首页 > 最新文献

Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective最新文献

英文 中文
Death from Failed Protection? An Evolutionary-Developmental Theory of Sudden Infant Death Syndrome. 保护失败导致死亡?婴儿猝死综合症的进化-发展理论。
IF 2.2 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-29 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-024-09474-6
Herbert Renz-Polster, Peter S Blair, Helen L Ball, Oskar G Jenni, Freia De Bock

Sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) has been mainly described from a risk perspective, with a focus on endogenous, exogenous, and temporal risk factors that can interact to facilitate lethal outcomes. Here we discuss the limitations that this risk-based paradigm may have, using two of the major risk factors for SIDS, prone sleep position and bed-sharing, as examples. Based on a multipronged theoretical model encompassing evolutionary theory, developmental biology, and cultural mismatch theory, we conceptualize the vulnerability to SIDS as an imbalance between current physiologic-regulatory demands and current protective abilities on the part of the infant. From this understanding, SIDS appears as a developmental condition in which competencies relevant to self-protection fail to develop appropriately in the future victims. Since all of the protective resources in question are bound to emerge during normal infant development, we contend that SIDS may reflect an evolutionary mismatch situation-a constellation in which certain modern developmental influences may overextend the child's adaptive (evolutionary) repertoire. We thus argue that SIDS may be better understood if the focus on risk factors is complemented by a deeper appreciation of the protective resources that human infants acquire during their normal development. We extensively analyze this evolutionary-developmental theory against the body of epidemiological and experimental evidence in SIDS research and thereby also address the as-of-yet unresolved question of why breastfeeding may be protective against SIDS.

婴儿猝死综合症(SIDS)主要是从风险的角度进行描述的,其重点是内源性、外源性和时间性风险因素,这些因素相互作用,可能导致致命的结果。在此,我们以婴儿猝死综合症的两个主要风险因素--俯卧睡姿和同床共枕为例,讨论这种基于风险的范式可能存在的局限性。基于包括进化理论、发育生物学和文化错配理论在内的多管齐下的理论模型,我们将婴儿猝死综合症的脆弱性概念化为当前生理调节需求与婴儿当前保护能力之间的不平衡。根据这种理解,婴儿猝死综合症似乎是一种发育状况,在这种状况下,与自我保护相关的能力未能在未来的受害者身上得到适当的发展。由于所有相关的保护资源都必然会在婴儿的正常发育过程中出现,我们认为,婴儿猝死综合症可能反映了一种进化不匹配的情况--在这种情况下,某些现代发育影响因素可能会过度扩展儿童的适应(进化)范围。因此,我们认为,如果在关注风险因素的同时,更深入地了解人类婴儿在正常发育过程中获得的保护性资源,就能更好地理解婴儿猝死综合症。我们对照婴儿猝死综合症研究中的流行病学和实验证据,对这一进化-发展理论进行了广泛分析,从而也解决了母乳喂养为何可预防婴儿猝死综合症这一尚未解决的问题。
{"title":"Death from Failed Protection? An Evolutionary-Developmental Theory of Sudden Infant Death Syndrome.","authors":"Herbert Renz-Polster, Peter S Blair, Helen L Ball, Oskar G Jenni, Freia De Bock","doi":"10.1007/s12110-024-09474-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s12110-024-09474-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) has been mainly described from a risk perspective, with a focus on endogenous, exogenous, and temporal risk factors that can interact to facilitate lethal outcomes. Here we discuss the limitations that this risk-based paradigm may have, using two of the major risk factors for SIDS, prone sleep position and bed-sharing, as examples. Based on a multipronged theoretical model encompassing evolutionary theory, developmental biology, and cultural mismatch theory, we conceptualize the vulnerability to SIDS as an imbalance between current physiologic-regulatory demands and current protective abilities on the part of the infant. From this understanding, SIDS appears as a developmental condition in which competencies relevant to self-protection fail to develop appropriately in the future victims. Since all of the protective resources in question are bound to emerge during normal infant development, we contend that SIDS may reflect an evolutionary mismatch situation-a constellation in which certain modern developmental influences may overextend the child's adaptive (evolutionary) repertoire. We thus argue that SIDS may be better understood if the focus on risk factors is complemented by a deeper appreciation of the protective resources that human infants acquire during their normal development. We extensively analyze this evolutionary-developmental theory against the body of epidemiological and experimental evidence in SIDS research and thereby also address the as-of-yet unresolved question of why breastfeeding may be protective against SIDS.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":" ","pages":"153-196"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11317453/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141789485","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Cultural Evolution of Games of Chance : A Historical Investigation of Chinese Gambling. 机会游戏的文化演变:中国赌博的历史考察》(The Cultural Evolution of Games of Chance : A Historical Investigation of Chinese Gambling.
IF 2.2 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-31 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-024-09471-9
Ze Hong

Chance-based gambling has been a recurrent cultural activity throughout history and across many diverse human societies. In this paper, I combine quantitative and qualitative data and present a cultural evolutionary framework to explain why the odds in games of chance in premodern China appeared "designed" to ensure a moderate yet favorable house advantage. This is especially intriguing since extensive research in the history of probability has shown that, prior to the development of probability theory, people had very limited understanding of the nature of random events and were generally disinclined to think mathematically about the frequency of their occurrence. I argue that games of chance in the context of gambling may have culturally evolved into their documented forms via a process of selective imitation and retention, and neither the customers nor the gambling houses understood the probability calculus involved in these games.

在历史上和许多不同的人类社会中,基于机会的赌博一直是一种经常性的文化活动。在本文中,我结合定量和定性数据,提出了一个文化进化框架,以解释为什么在前现代中国,概率游戏的赔率似乎是 "设计 "出来的,以确保适度但有利的庄家优势。这一点尤其耐人寻味,因为对概率史的广泛研究表明,在概率论发展之前,人们对随机事件本质的理解非常有限,而且普遍不愿意用数学方法来思考随机事件发生的频率。我认为,赌博中的概率游戏可能是通过选择性模仿和保留的过程在文化上演变成其记录形式的,而顾客和赌场都不了解这些游戏所涉及的概率计算。
{"title":"The Cultural Evolution of Games of Chance : A Historical Investigation of Chinese Gambling.","authors":"Ze Hong","doi":"10.1007/s12110-024-09471-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s12110-024-09471-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Chance-based gambling has been a recurrent cultural activity throughout history and across many diverse human societies. In this paper, I combine quantitative and qualitative data and present a cultural evolutionary framework to explain why the odds in games of chance in premodern China appeared \"designed\" to ensure a moderate yet favorable house advantage. This is especially intriguing since extensive research in the history of probability has shown that, prior to the development of probability theory, people had very limited understanding of the nature of random events and were generally disinclined to think mathematically about the frequency of their occurrence. I argue that games of chance in the context of gambling may have culturally evolved into their documented forms via a process of selective imitation and retention, and neither the customers nor the gambling houses understood the probability calculus involved in these games.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":" ","pages":"89-113"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11317449/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141181156","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Historical Mortality Dynamics on the Baja California Peninsula : Marriage, Mining Booms, Migration, and Infectious Disease. 下加利福尼亚半岛的历史死亡率动态 :婚姻、采矿业繁荣、移民和传染病。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-14 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-024-09469-3
Shane J Macfarlan, Ryan Schacht, Isabelle Forrest, Abigail Swanson, Cynthia Moses, Thomas McNulty, Katelyn Cowley, Celeste Henrickson

Historical demographic research shows that the factors influencing mortality risk are labile across time and space. This is particularly true for datasets that span societal transitions. Here, we seek to understand how marriage, migration, and the local economy influenced mortality dynamics in a rapidly changing environment characterized by high in-migration and male-biased sex ratios. Mortality records were extracted from a compendium of historical vital records for the Baja California peninsula (Mexico). Our sample consists of 1,201 mortality records spanning AD 1835-1900. Findings from Cox proportional hazard models indicate that (1) marriage was associated with a protective effect for both sexes; (2) residing in a mining town was associated with higher mortality for men, but not women; (3) migration was associated with decreased mortality risk for women, but not men; and (4) the risk of mortality increased in the face of infectious disease, but decreased over time. Despite the early initiation of reproduction for women, marriage had a protective effect, likely because marriage linked women to resources. Although mining boomtowns were associated with elevated risk factors generally, only men experienced greater mortality risk, likely due to dangerous working conditions that women did not experience. Last, female, but not male, migrants experienced greater longevity, possibly because exposure to harsh labor conditions eroded the protective effect of selection bias for men. Together, these results shed light on an understudied historical population and broaden our understanding of demographic dynamics in preindustrial settings.

历史人口研究表明,影响死亡风险的因素在时间和空间上是不稳定的。对于跨越社会转型的数据集来说尤其如此。在这里,我们试图了解婚姻、移民和当地经济是如何影响以高移民率和男性偏多的性别比为特征的快速变化环境中的死亡率动态的。我们从下加利福尼亚半岛(墨西哥)的历史生命记录汇编中提取了死亡率记录。我们的样本包括 1,201 份死亡记录,时间跨度为公元 1835-1900 年。Cox 比例危险模型的研究结果表明:(1) 婚姻对两性都有保护作用;(2) 居住在矿业城镇与男性死亡率升高有关,但与女性无关;(3) 迁徙与女性死亡率降低有关,但与男性无关;(4) 面对传染病时,死亡风险会升高,但随着时间的推移会降低。尽管妇女开始生育的时间较早,但婚姻具有保护作用,这可能是因为婚姻将妇女与资源联系在一起。虽然矿业繁荣城镇普遍存在风险因素升高的问题,但只有男性面临更大的死亡风险,这可能是由于女性没有经历过的危险工作条件造成的。最后,女性移民寿命更长,而男性移民寿命较短,这可能是因为恶劣的劳动条件削弱了选择偏差对男性的保护作用。总之,这些结果揭示了一个未被充分研究的历史人群,并拓宽了我们对工业化前环境中人口动态的理解。
{"title":"Historical Mortality Dynamics on the Baja California Peninsula : Marriage, Mining Booms, Migration, and Infectious Disease.","authors":"Shane J Macfarlan, Ryan Schacht, Isabelle Forrest, Abigail Swanson, Cynthia Moses, Thomas McNulty, Katelyn Cowley, Celeste Henrickson","doi":"10.1007/s12110-024-09469-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s12110-024-09469-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Historical demographic research shows that the factors influencing mortality risk are labile across time and space. This is particularly true for datasets that span societal transitions. Here, we seek to understand how marriage, migration, and the local economy influenced mortality dynamics in a rapidly changing environment characterized by high in-migration and male-biased sex ratios. Mortality records were extracted from a compendium of historical vital records for the Baja California peninsula (Mexico). Our sample consists of 1,201 mortality records spanning AD 1835-1900. Findings from Cox proportional hazard models indicate that (1) marriage was associated with a protective effect for both sexes; (2) residing in a mining town was associated with higher mortality for men, but not women; (3) migration was associated with decreased mortality risk for women, but not men; and (4) the risk of mortality increased in the face of infectious disease, but decreased over time. Despite the early initiation of reproduction for women, marriage had a protective effect, likely because marriage linked women to resources. Although mining boomtowns were associated with elevated risk factors generally, only men experienced greater mortality risk, likely due to dangerous working conditions that women did not experience. Last, female, but not male, migrants experienced greater longevity, possibly because exposure to harsh labor conditions eroded the protective effect of selection bias for men. Together, these results shed light on an understudied historical population and broaden our understanding of demographic dynamics in preindustrial settings.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":" ","pages":"1-20"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140121037","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Alloparental Support and Infant Psychomotor Developmental Delay. 全职父母的支持与婴儿心理运动发育迟缓。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-14 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-024-09468-4
David Waynforth

Receiving social support from community and extended family has been typical for mothers with infants in human societies past and present. In non-industrialised contexts, infants of mothers with extended family support often have better health and higher survival through the vulnerable infant period, and hence shared infant care has a clear fitness benefit. However, there is scant evidence that these benefits continue in industrialised contexts. Better infant health and development with allocare support would indicate continued evolutionary selection for allocare. The research reported here used multiple logistic regression analysis to test whether a lack of family and other social support for mothers was associated with an increased risk of developmental delay in 9-month-old infants in the UK Millennium Cohort (analysis sample size, 15,696 infants). Extended family-based childcare during work hours and more maternal time spent with friends were the most influential kin and social support variables: infants of mothers with kin-based childcare versus all other childcare arrangements had a lower risk of developmental delay (OR = 0.61, 95% CIs: 0.46-0.82). Infants of mothers who spent no time with friends when compared with those who saw friends every day had double the odds of delay. Greater paternal involvement in infant care was associated with a lower odds of developmental delay. In conclusion, shared care of infants and social support for mothers may influence fitness-related traits in industrialised societies rather than being factors that influenced selection only in the past and in societies which retain close kin networks and a strong local community focus.

在过去和现在的人类社会中,有婴儿的母亲通常会得到社区和大家庭的社会支持。在非工业化背景下,有大家庭支持的母亲所生的婴儿通常健康状况更好,在脆弱的婴儿期存活率更高,因此共同照顾婴儿对健康有明显的好处。然而,很少有证据表明,这些好处在工业化环境中依然存在。如果婴儿的健康和发育在共同照料的支持下得到改善,则表明共同照料在进化过程中会继续得到选择。本文报告的研究使用多重逻辑回归分析,检验了在英国千禧队列(分析样本量为 15,696 名婴儿)中,母亲缺乏家庭和其他社会支持是否与 9 个月大婴儿发育迟缓的风险增加有关。在工作时间由家庭延伸照顾孩子以及母亲有更多时间与朋友在一起是最有影响力的亲属和社会支持变量:由亲属照顾孩子的母亲与所有其他照顾孩子安排的母亲相比,婴儿发育迟缓的风险较低(OR = 0.61,95% CIs:0.46-0.82)。与每天都与朋友见面的母亲相比,没有时间与朋友相处的母亲的婴儿发育迟缓几率要高出一倍。父亲更多地参与婴儿护理与较低的发育迟缓几率有关。总之,在工业化社会中,对婴儿的共同照顾和对母亲的社会支持可能会影响与体格有关的特征,而不是在过去以及在保留密切亲属网络和以当地社区为中心的社会中才会影响选择的因素。
{"title":"Alloparental Support and Infant Psychomotor Developmental Delay.","authors":"David Waynforth","doi":"10.1007/s12110-024-09468-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s12110-024-09468-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Receiving social support from community and extended family has been typical for mothers with infants in human societies past and present. In non-industrialised contexts, infants of mothers with extended family support often have better health and higher survival through the vulnerable infant period, and hence shared infant care has a clear fitness benefit. However, there is scant evidence that these benefits continue in industrialised contexts. Better infant health and development with allocare support would indicate continued evolutionary selection for allocare. The research reported here used multiple logistic regression analysis to test whether a lack of family and other social support for mothers was associated with an increased risk of developmental delay in 9-month-old infants in the UK Millennium Cohort (analysis sample size, 15,696 infants). Extended family-based childcare during work hours and more maternal time spent with friends were the most influential kin and social support variables: infants of mothers with kin-based childcare versus all other childcare arrangements had a lower risk of developmental delay (OR = 0.61, 95% CIs: 0.46-0.82). Infants of mothers who spent no time with friends when compared with those who saw friends every day had double the odds of delay. Greater paternal involvement in infant care was associated with a lower odds of developmental delay. In conclusion, shared care of infants and social support for mothers may influence fitness-related traits in industrialised societies rather than being factors that influenced selection only in the past and in societies which retain close kin networks and a strong local community focus.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":" ","pages":"43-62"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11052766/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139730737","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Agential View of Misfortune. 从行动角度看不幸。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-20 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-024-09470-w
Ronald J Planer, Kim Sterelny

In many traditional, small-scale societies, death and other misfortunes are commonly explained as a result of others' malign occult agency. Here, we call this family of epistemic tendencies "the agential view of misfortune." After reviewing several ethnographic case studies that illustrate this view, we argue that its origins and stability are puzzling from an evolutionary perspective. Not only is the agential view of misfortune false; it imposes costs on individuals and social groups that seem to far outweigh whatever benefits the view might provide. We thus doubt that the agential view of misfortune is explainable in terms of adaptive effects. However, neither does it seem readily explainable as a consequence of belief formation strategies that are on the whole adaptive (as is plausibly the case for certain other of our false beliefs, including some that are costly). Accordingly, we contend that the commonness of the agential view of misfortune demands a special evolutionary explanation of some kind. We provide a partial explanation of this phenomenon by highlighting the adaptive benefits that often flow to occult specialists in environments where the agential view of misfortune is entrenched. What this does not explain, however, is the general lack of resistance we observe in response to occultists' exploitative behaviours over (cultural) evolutionary timescales. We conclude by canvassing a few possible explanations for this puzzling lack of resistance, and while we commit ourselves to none, we do find one option more promising than the others.

在许多传统的小规模社会中,死亡和其他不幸通常被解释为他人的恶性神秘作用的结果。在此,我们将这种认识论倾向称为 "代理的不幸观"。在回顾了几个说明这种观点的人种学案例研究之后,我们认为,从进化论的角度来看,这种观点的起源和稳定性令人费解。代理不幸观不仅是错误的,而且它给个人和社会群体带来的代价似乎远远超过了这种观点可能带来的益处。因此,我们怀疑是否可以用适应性效应来解释代理不幸观。然而,它似乎也不能被解释为整体上具有适应性的信念形成策略的结果(就像我们的某些其他错误信念,包括一些代价高昂的信念一样)。因此,我们认为,对不幸的代理观点的普遍性需要某种特殊的进化解释。我们强调了在代理不幸观根深蒂固的环境中,神秘学专家往往能获得适应性利益,从而为这一现象提供了部分解释。然而,这并不能解释我们在(文化)进化时间尺度上观察到的对神秘学家的剥削行为普遍缺乏抵制的现象。最后,我们对这种令人费解的缺乏抵抗的现象提出了几种可能的解释,虽然我们对任何一种解释都不认同,但我们确实发现有一种解释比其他解释更有希望。
{"title":"The Agential View of Misfortune.","authors":"Ronald J Planer, Kim Sterelny","doi":"10.1007/s12110-024-09470-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s12110-024-09470-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In many traditional, small-scale societies, death and other misfortunes are commonly explained as a result of others' malign occult agency. Here, we call this family of epistemic tendencies \"the agential view of misfortune.\" After reviewing several ethnographic case studies that illustrate this view, we argue that its origins and stability are puzzling from an evolutionary perspective. Not only is the agential view of misfortune false; it imposes costs on individuals and social groups that seem to far outweigh whatever benefits the view might provide. We thus doubt that the agential view of misfortune is explainable in terms of adaptive effects. However, neither does it seem readily explainable as a consequence of belief formation strategies that are on the whole adaptive (as is plausibly the case for certain other of our false beliefs, including some that are costly). Accordingly, we contend that the commonness of the agential view of misfortune demands a special evolutionary explanation of some kind. We provide a partial explanation of this phenomenon by highlighting the adaptive benefits that often flow to occult specialists in environments where the agential view of misfortune is entrenched. What this does not explain, however, is the general lack of resistance we observe in response to occultists' exploitative behaviours over (cultural) evolutionary timescales. We conclude by canvassing a few possible explanations for this puzzling lack of resistance, and while we commit ourselves to none, we do find one option more promising than the others.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":" ","pages":"63-88"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11052817/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177150","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Discriminative Grandparental Investment in China : Evidence from an Undergraduate Questionnaire Study. 中国祖父母的歧视性投资:来自大学生问卷调查的证据。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-16 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-024-09467-5
Liqun Luo, Yinan Zuo, Xinzhu Xiong

Many studies in Western societies show a pattern of discriminative grandparental investment as follows: maternal grandmothers (MGMs) > maternal grandfathers (MGFs) > paternal grandmothers (PGMs) > paternal grandfathers (PGFs). This pattern is in line with the expectation from evolutionary reasoning. Yet whether or not this pattern applies in China is in question. The present study was based on a questionnaire survey at a university in Central China (N = 1,195). Results show that (1) when grandparent-grandchild residential distance during grandchildren's childhood is controlled, in the case of grandsons and granddaughters as a whole and granddaughters only, both grandparental caregiving and grandchildren's emotional closeness to grandparents display a rank order of MGM > MGF > PGM > PGF, but in the case of grandsons only, this order is not statistically significant. (2) There are stable relationships between grandparental caregiving/grandchildren's emotional closeness and residential distance/similarity in appearance. (3) The effects of residential distance on either PGFs' or PGMs' caregiving exceed those on either MGFs' or MGMs'. (4) The PGF and PGM prefer grandsons to granddaughters in their caregiving, whereas the MGF and MGM do not have a sex preference, and (5) the fact that the PGF and PGM invest more in grandsons than in granddaughters does not depend on grandsons' duration of living in a rural area. Our results suggest that (1) in general, the Chinese display a pattern of differential grandparental investment predicted by an evolutionary perspective, (2) the evolutionary perspective that combines the two factors of paternal uncertainty and sex-specific reproductive strategies is applicable to grandparental investment in China, and (3) the traditional son-preference culture also plays some role in affecting grandparental investment in China, though the roles of culture and urban-rural cultural difference should not be exaggerated.

西方社会的许多研究表明,祖父母的歧视性投资模式如下:母系祖母(MGMs)>母系祖父(MGFs)>父系祖母(PGMs)>父系祖父(PGFs)。这种模式符合进化论的预期。然而,这一模式是否适用于中国却是个问题。本研究基于对华中某大学的问卷调查(N = 1,195)。结果表明:(1) 在控制孙子女童年时期祖孙居住距离的情况下,无论是孙子女还是孙女,祖辈照料和孙子女对祖辈的情感亲密程度都呈现出 MGM > MGF > PGM > PGF 的排序,但在孙子女的情况下,这一排序在统计学上并不显著。(2) 祖父母对孙子女的照顾/孙子女的情感亲密程度与居住距离/外貌相似度之间存在稳定的关系。(3) 居住距离对 PGFs 或 PGMs 照料的影响超过对 MGFs 或 MGMs 的影响。(4) 与孙女相比,PGF 和 PGM 在照顾孙子方面更偏爱孙女,而 MGF 和 MGM 没有性别偏好;(5) PGF 和 PGM 对孙子的投资多于对孙女的投资,这与孙子在农村生活的时间长短无关。我们的研究结果表明:(1)总体而言,中国人表现出进化论视角所预测的祖辈投资差异模式;(2)结合父系不确定性和性别特异性生殖策略两个因素的进化论视角适用于中国的祖辈投资;(3)传统的重男轻女文化也对中国的祖辈投资产生了一定的影响,但不应夸大文化和城乡文化差异的作用。
{"title":"Discriminative Grandparental Investment in China : Evidence from an Undergraduate Questionnaire Study.","authors":"Liqun Luo, Yinan Zuo, Xinzhu Xiong","doi":"10.1007/s12110-024-09467-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s12110-024-09467-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many studies in Western societies show a pattern of discriminative grandparental investment as follows: maternal grandmothers (MGMs) > maternal grandfathers (MGFs) > paternal grandmothers (PGMs) > paternal grandfathers (PGFs). This pattern is in line with the expectation from evolutionary reasoning. Yet whether or not this pattern applies in China is in question. The present study was based on a questionnaire survey at a university in Central China (N = 1,195). Results show that (1) when grandparent-grandchild residential distance during grandchildren's childhood is controlled, in the case of grandsons and granddaughters as a whole and granddaughters only, both grandparental caregiving and grandchildren's emotional closeness to grandparents display a rank order of MGM > MGF > PGM > PGF, but in the case of grandsons only, this order is not statistically significant. (2) There are stable relationships between grandparental caregiving/grandchildren's emotional closeness and residential distance/similarity in appearance. (3) The effects of residential distance on either PGFs' or PGMs' caregiving exceed those on either MGFs' or MGMs'. (4) The PGF and PGM prefer grandsons to granddaughters in their caregiving, whereas the MGF and MGM do not have a sex preference, and (5) the fact that the PGF and PGM invest more in grandsons than in granddaughters does not depend on grandsons' duration of living in a rural area. Our results suggest that (1) in general, the Chinese display a pattern of differential grandparental investment predicted by an evolutionary perspective, (2) the evolutionary perspective that combines the two factors of paternal uncertainty and sex-specific reproductive strategies is applicable to grandparental investment in China, and (3) the traditional son-preference culture also plays some role in affecting grandparental investment in China, though the roles of culture and urban-rural cultural difference should not be exaggerated.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":" ","pages":"21-42"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139742348","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptions of Income Inequality and Women's Intrasexual Competition. 对收入不平等和女性性内竞争的看法。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-20 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-023-09466-y
Abby M Ruder, Gary L Brase, Nora J Balboa, Jordann L Brandner, Sydni A J Basha

Income inequality has been empirically linked to interpersonal competition and risk-taking behaviors, but a separate line of findings consistently shows that individuals have inaccurate perceptions of the actual levels of income inequality in society. How can inequality be both consistently misperceived and yet a reliable predictor of behavior? The present study extends both these lines of research by evaluating if the scope of input used to assess income inequality (i.e., at the national, state, county, or postal code level) can account for perception discrepancies and if actual/perceived inequality is associated with female intrasexual competition. Female participants recruited online from the general US population (n = 691) provided demographic information, measures of perceived income inequality, and measures of intrasexual competition attitudes and behavior. Actual and perceived income inequality (at any level) did not predict negative attitudes toward other women or female weighting of physical appearance as a desirable trait. Perceived income inequality and actual county-level inequality was, however, predictive of female competition in the form of self-sexualization clothing choice. Further analyses found that age and importance placed on physical attractiveness also predicted women's clothing choices. Perceptions of income inequality were predicted not by actual Gini indices, but by beliefs about the levels of poverty and income gaps. These results highlight the importance of better understanding the proximate cues by which people perceive environmental features such as inequality, and how those cues are used to adjust interpersonal behaviors.

根据经验,收入不平等与人际竞争和冒险行为有关,但另一系列研究结果却一致表明,个人对社会收入不平等的实际水平有着不准确的认知。不平等怎么会既被持续误解,又能成为行为的可靠预测因素呢?本研究通过评估用于评估收入不平等的输入范围(即国家、州、县或邮政编码级别)是否能解释感知差异,以及实际/感知的不平等是否与女性性内竞争相关,对上述两个研究方向进行了扩展。从美国普通人群中在线招募的女性参与者(n = 691)提供了人口统计学信息、感知的收入不平等度量以及性内竞争态度和行为的度量。实际和感知到的收入不平等(任何水平)都不能预测对其他女性的负面态度,也不能预测女性将外貌作为理想特质的权重。然而,感知到的收入不平等和实际的县级不平等却能预测女性在自我性化服装选择方面的竞争。进一步分析发现,年龄和对外貌吸引力的重视程度也能预测女性的服装选择。对收入不平等的看法不是由实际的基尼指数预测的,而是由对贫困程度和收入差距的看法预测的。这些结果凸显了更好地理解人们感知不平等等环境特征的近似线索,以及如何利用这些线索调整人际行为的重要性。
{"title":"Perceptions of Income Inequality and Women's Intrasexual Competition.","authors":"Abby M Ruder, Gary L Brase, Nora J Balboa, Jordann L Brandner, Sydni A J Basha","doi":"10.1007/s12110-023-09466-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s12110-023-09466-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Income inequality has been empirically linked to interpersonal competition and risk-taking behaviors, but a separate line of findings consistently shows that individuals have inaccurate perceptions of the actual levels of income inequality in society. How can inequality be both consistently misperceived and yet a reliable predictor of behavior? The present study extends both these lines of research by evaluating if the scope of input used to assess income inequality (i.e., at the national, state, county, or postal code level) can account for perception discrepancies and if actual/perceived inequality is associated with female intrasexual competition. Female participants recruited online from the general US population (n = 691) provided demographic information, measures of perceived income inequality, and measures of intrasexual competition attitudes and behavior. Actual and perceived income inequality (at any level) did not predict negative attitudes toward other women or female weighting of physical appearance as a desirable trait. Perceived income inequality and actual county-level inequality was, however, predictive of female competition in the form of self-sexualization clothing choice. Further analyses found that age and importance placed on physical attractiveness also predicted women's clothing choices. Perceptions of income inequality were predicted not by actual Gini indices, but by beliefs about the levels of poverty and income gaps. These results highlight the importance of better understanding the proximate cues by which people perceive environmental features such as inequality, and how those cues are used to adjust interpersonal behaviors.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":" ","pages":"605-620"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10947790/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138811058","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Facial Expression of TIPI Personality and CHMP-Tri Psychopathy Traits in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) : Evidence for Honest Signalling? 黑猩猩TIPI人格和CHMP三种精神病特征的面部表达:诚实信号的证据?
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-07 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-023-09462-2
Lindsay Murray, Jade Goddard, David Gordon

Honest signalling theory suggests that humans and chimpanzees can extract socially relevant information relating to personality from the faces of their conspecifics. Humans are also able to extract information from chimpanzees' faces. Here, we examine whether personality characteristics of chimpanzees, including measures of psychopathy, can be discerned based purely on facial morphology in photographs. Twenty-one chimpanzees were given naïve and expert personality ratings on the Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI) and the Chimpanzee Triarchic Model of Psychopathy (CHMP-Tri) before and following behavioural observations. Characteristics relating to openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, and disinhibition could be distinguished from the faces of chimpanzees. Individuals higher on disinhibition have lower scores on conscientiousness and emotional stability and higher scores on extraversion, while those higher on meanness have lower conscientiousness and agreeableness. Facial expressions are linked to personality traits present in the TIPI and CHMP-Tri models: the Relaxed Face and the Grooming Face were displayed more by chimpanzees higher on agreeableness, whereas the Compressed Lips Face was observed more in those individuals higher on boldness, and the Full Open Grin was displayed more by chimpanzees higher on extraversion but lower on emotional stability and conscientiousness. Facial expressions were also found to be associated with particular behavioural contexts, namely the Grooming Face in affiliative contexts and the Relaxed and Relaxed Open Mouth Faces in neutral contexts. Dominant chimpanzees display higher levels of boldness and more Compressed Lips Faces, Relaxed Open Mouth Faces, and Grooming Faces than subordinate individuals. These findings support and extend evidence for an honest signalling system and a personality structure shared between humans and chimpanzees. Future research could further explore how personality is conveyed through the face, perhaps through more than just singular aspects of character, and maybe reflecting what chimpanzees themselves are able to do.

诚实信号理论表明,人类和黑猩猩可以从其同类的脸上提取与个性有关的社会相关信息。人类还能够从黑猩猩的脸上提取信息。在这里,我们研究了黑猩猩的性格特征,包括精神病的衡量标准,是否可以纯粹根据照片中的面部形态来辨别。在行为观察之前和之后,21只黑猩猩在十项人格量表(TIPI)和黑猩猩精神病三联模型(CHMP Tri)中接受了幼稚和专家人格评级。黑猩猩的面孔具有开放、认真、外向和去抑制的特征。去抑制程度越高的个体,其责任感和情绪稳定性得分越低,外向性得分越高,而刻薄程度越高,其责任性和宜人性得分越低。面部表情与TIPI和CHMP三个模型中存在的性格特征有关:放松脸和修饰脸在宜人性较高的黑猩猩身上表现得更多,而在大胆性较高的个体身上观察到的压缩嘴唇脸更多,黑猩猩在外向性方面表现得更高,但在情绪稳定性和责任感方面表现得更低。面部表情也被发现与特定的行为环境有关,即附属环境中的修饰脸和中性环境中的放松和张开嘴巴的脸。与从属个体相比,处于优势地位的黑猩猩表现出更高的勇气和更多的紧缩嘴唇脸、放松张开嘴巴脸和梳毛脸。这些发现支持并扩展了人类和黑猩猩之间共享的诚实信号系统和人格结构的证据。未来的研究可能会进一步探索性格是如何通过面部传达的,也许不仅仅是通过性格的单一方面,也许还反映了黑猩猩自己的能力。
{"title":"Facial Expression of TIPI Personality and CHMP-Tri Psychopathy Traits in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) : Evidence for Honest Signalling?","authors":"Lindsay Murray, Jade Goddard, David Gordon","doi":"10.1007/s12110-023-09462-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s12110-023-09462-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Honest signalling theory suggests that humans and chimpanzees can extract socially relevant information relating to personality from the faces of their conspecifics. Humans are also able to extract information from chimpanzees' faces. Here, we examine whether personality characteristics of chimpanzees, including measures of psychopathy, can be discerned based purely on facial morphology in photographs. Twenty-one chimpanzees were given naïve and expert personality ratings on the Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI) and the Chimpanzee Triarchic Model of Psychopathy (CHMP-Tri) before and following behavioural observations. Characteristics relating to openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, and disinhibition could be distinguished from the faces of chimpanzees. Individuals higher on disinhibition have lower scores on conscientiousness and emotional stability and higher scores on extraversion, while those higher on meanness have lower conscientiousness and agreeableness. Facial expressions are linked to personality traits present in the TIPI and CHMP-Tri models: the Relaxed Face and the Grooming Face were displayed more by chimpanzees higher on agreeableness, whereas the Compressed Lips Face was observed more in those individuals higher on boldness, and the Full Open Grin was displayed more by chimpanzees higher on extraversion but lower on emotional stability and conscientiousness. Facial expressions were also found to be associated with particular behavioural contexts, namely the Grooming Face in affiliative contexts and the Relaxed and Relaxed Open Mouth Faces in neutral contexts. Dominant chimpanzees display higher levels of boldness and more Compressed Lips Faces, Relaxed Open Mouth Faces, and Grooming Faces than subordinate individuals. These findings support and extend evidence for an honest signalling system and a personality structure shared between humans and chimpanzees. Future research could further explore how personality is conveyed through the face, perhaps through more than just singular aspects of character, and maybe reflecting what chimpanzees themselves are able to do.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":" ","pages":"513-538"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10739467/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71487515","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Link Between Age and Partner Preferences in a Large, International Sample of Single Women. 国际单身女性样本中年龄与伴侣偏好之间的联系。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-26 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-023-09460-4
Laura J Botzet, Amanda Shea, Virginia J Vitzthum, Anna Druet, Maddie Sheesley, Tanja M Gerlach

Women's capacity to reproduce varies over the life span, and developmental goals such as family formation are age-graded and shaped by social norms about the appropriate age for completing specific developmental tasks. Thus, a woman's age may be linked to her ideas about what an ideal partner should be like. With the goals of replicating and extending prior research, in this study we examined the role of age in women's partner preferences across the globe. We investigated associations of age with ideal long-term partner preferences in a cross-cultural sample of 17,254 single (i.e., unpartnered) heterosexual women, ages 18 to 67, from 147 countries. Data were collected via an online questionnaire, the Ideal Partner Survey. Confirming our preregistered hypotheses, we found no or only negligible age effects on preferences for kindness-supportiveness, attractiveness, financial security-successfulness, or education-intelligence. Age was, however, positively associated with preferences for confidence-assertiveness. Consistent with family formation goals, age was associated with an ideal partner's parenting intentions (high until approximately age 30, then decreasing afterward). Age range deemed acceptable (and in particular, the discrepancy between one's own age and the minimum ideal age of a partner) increased with age. This latter pattern also replicated in exploratory analyses based on subsamples of lesbian and bisexual women. In summary, age has a limited impact on partner preferences. Of the attributes investigated, only preference for confidence-assertiveness was linked with age. However, age range deemed acceptable and an ideal partner's parenting intention, a dimension mostly neglected in earlier research, substantially vary with age.

