首页 > 最新文献

VISUAL COGNITION最新文献

英文 中文
Spatial biases in inhibition of return 抑制返回的空间偏差
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-26 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2023.2188336
Paula Soballa, Lars-Michael Schöpper, C. Frings, Simon Merz
ABSTRACT Inhibition of return (IOR) describes the phenomenon that reaction times (RT) to a target which appears at a previously cued location are slowed down. Spalek and Hammad ([2004]. Supporting the attentional momentum view of IOR: Is attention biased to go right? Perception & Psychophysics, 66(2), 219–233. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194874) reported that IOR effects were smaller at a lower or right location, compared to an upper or left location. In contrast, Snyder and Schmidt ([2014]. No evidence for directional biases in inhibition of return. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 432–435. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0511-3) argued that IOR is unaffected by spatial biases and that any observed differences are better explained by general reaction time differences depending on the target’s location. In two experiments (both N = 31), we aimed to test both diverging predictions by presenting cue and target at four locations along the vertical and horizontal axis. Controlling for a main effect of RTs at different target locations, we still observed a spatial bias on IOR, in that the effect was smaller at the lower than the upper target location. We also found a comparable spatial bias on the IOR-related phenomenon of early facilitation (EF). The results suggest that the magnitude and occurrence of both IOR and EF are affected by spatial configurations. Similarities with spatial biases on other visual phenomena as well as theoretical implications are discussed.
摘要抑制返回(IOR)描述了对出现在先前提示位置的目标的反应时间(RT)减慢的现象。Spalek和Hammad([2004].支持IOR的注意力动量观点:注意力偏向正确吗?感知与心理物理学,66(2),219–233。https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194874)报告称,与上部或左侧位置相比,下部或右侧位置的IOR效果较小。相反,Snyder和Schmidt(【2014】。没有证据表明回归抑制中存在方向性偏差。《心理通讯与评论》,21(2),432–435。https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0511-3)认为IOR不受空间偏差的影响,任何观察到的差异都可以通过取决于目标位置的一般反应时间差异来更好地解释。在两个实验中(均为N = 31),我们旨在通过在纵轴和横轴的四个位置呈现线索和目标来测试这两种发散预测。在控制不同目标位置的RT的主要影响时,我们仍然观察到IOR的空间偏差,因为较低目标位置的影响小于较高目标位置。我们还发现,与IOR相关的早期促进现象(EF)存在可比的空间偏差。结果表明,IOR和EF的大小和出现都受到空间配置的影响。讨论了在其他视觉现象上与空间偏见的相似性以及理论含义。
{"title":"Spatial biases in inhibition of return","authors":"Paula Soballa, Lars-Michael Schöpper, C. Frings, Simon Merz","doi":"10.1080/13506285.2023.2188336","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2023.2188336","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Inhibition of return (IOR) describes the phenomenon that reaction times (RT) to a target which appears at a previously cued location are slowed down. Spalek and Hammad ([2004]. Supporting the attentional momentum view of IOR: Is attention biased to go right? Perception & Psychophysics, 66(2), 219–233. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194874) reported that IOR effects were smaller at a lower or right location, compared to an upper or left location. In contrast, Snyder and Schmidt ([2014]. No evidence for directional biases in inhibition of return. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 432–435. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0511-3) argued that IOR is unaffected by spatial biases and that any observed differences are better explained by general reaction time differences depending on the target’s location. In two experiments (both N = 31), we aimed to test both diverging predictions by presenting cue and target at four locations along the vertical and horizontal axis. Controlling for a main effect of RTs at different target locations, we still observed a spatial bias on IOR, in that the effect was smaller at the lower than the upper target location. We also found a comparable spatial bias on the IOR-related phenomenon of early facilitation (EF). The results suggest that the magnitude and occurrence of both IOR and EF are affected by spatial configurations. Similarities with spatial biases on other visual phenomena as well as theoretical implications are discussed.","PeriodicalId":47961,"journal":{"name":"VISUAL COGNITION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48982618","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unfamiliar faces might as well be another species: Evidence from a face matching task with human and monkey faces 不熟悉的面孔也可能是另一个物种:来自人类和猴子面孔匹配任务的证据
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-26 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2023.2184894
K. Ritchie, Tessa R. Flack, L. Maréchal
ABSTRACT Humans are good at recognizing familiar faces, but are more error-prone at recognizing an unfamiliar person across different images. It has been suggested that familiar and unfamiliar faces are processed qualitatively differently. But are unfamiliar faces at least processed differently from monkey faces? Here we tested 366 volunteers on a face matching test – two images presented side-by-side with participants judging whether the images show the same identity or two different identities – comparing performance with familiar and unfamiliar human faces, and monkey faces. The results showed that performance was most accurate for familiar faces, and was above chance for monkey faces. Although accuracy was higher for unfamiliar humans than monkeys on different identity trials, there was no unfamiliar human advantage over monkeys on same identity trials. The results give new insights into unfamiliar face processing, showing that in some ways unfamiliar faces might as well be another species.
