this area has been biased by selection on the dependent variable, resulting from an examination of exclusively politically oriented fears that privilege conservative values. Because the adaptation regulating fear should be based upon both universal and ancestral-specific selection pressures combined with developmental and individual differences, the elicitation of it should prove variable across the ideological continuum dependent upon specific combinations of fear and value domains. In a sample of ~ 1,600 Australians assessed with a subset of the Fear Survey Schedule II, we find fears not infused with political content are differentially influential across the political spectrum. Specifically, those who are more fearful of sharp objects, graveyards, and urinating in public are more socially conservative and less supportive of gay rights. Those who are more fearful of death are more supportive of gay rights. Those who are more fearful of suffocating and swimming alone are more concerned about emissions controls and immigration, while those who are more fearful of thunderstorms are also more anti-immigration. Contrary to existing research, both liberals and conservatives are more fearful of different circumstances, and the role of dispositional fears are attitude-specific.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"31 4","pages":"387-405"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2020-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-020-09378-1","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38331482","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
The "Social Brain," Reciprocity, and Social Network Segregation along Ethnic Boundaries. “社会大脑”,互惠,以及沿种族边界的社会网络隔离。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2020-12-01 Epub Date: 2021-01-11 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-020-09382-5
Michael Windzio

How does segregation along ethnic boundaries emerge in social networks? Human evolution resulted in highly social beings, capable of prosociality, mindreading, and self-control, which are important aspects of the "social brain." Our neurophysiologically "wired" social cognition implies different cognitive goal frames. In line with recent developments in behavioral theory, the present study defines network ties as episodes of social exchange. This dynamic definition can account for shifts in goal frames during an exchange episode: whereas deliberate choice and hedonic or gain goals drive the initiation of a tie, given the opportunity structure, the normative goal frame activates a strong dynamic effect of reciprocity, which limits actors' choice set and appears as "self-organization" at the network level. Longitudinal analyses of 18 birthday party networks comprising 501 students support the definition of network ties as exchange episodes, as well as the relevance of humans' inherent tendency to reciprocate. However, reciprocation is much stronger in dyads of the same ethnicity than in dyads of different ethnicities. Network segregation along ethnic boundaries results from deliberate decisions during the initiation of an episode, but also from different commitments to reciprocity during the ongoing exchange process, depending on intra or interethnic dyadic constellations.

沿着种族边界的隔离是如何在社会网络中出现的?人类进化产生了高度社会化的生物,能够亲社会、读心术和自我控制,这些都是“社会大脑”的重要方面。我们的神经生理学“有线”社会认知意味着不同的认知目标框架。根据行为理论的最新发展,本研究将网络联系定义为社会交换的片段。这种动态定义可以解释交换过程中目标框架的变化:考虑到机会结构,故意选择和享乐或收益目标驱动纽带的启动,规范的目标框架激活了互惠的强大动态效应,这限制了行动者的选择集,并在网络层面上表现为“自组织”。对由501名学生组成的18个生日派对网络的纵向分析支持了网络关系作为交换事件的定义,以及人类固有的互惠倾向的相关性。然而,同种族的二代比不同种族的二代的相互作用要强得多。沿着种族边界的网络隔离是由事件开始时的深思熟虑的决定造成的,但也源于正在进行的交换过程中对互惠的不同承诺,这取决于种族内部或种族间的二元星座。
{"title":"The \"Social Brain,\" Reciprocity, and Social Network Segregation along Ethnic Boundaries.","authors":"Michael Windzio","doi":"10.1007/s12110-020-09382-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-020-09382-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How does segregation along ethnic boundaries emerge in social networks? Human evolution resulted in highly social beings, capable of prosociality, mindreading, and self-control, which are important aspects of the \"social brain.\" Our neurophysiologically \"wired\" social cognition implies different cognitive goal frames. In line with recent developments in behavioral theory, the present study defines network ties as episodes of social exchange. This dynamic definition can account for shifts in goal frames during an exchange episode: whereas deliberate choice and hedonic or gain goals drive the initiation of a tie, given the opportunity structure, the normative goal frame activates a strong dynamic effect of reciprocity, which limits actors' choice set and appears as \"self-organization\" at the network level. Longitudinal analyses of 18 birthday party networks comprising 501 students support the definition of network ties as exchange episodes, as well as the relevance of humans' inherent tendency to reciprocate. However, reciprocation is much stronger in dyads of the same ethnicity than in dyads of different ethnicities. Network segregation along ethnic boundaries results from deliberate decisions during the initiation of an episode, but also from different commitments to reciprocity during the ongoing exchange process, depending on intra or interethnic dyadic constellations.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"31 4","pages":"443-461"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2020-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-020-09382-5","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38805511","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Are Moral Intuitions Heritable? 道德直觉是遗传的吗?
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2020-12-01 Epub Date: 2021-01-08 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-020-09380-7
Kevin Smith, Peter K Hatemi

Two prominent theoretical frameworks in moral psychology, Moral Foundations and Dual Process Theory, share a broad foundational assumption that individual differences in human morality are dispositional and in part due to genetic variation. The only published direct test of heritability, however, found little evidence of genetic influences on moral judgments using instrumentation approaches associated with Moral Foundations Theory. This raised questions about one of the core assumptions underpinning intuitionist theories of moral psychology. Here we examine the heritability of moral psychology using the moral dilemmas approach commonly used in Dual Process Theory research. Using such measures, we find consistent and significant evidence of heritability. These findings have important implications not only for understanding which measures do, or do not, tap into the genetically influenced aspects of moral decision-making, but in better establishing the utility and validity of different intuitionist theoretical frameworks and the source of why people differ in those frameworks.

道德心理学中两个重要的理论框架,道德基础理论和双重过程理论,都有一个广泛的基本假设,即人类道德的个体差异是性格的,部分是由于遗传变异。然而,唯一发表的直接遗传测试发现,使用与道德基础理论相关的工具方法,基因对道德判断的影响几乎没有证据。这引发了对道德心理学直觉主义理论的核心假设之一的质疑。在这里,我们使用双重过程理论研究中常用的道德困境方法来检验道德心理的遗传性。使用这些措施,我们发现了一致和显著的遗传证据。这些发现不仅对理解哪些措施能或不能利用道德决策的遗传影响方面具有重要意义,而且有助于更好地确立不同直觉主义理论框架的效用和有效性,以及人们在这些框架中存在差异的原因。
{"title":"Are Moral Intuitions Heritable?","authors":"Kevin Smith,&nbsp;Peter K Hatemi","doi":"10.1007/s12110-020-09380-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-020-09380-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two prominent theoretical frameworks in moral psychology, Moral Foundations and Dual Process Theory, share a broad foundational assumption that individual differences in human morality are dispositional and in part due to genetic variation. The only published direct test of heritability, however, found little evidence of genetic influences on moral judgments using instrumentation approaches associated with Moral Foundations Theory. This raised questions about one of the core assumptions underpinning intuitionist theories of moral psychology. Here we examine the heritability of moral psychology using the moral dilemmas approach commonly used in Dual Process Theory research. Using such measures, we find consistent and significant evidence of heritability. These findings have important implications not only for understanding which measures do, or do not, tap into the genetically influenced aspects of moral decision-making, but in better establishing the utility and validity of different intuitionist theoretical frameworks and the source of why people differ in those frameworks.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"31 4","pages":"406-420"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2020-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-020-09380-7","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39135480","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
期刊
Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1