妇女的生育能力在一生中各不相同,家庭组建等发展目标是根据完成特定发展任务的适当年龄的社会规范进行年龄分级和塑造的。因此,女性的年龄可能与她对理想伴侣的想法有关。为了复制和扩展先前的研究,在这项研究中,我们考察了年龄在全球女性伴侣偏好中的作用。我们对来自147个国家的17254名18岁至67岁的单身(即无伴侣)异性恋女性的跨文化样本进行了年龄与理想长期伴侣偏好的相关性调查。数据是通过在线问卷收集的,即理想伴侣调查。证实了我们预先登记的假设,我们发现年龄对善良、支持、吸引力、经济安全成功或教育智力的偏好没有影响,或者只有微不足道的影响。然而,年龄与自信和自信的偏好呈正相关。与组建家庭的目标一致,年龄与理想伴侣的育儿意愿有关(直到30岁左右,年龄一直很高,然后逐渐降低)。被认为可以接受的年龄范围(尤其是自己的年龄和伴侣的最低理想年龄之间的差异)随着年龄的增长而增加。后一种模式在基于女同性恋和双性恋女性子样本的探索性分析中也得到了复制。总之,年龄对伴侣偏好的影响有限。在调查的属性中,只有对自信和自信的偏好与年龄有关。然而,被认为是可以接受的年龄范围和理想伴侣的育儿意愿(这一维度在早期研究中大多被忽视)随着年龄的增长而变化很大。
{"title":"The Link Between Age and Partner Preferences in a Large, International Sample of Single Women.","authors":"Laura J Botzet, Amanda Shea, Virginia J Vitzthum, Anna Druet, Maddie Sheesley, Tanja M Gerlach","doi":"10.1007/s12110-023-09460-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s12110-023-09460-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Women's capacity to reproduce varies over the life span, and developmental goals such as family formation are age-graded and shaped by social norms about the appropriate age for completing specific developmental tasks. Thus, a woman's age may be linked to her ideas about what an ideal partner should be like. With the goals of replicating and extending prior research, in this study we examined the role of age in women's partner preferences across the globe. We investigated associations of age with ideal long-term partner preferences in a cross-cultural sample of 17,254 single (i.e., unpartnered) heterosexual women, ages 18 to 67, from 147 countries. Data were collected via an online questionnaire, the Ideal Partner Survey. Confirming our preregistered hypotheses, we found no or only negligible age effects on preferences for kindness-supportiveness, attractiveness, financial security-successfulness, or education-intelligence. Age was, however, positively associated with preferences for confidence-assertiveness. Consistent with family formation goals, age was associated with an ideal partner's parenting intentions (high until approximately age 30, then decreasing afterward). Age range deemed acceptable (and in particular, the discrepancy between one's own age and the minimum ideal age of a partner) increased with age. This latter pattern also replicated in exploratory analyses based on subsamples of lesbian and bisexual women. In summary, age has a limited impact on partner preferences. Of the attributes investigated, only preference for confidence-assertiveness was linked with age. However, age range deemed acceptable and an ideal partner's parenting intention, a dimension mostly neglected in earlier research, substantially vary with age.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":" ","pages":"539-568"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10739319/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41161864","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differences in the Social Motivations and Emotions of Humans and Other Great Apes. 人类和其他类人猿的社会动机和情感差异。
IF 2.5 2区 社会学 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-16 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-023-09464-0
Michael Tomasello

Humans share with other mammals and primates many social motivations and emotions, but they are also much more cooperative than even their closest primate relatives. Here I review recent comparative experiments and analyses that illustrate humans' species-typical social motivations and emotions for cooperation in comparison with those of other great apes. These may be classified most generally as (i) 'you > me' (e.g., prosocial sympathy, informative and pedagogical motives in communication); (ii) 'you = me' (e.g., feelings of mutual respect, fairness, resentment); (iii) 'we > me' (e.g., feelings of obligation and guilt); and (iv) 'WE (in the group) > me' (e.g., in-group loyalty and conformity to norms, shame, and many in-group biases). The existence of these species-typical and species-universal motivations and emotions provides compelling evidence for the importance of cooperative activities in the human species.

人类与其他哺乳动物和灵长类动物有许多共同的社会动机和情感,但他们也比他们最近的灵长类亲戚更合作。在这里,我回顾了最近的比较实验和分析,这些实验和分析说明了人类与其他类人猿的典型社会动机和合作情感。最一般的分类是(i)“你爱我”(例如,亲社会的同情,交流中的信息和教学动机);(2)“你=我”(例如,相互尊重、公平、怨恨的感觉);(3)“we > me”(例如,义务感和内疚感);和(四)“我们(在群体中)喜欢我”(例如,群体内的忠诚和遵守规范、羞耻和许多群体内的偏见)。这些物种典型的和物种普遍的动机和情感的存在为人类物种合作活动的重要性提供了令人信服的证据。
{"title":"Differences in the Social Motivations and Emotions of Humans and Other Great Apes.","authors":"Michael Tomasello","doi":"10.1007/s12110-023-09464-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s12110-023-09464-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans share with other mammals and primates many social motivations and emotions, but they are also much more cooperative than even their closest primate relatives. Here I review recent comparative experiments and analyses that illustrate humans' species-typical social motivations and emotions for cooperation in comparison with those of other great apes. These may be classified most generally as (i) 'you > me' (e.g., prosocial sympathy, informative and pedagogical motives in communication); (ii) 'you = me' (e.g., feelings of mutual respect, fairness, resentment); (iii) 'we > me' (e.g., feelings of obligation and guilt); and (iv) 'WE (in the group) > me' (e.g., in-group loyalty and conformity to norms, shame, and many in-group biases). The existence of these species-typical and species-universal motivations and emotions provides compelling evidence for the importance of cooperative activities in the human species.</p>","PeriodicalId":47797,"journal":{"name":"Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective","volume":" ","pages":"588-604"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10739453/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136399799","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Human Nature-An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1