人类擅长识别熟悉的面孔,但在不同图像中识别不熟悉的人更容易出错。有人认为,熟悉和不熟悉的面孔在质上的处理是不同的。但是,至少对陌生面孔的处理与对猴子面孔的处理是不同的吗?在这里,我们对366名志愿者进行了面部匹配测试——两张图片并排呈现,参与者判断这两张图片显示的是相同的身份还是两个不同的身份——比较熟悉的和不熟悉的人脸,以及猴子的脸的表现。结果表明,对于熟悉的面孔,准确率最高,而对于猴子的面孔,准确率更高。尽管在不同身份的实验中,不熟悉的人比猴子的准确率更高,但在相同身份的实验中,不熟悉的人比猴子没有优势。研究结果为不熟悉面孔的处理提供了新的见解,表明在某些方面,不熟悉的面孔也可能是另一种物种。
{"title":"Unfamiliar faces might as well be another species: Evidence from a face matching task with human and monkey faces","authors":"K. Ritchie, Tessa R. Flack, L. Maréchal","doi":"10.1080/13506285.2023.2184894","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2023.2184894","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Humans are good at recognizing familiar faces, but are more error-prone at recognizing an unfamiliar person across different images. It has been suggested that familiar and unfamiliar faces are processed qualitatively differently. But are unfamiliar faces at least processed differently from monkey faces? Here we tested 366 volunteers on a face matching test – two images presented side-by-side with participants judging whether the images show the same identity or two different identities – comparing performance with familiar and unfamiliar human faces, and monkey faces. The results showed that performance was most accurate for familiar faces, and was above chance for monkey faces. Although accuracy was higher for unfamiliar humans than monkeys on different identity trials, there was no unfamiliar human advantage over monkeys on same identity trials. The results give new insights into unfamiliar face processing, showing that in some ways unfamiliar faces might as well be another species.","PeriodicalId":47961,"journal":{"name":"VISUAL COGNITION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44554706","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Time course of encoding and maintenance of stereoscopically induced size–distance scaling 编码和维持立体诱导尺寸-距离缩放的时间过程
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-26 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2023.2174232
Wanyi Guan, Binglong Li, J. Qian
ABSTRACT The mechanism of size constancy assures that an object is perceived to be constant in size despite that its retinal size varies with viewing distance. Conversely, an object can be perceived as illusorily larger if the perceived distance becomes greater, due to the size–distance scaling mechanism. The present study aimed at exploring how size–distance scaling is modulated by the encoding duration and how its memory is affected by the retention duration. In Experiment 1, we presented two stimuli simultaneously at two stereoscopic depth planes and manipulated the presentation duration, and found that the magnitude of the size scaling increased with presentation duration. In Experiment 2, we examined the maintenance of size–distance scaling when component stimulus was kept in working memory with variable delays. The results showed that the size scaling was reliably retrieved from working memory if there was no disparity manipulation on the to-be-memorized item, but it decreased with retention if a disparity was applied to the to-be-memorized item. The findings suggest that although the post-scaling size can be stored in working memory, the scaling mechanism may still be in effect when there were conflicts in the oculomotor cues and disparity cues that produces depth perception.
尺寸恒定的机制保证了一个物体被认为是恒定的大小,尽管它的视网膜大小随观看距离而变化。相反,由于尺寸-距离缩放机制,如果感知距离变大,物体就会被误认为变大。本研究旨在探讨编码持续时间如何调节大小-距离缩放,以及其记忆如何受到保留持续时间的影响。在实验1中,我们在两个立体深度平面上同时呈现两个刺激,并对呈现时间进行操纵,发现尺寸缩放幅度随呈现时间的增加而增加。在实验2中,我们考察了在工作记忆中以可变延迟保存成分刺激时尺寸-距离尺度的维持情况。结果表明,在未对待记忆项目进行视差操作的情况下,工作记忆的尺寸缩放量能够得到可靠的恢复,但在待记忆项目上施加视差操作时,工作记忆的尺寸缩放量会随着记忆的保留而降低。研究结果表明,虽然后尺度可以存储在工作记忆中,但当产生深度知觉的动眼线索和视差线索存在冲突时,尺度机制可能仍然有效。
{"title":"Time course of encoding and maintenance of stereoscopically induced size–distance scaling","authors":"Wanyi Guan, Binglong Li, J. Qian","doi":"10.1080/13506285.2023.2174232","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2023.2174232","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT The mechanism of size constancy assures that an object is perceived to be constant in size despite that its retinal size varies with viewing distance. Conversely, an object can be perceived as illusorily larger if the perceived distance becomes greater, due to the size–distance scaling mechanism. The present study aimed at exploring how size–distance scaling is modulated by the encoding duration and how its memory is affected by the retention duration. In Experiment 1, we presented two stimuli simultaneously at two stereoscopic depth planes and manipulated the presentation duration, and found that the magnitude of the size scaling increased with presentation duration. In Experiment 2, we examined the maintenance of size–distance scaling when component stimulus was kept in working memory with variable delays. The results showed that the size scaling was reliably retrieved from working memory if there was no disparity manipulation on the to-be-memorized item, but it decreased with retention if a disparity was applied to the to-be-memorized item. The findings suggest that although the post-scaling size can be stored in working memory, the scaling mechanism may still be in effect when there were conflicts in the oculomotor cues and disparity cues that produces depth perception.","PeriodicalId":47961,"journal":{"name":"VISUAL COGNITION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47614654","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Gaze cues vs. arrow cues at short vs. long durations 短时间和长时间的凝视提示与箭头提示
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-10-21 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2022.2154878
Tarini Singh, Lars-Michael Schöpper, G. Domes, C. Frings
ABSTRACT Information processing is more efficient at cued relative to non-cued locations. A number of studies have examined whether non-predictive gaze cues are special due to their biological relevance. While most studies indicate that cueing effects of gaze cues and arrow cues are similar, one aspect remains to be examined – cue duration. