首页 > 最新文献

Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective最新文献

英文 中文
A Naturalistic Study of Norm Conformity, Punishment, and the Veneration of the Dead at Texas A&M University, USA. 美国德州农工大学规范遵从、惩罚与死者崇拜的自然主义研究。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-09-01 Epub Date: 2021-09-17 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09413-9
Michael Alvard, Katherine Daiy

Culturally inherited institutional norms structure much of human social life. Successfully replicating institutions train their current members to behave in the generally adaptive ways that served past members. Ancestor veneration is a well-known manifestation of this phenomenon whereby deference is conferred to prestigious past members who are used as cultural models. Such norms of respect may be maintained by punishment based on evidence from theory and laboratory experiments, but there is little observational evidence to show that punishment is commonly used. To test for punishment as a mechanism that maintains these norms, we examine a norm of ancestor veneration in a natural field experiment at the Memorial Student Center (MSC) at Texas A&M University. The MSC is a memorial to university war dead, and the expectation is that all who enter the building remove their hats out of respect. Observations reveal that hat removal is significantly more common at the MSC than at two control locations. Survey data indicate that most, but not all, subjects understand the norm to be veneration of the dead, and most expect others to follow the norm. Many report a strong negative emotional response when asked to imagine the norm being violated. Sixty-two percent report they would definitely or probably ask the noncompliant to uncover. Focal follow data show that punishment is relatively rare, however, with the majority of behatted subjects going unreproached as they pass through the building. Both survey and observational data indicate there is a motivated minority that enthusiastically enforces the norm.

文化上继承下来的制度规范构成了人类社会生活的大部分。成功复制的机构会训练现有成员,让他们按照服务于过去成员的普遍适应性方式行事。祖先崇拜是这种现象的一种众所周知的表现形式,即对作为文化典范的有声望的过去成员给予尊重。基于理论和实验室实验的证据,惩罚可以维持这种尊重规范,但很少有观察证据表明惩罚是普遍使用的。为了检验惩罚作为维持这些规范的机制,我们在德克萨斯州农工大学纪念学生中心(MSC)的自然田野实验中考察了祖先崇拜的规范。MSC是纪念大学战争死难者的地方,人们希望所有进入大楼的人都摘下帽子以示尊重。观察结果显示,脱帽在MSC比在两个对照位置明显更常见。调查数据显示,大多数(但不是全部)研究对象都理解对死者的尊敬是规范,而且大多数人都希望其他人遵循这一规范。当被要求想象违反规范时,许多人都表现出强烈的负面情绪反应。62%的人表示,他们肯定或可能会要求不合规的人揭露。然而,焦点跟踪数据显示,惩罚相对较少,大多数被殴打的对象在穿过大楼时都没有受到责备。调查和观察数据都表明,有少数人积极地执行规范。
{"title":"A Naturalistic Study of Norm Conformity, Punishment, and the Veneration of the Dead at Texas A&M University, USA.","authors":"Michael Alvard,&nbsp;Katherine Daiy","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09413-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09413-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Culturally inherited institutional norms structure much of human social life. Successfully replicating institutions train their current members to behave in the generally adaptive ways that served past members. Ancestor veneration is a well-known manifestation of this phenomenon whereby deference is conferred to prestigious past members who are used as cultural models. Such norms of respect may be maintained by punishment based on evidence from theory and laboratory experiments, but there is little observational evidence to show that punishment is commonly used. To test for punishment as a mechanism that maintains these norms, we examine a norm of ancestor veneration in a natural field experiment at the Memorial Student Center (MSC) at Texas A&M University. The MSC is a memorial to university war dead, and the expectation is that all who enter the building remove their hats out of respect. Observations reveal that hat removal is significantly more common at the MSC than at two control locations. Survey data indicate that most, but not all, subjects understand the norm to be veneration of the dead, and most expect others to follow the norm. Many report a strong negative emotional response when asked to imagine the norm being violated. Sixty-two percent report they would definitely or probably ask the noncompliant to uncover. Focal follow data show that punishment is relatively rare, however, with the majority of behatted subjects going unreproached as they pass through the building. Both survey and observational data indicate there is a motivated minority that enthusiastically enforces the norm.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 3","pages":"652-675"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39425058","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Maternal Grandmothers' Household Residency, Children's Growth, and Body Composition Are Not Related in Urban Maya Families from Yucatan. 尤卡坦地区玛雅城市家庭中外祖母居住、儿童生长和身体组成无关。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09402-y
Hugo Azcorra, Barry Bogin, Federico Dickinson, Maria Inês Varela-Silva

This study analyzes the influence of grandmothers' household residency on the presence of low height-for-age and excessive fat (FMI = fat mass [kg]/height [m2]), waist circumference, and sum of triceps and subscapular skinfolds in a sample of 247 6- to 8-year-old urban Maya children from Yucatan, Mexico. Between September 2011 and January 2014, we obtained anthropometric and body composition data from children and mothers, as well as socioeconomic characteristics of participants and households. Grandmothers' place of residence was categorized as either in the same household as their grandchildren (n = 71) or in separate households (n = 176). Multiple logistic regression models were used to analyze the association between grandmothers' residency and outcome variables. Models were adjusted for maternal anthropometric characteristics and the following socioeconomic variables: family size, location, maternal education, monthly family income, and household crowding. Models showed that the presence of grandmothers in their grandchildren's households was not associated with any of the outcome variables. In contrast, larger family size, overcrowding in the household, and lower family income predicted low height-for-age in children. Larger family size decreased the risk for being overweight based on the three parameters of body composition. Overcrowding in the household increased the risk for greater skinfolds thickness, while low family income increased the risk for higher fat mass index. The residency of grandmothers in their adult daughters' households is not significantly associated with the outcome variables in this sample of urban Maya families. Instead, maternal anthropometric characteristics and socioeconomic conditions of the family have a greater influence on the overall growth of children.

本研究分析了来自墨西哥尤卡坦半岛的247名6- 8岁玛雅城市儿童的祖母居住情况对低年龄身高和过度脂肪(FMI =脂肪质量[kg]/身高[m2])、腰围、肱三头肌和肩胛下皮肤皱纹总和的影响。在2011年9月至2014年1月期间,我们获得了儿童和母亲的人体测量和身体成分数据,以及参与者和家庭的社会经济特征。祖母的居住地分为与孙辈在同一个家庭(n = 71)或在不同的家庭(n = 176)。采用多元logistic回归模型分析外祖母居住状况与结果变量的关系。根据产妇的人体测量特征和以下社会经济变量对模型进行了调整:家庭规模、地点、产妇教育程度、家庭月收入和家庭拥挤程度。模型显示,孙辈家庭中祖母的存在与任何结果变量都无关。相比之下,较大的家庭规模、过度拥挤的家庭和较低的家庭收入会导致儿童的身高低于年龄。根据身体组成的三个参数,家庭规模越大,超重的风险就越低。过度拥挤的家庭增加了皮肤褶皱厚度的风险,而低收入家庭增加了脂肪质量指数较高的风险。在这个城市玛雅家庭样本中,祖母在成年女儿家庭中的居住情况与结果变量没有显著关联。相反,母亲的人体测量特征和家庭的社会经济条件对儿童的整体成长有更大的影响。
{"title":"Maternal Grandmothers' Household Residency, Children's Growth, and Body Composition Are Not Related in Urban Maya Families from Yucatan.","authors":"Hugo Azcorra,&nbsp;Barry Bogin,&nbsp;Federico Dickinson,&nbsp;Maria Inês Varela-Silva","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09402-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09402-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study analyzes the influence of grandmothers' household residency on the presence of low height-for-age and excessive fat (FMI = fat mass [kg]/height [m<sup>2</sup>]), waist circumference, and sum of triceps and subscapular skinfolds in a sample of 247 6- to 8-year-old urban Maya children from Yucatan, Mexico. Between September 2011 and January 2014, we obtained anthropometric and body composition data from children and mothers, as well as socioeconomic characteristics of participants and households. Grandmothers' place of residence was categorized as either in the same household as their grandchildren (n = 71) or in separate households (n = 176). Multiple logistic regression models were used to analyze the association between grandmothers' residency and outcome variables. Models were adjusted for maternal anthropometric characteristics and the following socioeconomic variables: family size, location, maternal education, monthly family income, and household crowding. Models showed that the presence of grandmothers in their grandchildren's households was not associated with any of the outcome variables. In contrast, larger family size, overcrowding in the household, and lower family income predicted low height-for-age in children. Larger family size decreased the risk for being overweight based on the three parameters of body composition. Overcrowding in the household increased the risk for greater skinfolds thickness, while low family income increased the risk for higher fat mass index. The residency of grandmothers in their adult daughters' households is not significantly associated with the outcome variables in this sample of urban Maya families. Instead, maternal anthropometric characteristics and socioeconomic conditions of the family have a greater influence on the overall growth of children.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 2","pages":"434-449"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-021-09402-y","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38970337","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploratory Analysis of the Relationship between Social Identification and Testosterone Reactivity to Vicarious Combat. 社会认同与替代战斗睾酮反应关系的探索性分析。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-06-01 Epub Date: 2021-07-07 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09407-7
Kathleen V Casto, Zach L Root, Shawn N Geniole, Justin M Carré, Mark W Bruner

Testosterone (T) fluctuates in response to competitive social interactions, with the direction of change typically depending on factors such as contest outcome. Watching a competition may be sufficient to activate T among fans and others who are invested in the outcome. This study explores the change in T associated with vicarious experiences of competition among combat sport athletes viewing a teammate win or lose and assesses how individual differences in social identification with one's team relates to these patterns of T reactivity. Twenty-six male combat athletes completed a social identity questionnaire on a neutral day. Later, salivary samples (assayed for T) were obtained before and after athletes viewed a video of a teammate engaged in a formal contest. T reactivity to viewing a teammate compete varied among participants in both the magnitude and direction of change, independent of contest outcome. Individual differences in cognitive centrality, a core feature of social identification, showed a strong positive relationship with T reactivity, particularly if their teammate won. Initial findings suggest that dominance-linked androgen responses associated with watching a teammate win a competition might depend on the belief that team membership is central to one's own identity. These exploratory results in a small sample of combat athletes should be interpreted with caution. Uncovering the role of social group dynamics in influencing T responses to competition is particularly important in light of the evolutionary history of coalitional combat in humans.

睾酮(T)在竞争性社会互动中波动,其变化方向通常取决于竞赛结果等因素。观看比赛可能足以激活粉丝和其他对比赛结果感兴趣的人的T。本研究探讨了搏击运动运动员在观看队友胜利或失败时,与替代竞争经验相关的T的变化,并评估了与团队社会认同的个体差异如何与这些T反应模式相关。26名男性格斗运动员在中性的一天完成了一份社会认同问卷。随后,在运动员观看队友参加正式比赛的视频之前和之后,获得唾液样本(检测T)。参与者对观看队友竞争的反应在变化的幅度和方向上都有所不同,与比赛结果无关。认知中心性(社会认同的核心特征)的个体差异显示出与T反应的强烈正相关,尤其是当他们的队友获胜时。最初的研究结果表明,与观看队友赢得比赛相关的与优势相关的雄激素反应可能取决于团队成员身份是个人身份的核心这一信念。这些在小样本格斗运动员中的探索性结果应该谨慎解释。鉴于人类联合作战的进化史,揭示社会群体动态在影响T对竞争反应中的作用尤为重要。
{"title":"Exploratory Analysis of the Relationship between Social Identification and Testosterone Reactivity to Vicarious Combat.","authors":"Kathleen V Casto,&nbsp;Zach L Root,&nbsp;Shawn N Geniole,&nbsp;Justin M Carré,&nbsp;Mark W Bruner","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09407-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09407-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Testosterone (T) fluctuates in response to competitive social interactions, with the direction of change typically depending on factors such as contest outcome. Watching a competition may be sufficient to activate T among fans and others who are invested in the outcome. This study explores the change in T associated with vicarious experiences of competition among combat sport athletes viewing a teammate win or lose and assesses how individual differences in social identification with one's team relates to these patterns of T reactivity. Twenty-six male combat athletes completed a social identity questionnaire on a neutral day. Later, salivary samples (assayed for T) were obtained before and after athletes viewed a video of a teammate engaged in a formal contest. T reactivity to viewing a teammate compete varied among participants in both the magnitude and direction of change, independent of contest outcome. Individual differences in cognitive centrality, a core feature of social identification, showed a strong positive relationship with T reactivity, particularly if their teammate won. Initial findings suggest that dominance-linked androgen responses associated with watching a teammate win a competition might depend on the belief that team membership is central to one's own identity. These exploratory results in a small sample of combat athletes should be interpreted with caution. Uncovering the role of social group dynamics in influencing T responses to competition is particularly important in light of the evolutionary history of coalitional combat in humans.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 2","pages":"509-527"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-021-09407-7","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39159788","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
"A Solidarity-Type World": Need-Based Helping among Ranchers in the Southwestern United States. “一个团结型的世界”:美国西南部牧场主基于需求的帮助。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-06-01 Epub Date: 2021-07-09 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09406-8
Lee Cronk, Diego Guevara Beltrán, Denise Laya Mercado, Athena Aktipis

To better understand risk management and mutual aid among American ranchers, we interviewed and mailed a survey to ranchers in Hidalgo County, New Mexico, and Cochise County, Arizona, focusing on two questions: (1) When do ranchers expect repayment for the help they provide others? (2) What determines ranchers' degrees of involvement in networks of mutual aid, which they refer to as "neighboring"? When needs arise due to unpredictable events, such as injuries, most ranchers reported not expecting to be paid back for the help they provide. When help is provided for something that follows a known schedule or that can be scheduled, such as branding, most ranchers did expect something in return for the help they provide. This pattern makes sense in light of computational modeling that shows that transfers to those in need without expectations of repayment pool risk more effectively than transfers that create debt. Ranchers reported helping other ranchers more often when they belonged to more religious and civic organizations, when they owned larger ranches, when they relied less on ranch vs. other income, and when they had more relatives in the area. Operators of midsize ranches reported helping other ranchers more frequently than did those on smaller and larger ranches. None of our independent variables predicted how many times ranchers reported receiving help from other ranchers. Although ranch culture in the American West is often characterized by an ethic of individualism and independence, our study suggests that this ethic stands alongside an ethic of mutual aid during times of need.

为了更好地了解美国牧场主之间的风险管理和互助,我们采访了新墨西哥州伊达尔戈县和亚利桑那州科奇斯县的牧场主,并邮寄了一份调查问卷,主要关注两个问题:(1)牧场主期望何时偿还他们提供给他人的帮助?(2)是什么决定了牧场主在他们称之为“邻居”的互助网络中的参与程度?当由于不可预测的事件(如受伤)而产生需求时,大多数牧场主报告说,他们提供的帮助并不期望得到回报。当提供的帮助遵循一个已知的时间表或可以计划的事情,如品牌,大多数牧场主确实希望他们提供的帮助有所回报。根据计算模型,这种模式是有意义的,计算模型表明,与产生债务的转移相比,向有需要的人转移而不期望还款池风险更有效。当牧场主属于更多的宗教和民间组织时,当他们拥有更大的牧场时,当他们对牧场的依赖程度低于其他收入时,当他们在该地区有更多亲戚时,牧场主报告说他们更经常帮助其他牧场主。据报道,中型牧场的经营者比小型和大型牧场的经营者更频繁地帮助其他牧场主。我们的独立变量中没有一个预测农场主报告从其他农场主那里得到帮助的次数。尽管美国西部的牧场文化往往以个人主义和独立的伦理为特征,但我们的研究表明,这种伦理在需要时与互助的伦理并存。
{"title":"\"A Solidarity-Type World\": Need-Based Helping among Ranchers in the Southwestern United States.","authors":"Lee Cronk,&nbsp;Diego Guevara Beltrán,&nbsp;Denise Laya Mercado,&nbsp;Athena Aktipis","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09406-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09406-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To better understand risk management and mutual aid among American ranchers, we interviewed and mailed a survey to ranchers in Hidalgo County, New Mexico, and Cochise County, Arizona, focusing on two questions: (1) When do ranchers expect repayment for the help they provide others? (2) What determines ranchers' degrees of involvement in networks of mutual aid, which they refer to as \"neighboring\"? When needs arise due to unpredictable events, such as injuries, most ranchers reported not expecting to be paid back for the help they provide. When help is provided for something that follows a known schedule or that can be scheduled, such as branding, most ranchers did expect something in return for the help they provide. This pattern makes sense in light of computational modeling that shows that transfers to those in need without expectations of repayment pool risk more effectively than transfers that create debt. Ranchers reported helping other ranchers more often when they belonged to more religious and civic organizations, when they owned larger ranches, when they relied less on ranch vs. other income, and when they had more relatives in the area. Operators of midsize ranches reported helping other ranchers more frequently than did those on smaller and larger ranches. None of our independent variables predicted how many times ranchers reported receiving help from other ranchers. Although ranch culture in the American West is often characterized by an ethic of individualism and independence, our study suggests that this ethic stands alongside an ethic of mutual aid during times of need.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 2","pages":"482-508"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-021-09406-8","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39167753","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Do Local Sex Ratios Approximate Subjective Partner Markets? : Evidence from the German Family Panel. 本地性别比是否接近主观伴侣市场?:来自德国家庭小组的证据。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-06-01 Epub Date: 2021-06-19 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09397-6
Andreas Filser, Richard Preetz

Sex ratios have widely been recognized as an important link between demographic contexts and behavior because changes in the ratio shift sex-specific bargaining power in the partner market. Implicitly, the literature considers individual partner market experiences to be a function of local sex ratios. However, empirical evidence on the correspondence between subjective partner availability and local sex ratios is lacking so far. In this paper, we analyzed how closely a set of different local sex ratio measures correlates with subjective partner market experiences. Linking a longitudinal German survey to population data for different entities (states, counties, municipalities), we used multilevel logistic regression models to explore associations between singles' subjective partner market experiences and various operationalizations of local sex ratios. Results suggest that local sex ratios correlated only weakly with subjective partner market experiences. Adult sex ratios based on broad age brackets, including those for lower-level entities, did not significantly predict whether individuals predominantly met individuals of their own sex. More fine-grained, age-specific sex ratios prove to be better predictors of subjective partner market experiences, in particular when age hypergamy patterns were incorporated. Nevertheless, the respective associations were only significant for selected measures. In a complementary analysis, we illustrate the validity of the subjective indicator as a predictor of relationship formation. In sum, our results suggest that subjective partner availability is not adequately represented by the broad adult sex ratio measures that are frequently used in the literature. Future research should be careful not to equate local sex ratios and conscious partner market experiences.

性别比例被广泛认为是人口背景和行为之间的重要联系,因为性别比例的变化会改变伴侣市场中特定性别的议价能力。隐含地,文献认为个体伴侣市场经验是当地性别比例的函数。然而,关于主观伴侣可获得性与当地性别比例之间的对应关系的经验证据迄今尚缺乏。在本文中,我们分析了一组不同的地方性别比例测量与主观伴侣市场经验的密切关系。将德国的纵向调查与不同实体(州、县、市)的人口数据联系起来,我们使用多层次逻辑回归模型来探索单身人士的主观伴侣市场经验与当地性别比例的各种操作之间的关系。结果表明,当地性别比例与主观伴侣市场经验的相关性较弱。基于广泛年龄组的成人性别比率,包括较低层次实体的成人性别比率,并不能显著预测个体是否主要与自己性别的个体相遇。更细粒度的、特定年龄的性别比例被证明是主观伴侣市场经验的更好预测指标,特别是当年龄多配偶模式被纳入其中时。然而,各自的关联仅在选定的措施中具有显著性。在补充分析中,我们说明了主观指标作为关系形成预测器的有效性。总之,我们的研究结果表明,在文献中经常使用的广泛的成人性别比测量方法并不能充分代表主观的伴侣可获得性。未来的研究应该小心,不要把当地的性别比例和有意识的伴侣市场经验等同起来。
{"title":"Do Local Sex Ratios Approximate Subjective Partner Markets? : Evidence from the German Family Panel.","authors":"Andreas Filser,&nbsp;Richard Preetz","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09397-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09397-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sex ratios have widely been recognized as an important link between demographic contexts and behavior because changes in the ratio shift sex-specific bargaining power in the partner market. Implicitly, the literature considers individual partner market experiences to be a function of local sex ratios. However, empirical evidence on the correspondence between subjective partner availability and local sex ratios is lacking so far. In this paper, we analyzed how closely a set of different local sex ratio measures correlates with subjective partner market experiences. Linking a longitudinal German survey to population data for different entities (states, counties, municipalities), we used multilevel logistic regression models to explore associations between singles' subjective partner market experiences and various operationalizations of local sex ratios. Results suggest that local sex ratios correlated only weakly with subjective partner market experiences. Adult sex ratios based on broad age brackets, including those for lower-level entities, did not significantly predict whether individuals predominantly met individuals of their own sex. More fine-grained, age-specific sex ratios prove to be better predictors of subjective partner market experiences, in particular when age hypergamy patterns were incorporated. Nevertheless, the respective associations were only significant for selected measures. In a complementary analysis, we illustrate the validity of the subjective indicator as a predictor of relationship formation. In sum, our results suggest that subjective partner availability is not adequately represented by the broad adult sex ratio measures that are frequently used in the literature. Future research should be careful not to equate local sex ratios and conscious partner market experiences.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 2","pages":"406-433"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-021-09397-6","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39247391","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Correction to: Eyes, More Than Other Facial Features, Enhance Real-World Donation Behavior. 更正:眼睛比其他面部特征更能增强现实世界的捐赠行为。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-018-9331-5
Caroline Kelsey, Amrisha Vaish, Tobias Grossmann
{"title":"Correction to: Eyes, More Than Other Facial Features, Enhance Real-World Donation Behavior.","authors":"Caroline Kelsey,&nbsp;Amrisha Vaish,&nbsp;Tobias Grossmann","doi":"10.1007/s12110-018-9331-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-018-9331-5","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 2","pages":"528"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-018-9331-5","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"36675275","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Ethnic Markers and How to Find Them : An Ethnographic Investigation of Marker Presence, Recognition, and Social Information. 民族标记和如何找到它们:关于标记存在、识别和社会信息的人种学调查》(Ethnic Markers and How to Find Them : An Ethnographic Investigation of Marker Presence, Recognition, and Social Information)。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-06-01 Epub Date: 2021-06-09 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09401-z
Adrian Viliami Bell, Alina Paegle

Ethnic markers are a prominent organizing feature of human society when individuals engage in significant anonymous interactions. However, identifying markers in natural settings is nontrivial. Although ad hoc assignment of markers to groups is widely documented in the ethnographic literature, predicting the membership of individuals based on stylistic variation is less clear. We argue that a more systematic approach is required to satisfy the basic assumptions made in ethnic marker theory. To this end we introduce a three-step ethnographic method to assess the presence, recognition, and transmission of markers of group identity: (1) continual scans, (2) a utilization survey, and (3) a comparative classification task. Applying the method to a study of culturally significant motifs in the South Pacific Island nation of Tonga, we provide evidence that the motif set satisfies basic theoretical assumptions and thus the motifs are likely expressions for social coordination. We also found that the coordinating role of each motif is variable and requires further investigation.

当个体进行重要的匿名互动时,种族标记是人类社会的一个突出组织特征。然而,在自然环境中识别标记并非易事。虽然人种学文献中广泛记载了将标记物临时分配到群体中的情况,但根据风格差异预测个体的成员资格却不那么清晰。我们认为,需要一种更系统的方法来满足种族标记理论的基本假设。为此,我们介绍了一种分三步进行的人种学方法来评估群体身份标记的存在、识别和传播:(1) 持续扫描,(2) 使用情况调查,(3) 比较分类任务。通过对南太平洋岛国汤加具有重要文化意义的图案进行研究,我们发现图案集符合基本理论假设,因此这些图案很可能是社会协调的表现形式。我们还发现,每个图案的协调作用各不相同,需要进一步研究。
{"title":"Ethnic Markers and How to Find Them : An Ethnographic Investigation of Marker Presence, Recognition, and Social Information.","authors":"Adrian Viliami Bell, Alina Paegle","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09401-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s12110-021-09401-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Ethnic markers are a prominent organizing feature of human society when individuals engage in significant anonymous interactions. However, identifying markers in natural settings is nontrivial. Although ad hoc assignment of markers to groups is widely documented in the ethnographic literature, predicting the membership of individuals based on stylistic variation is less clear. We argue that a more systematic approach is required to satisfy the basic assumptions made in ethnic marker theory. To this end we introduce a three-step ethnographic method to assess the presence, recognition, and transmission of markers of group identity: (1) continual scans, (2) a utilization survey, and (3) a comparative classification task. Applying the method to a study of culturally significant motifs in the South Pacific Island nation of Tonga, we provide evidence that the motif set satisfies basic theoretical assumptions and thus the motifs are likely expressions for social coordination. We also found that the coordinating role of each motif is variable and requires further investigation.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 2","pages":"470-481"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8186961/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39093450","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parental Investment Is Biased toward Children Named for Their Fathers. 父母投资倾向于以父亲的名字命名的孩子。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-06-01 Epub Date: 2021-07-06 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09396-7
Gabriel Šaffa, Zuzana Štěrbová, Pavol Prokop

Namesaking (naming a child after a parent or other relative) can be viewed as a mechanism to increase perceived parent-child similarity and, consequently, parental investment. Male and, to a lesser extent, firstborn children are more frequently namesakes than female and later-born children, respectively. However, a direct link between namesaking and parental investment has not been examined. In the present study, 632 participants (98 men and 534 women) from Central Europe indicated their first name, sex, birth order, number of siblings, sexual orientation, socioeconomic status, paternal and maternal first names, as well as relationship quality with, and time and financial investment they received from, both parents during childhood. Mixed-effects models revealed associations between namesaking and parental investment. However, the effect of namesaking often appeared significant only in interaction with specific predictors, such as sex and primogeniture. It suggests instead that namesaking has an additive effect-it enhances the effect of biological factors on parental investment. In general, we found evidence for the bias in parental investment linked to name similarity among both parents, and support for the hypothesis that namesaking serves as a mechanism to increase paternity confidence and, thus, paternal investment. The effect of namesaking influences only certain types of parental investment-namely, those at the level of relationship quality. In addition, nonheterosexual orientation was the strongest negative predictor of paternal investment. Our study extends the research on parental investment by showing that cultural mechanisms, such as namesaking, can also exert some influence on parental rearing behavior.

命名(以父母或其他亲属的名字命名孩子)可以被视为一种增加感知亲子相似性的机制,从而增加亲代投资。男孩和长子(在较小程度上)比女孩和晚出生的孩子更容易同名。然而,命名与亲代投资之间的直接联系尚未得到研究。在本研究中,来自中欧的632名参与者(98名男性和534名女性)表明了他们的名字、性别、出生顺序、兄弟姐妹数量、性取向、社会经济地位、父母和母亲的名字,以及童年时期与父母双方的关系质量、从父母那里获得的时间和经济投资。混合效应模型揭示了同名与亲代投资之间的联系。然而,同名的影响往往只在与特定的预测因素(如性别和长子继承)相互作用时才显得显著。相反,它表明同名具有加性效应——它增强了生物因素对亲代投资的影响。总的来说,我们发现了证据,证明父母之间的名字相似性与亲代投资的偏见有关,并支持了同名作为一种增加父亲信心的机制,从而增加父亲投资的假设。同名效应只影响某些类型的父母投资,即关系质量水平的投资。此外,非异性恋取向是最强的负向预测因子。本研究在亲代投资研究的基础上,进一步揭示了同名等文化机制对亲代养育行为的影响。
{"title":"Parental Investment Is Biased toward Children Named for Their Fathers.","authors":"Gabriel Šaffa,&nbsp;Zuzana Štěrbová,&nbsp;Pavol Prokop","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09396-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09396-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Namesaking (naming a child after a parent or other relative) can be viewed as a mechanism to increase perceived parent-child similarity and, consequently, parental investment. Male and, to a lesser extent, firstborn children are more frequently namesakes than female and later-born children, respectively. However, a direct link between namesaking and parental investment has not been examined. In the present study, 632 participants (98 men and 534 women) from Central Europe indicated their first name, sex, birth order, number of siblings, sexual orientation, socioeconomic status, paternal and maternal first names, as well as relationship quality with, and time and financial investment they received from, both parents during childhood. Mixed-effects models revealed associations between namesaking and parental investment. However, the effect of namesaking often appeared significant only in interaction with specific predictors, such as sex and primogeniture. It suggests instead that namesaking has an additive effect-it enhances the effect of biological factors on parental investment. In general, we found evidence for the bias in parental investment linked to name similarity among both parents, and support for the hypothesis that namesaking serves as a mechanism to increase paternity confidence and, thus, paternal investment. The effect of namesaking influences only certain types of parental investment-namely, those at the level of relationship quality. In addition, nonheterosexual orientation was the strongest negative predictor of paternal investment. Our study extends the research on parental investment by showing that cultural mechanisms, such as namesaking, can also exert some influence on parental rearing behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 2","pages":"387-405"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-021-09396-7","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39154884","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Learning Through Shared Care : Allomaternal Care Impacts Cognitive Development in Early Infancy in a Western Population. 通过共同照顾学习:异母照顾对西方人群早期婴儿认知发展的影响。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-06-01 Epub Date: 2021-05-10 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09395-8
Britt Singletary

This study investigates how allomaternal care (AMC) impacts human development outside of energetics by evaluating relations between important qualitative and quantitative aspects of AMC and developmental outcomes in a Western population. This study seeks to determine whether there are measurable differences in cognitive and language outcomes as predicted by differences in exposure to AMC via formal (e.g., childcare facilities) and informal (e.g., family and friends) networks. Data were collected from 102 mothers and their typically developing infants aged 13-18 months. AMC predictor data were collected using questionnaires, structured daily diaries, and longitudinal interviews. Developmental outcomes were assessed using the Cognitive, Receptive Language, and Expressive Language subtests of the Bayley III Screening Test. Additional demographic covariates were also evaluated. Akaike Information Criterion (AIC)-informed model selection was used to identify the best-fitting model for each outcome across three working linear regression models. Although AMC variables had no significant effects on Receptive and Expressive Language subtest scores, highly involved familial AMC had a significant medium effect on Cognitive subtest score (β = 0.23, p < 0.01, semi-partial r = 0.28). Formal childcare had no effect on any outcome. This study provides preliminary evidence that there is a measurable connection between AMC and cognitive development in some populations and provides a methodological base from which to assess these connections cross-culturally through future studies. As these effects are attributable to AMC interactions with networks of mostly related individuals, these findings present an area for further investigation regarding the kin selection hypothesis for AMC.

本研究通过评估西方人群中异母护理(AMC)重要的定性和定量方面与发展结果之间的关系,探讨了异母护理(AMC)如何影响人类在能量学之外的发展。本研究旨在确定通过正式(如儿童保育设施)和非正式(如家庭和朋友)网络接触AMC的差异,是否存在可测量的认知和语言结果差异。数据收集自102位母亲和她们13-18个月的正常发育婴儿。AMC预测数据通过问卷调查、结构化的日常日记和纵向访谈收集。使用Bayley III筛选试验的认知、接受性语言和表达性语言子测试评估发育结果。还评估了其他人口统计学协变量。使用赤池信息标准(AIC)知情模型选择来确定三个工作线性回归模型中每个结果的最佳拟合模型。虽然AMC变量对接受性和表达性语言亚测试得分没有显著影响,但高度参与的家族AMC对认知亚测试得分有显著的中等影响(β = 0.23, p
{"title":"Learning Through Shared Care : Allomaternal Care Impacts Cognitive Development in Early Infancy in a Western Population.","authors":"Britt Singletary","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09395-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09395-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study investigates how allomaternal care (AMC) impacts human development outside of energetics by evaluating relations between important qualitative and quantitative aspects of AMC and developmental outcomes in a Western population. This study seeks to determine whether there are measurable differences in cognitive and language outcomes as predicted by differences in exposure to AMC via formal (e.g., childcare facilities) and informal (e.g., family and friends) networks. Data were collected from 102 mothers and their typically developing infants aged 13-18 months. AMC predictor data were collected using questionnaires, structured daily diaries, and longitudinal interviews. Developmental outcomes were assessed using the Cognitive, Receptive Language, and Expressive Language subtests of the Bayley III Screening Test. Additional demographic covariates were also evaluated. Akaike Information Criterion (AIC)-informed model selection was used to identify the best-fitting model for each outcome across three working linear regression models. Although AMC variables had no significant effects on Receptive and Expressive Language subtest scores, highly involved familial AMC had a significant medium effect on Cognitive subtest score (β = 0.23, p < 0.01, semi-partial r = 0.28). Formal childcare had no effect on any outcome. This study provides preliminary evidence that there is a measurable connection between AMC and cognitive development in some populations and provides a methodological base from which to assess these connections cross-culturally through future studies. As these effects are attributable to AMC interactions with networks of mostly related individuals, these findings present an area for further investigation regarding the kin selection hypothesis for AMC.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 2","pages":"326-362"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-021-09395-8","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38967130","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Women's Reaction to Opposite- and Same-Sex Infidelity in Three Cultures : Data from Canada, Samoa, and the Istmo Zapotec. 三种文化中女性对异性和同性不忠的反应:来自加拿大、萨摩亚和萨摩亚群岛的数据。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-06-01 Epub Date: 2021-06-30 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09405-9
Scott W Semenyna, Francisco R Gómez Jiménez, Paul L Vasey

Previous research indicates that Euro-American women are more upset by imagining their male partners committing homosexual infidelities than heterosexual ones. The present studies sought to replicate these findings and extend them to two non-Western cultures wherein masculine men frequently engage in sexual interactions with feminine third-gender males. Across six studies in three cultural locales (Canada, Samoa, and the Istmo Zapotec), women were asked to rate their degree of upset when imagining that their partner committed infidelity that was heterosexual in nature, as well as infidelity that was homosexual. In two Canadian undergraduate samples, women reported greater upset at imagining partner infidelity with a female, whereas a community sample of middle-aged women reported equal upset across infidelity types. Samoan women reported substantially less upset at the thought of partner infidelity with a third-gender male (fa'afafine) than with a female. Istmo Zapotec women reported equal upset toward infidelity with a female or a third-gender male (muxe), whereas a second Zapotec sample reported slightly greater upset at the thought of infidelity with a muxe. Results illustrate how cultural contexts moderate the degree to which same-sex infidelity scenarios are upsetting to women.

先前的研究表明,欧美女性想象自己的男性伴侣对同性不忠时比想象异性恋伴侣时更不安。目前的研究试图复制这些发现,并将其扩展到两个非西方文化中,在这些文化中,男性化的男性经常与女性化的第三性男性发生性互动。在三个文化地区(加拿大、萨摩亚和伊斯特莫萨波特克)进行的六项研究中,研究人员要求女性在想象自己的伴侣对异性恋和同性恋不忠时,对自己的不安程度进行评分。在两个加拿大大学生样本中,女性在想象伴侣对女性不忠时表现出更大的不安,而一个社区中年女性样本在各种不忠类型中表现出同样的不安。萨摩亚女性报告说,与伴侣对女性不忠相比,伴侣对第三性别男性(fa'afafine)不忠要少得多。萨波特克地区的女性对与女性或第三性别男性(muxe)不忠的不满程度是一样的,而另一个萨波特克地区的女性对与muxe不忠的不满程度略高。研究结果说明了文化背景如何缓和同性不忠场景对女性的困扰程度。
{"title":"Women's Reaction to Opposite- and Same-Sex Infidelity in Three Cultures : Data from Canada, Samoa, and the Istmo Zapotec.","authors":"Scott W Semenyna,&nbsp;Francisco R Gómez Jiménez,&nbsp;Paul L Vasey","doi":"10.1007/s12110-021-09405-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09405-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research indicates that Euro-American women are more upset by imagining their male partners committing homosexual infidelities than heterosexual ones. The present studies sought to replicate these findings and extend them to two non-Western cultures wherein masculine men frequently engage in sexual interactions with feminine third-gender males. Across six studies in three cultural locales (Canada, Samoa, and the Istmo Zapotec), women were asked to rate their degree of upset when imagining that their partner committed infidelity that was heterosexual in nature, as well as infidelity that was homosexual. In two Canadian undergraduate samples, women reported greater upset at imagining partner infidelity with a female, whereas a community sample of middle-aged women reported equal upset across infidelity types. Samoan women reported substantially less upset at the thought of partner infidelity with a third-gender male (fa'afafine) than with a female. Istmo Zapotec women reported equal upset toward infidelity with a female or a third-gender male (muxe), whereas a second Zapotec sample reported slightly greater upset at the thought of infidelity with a muxe. Results illustrate how cultural contexts moderate the degree to which same-sex infidelity scenarios are upsetting to women.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":"32 2","pages":"450-469"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12110-021-09405-9","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39123656","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1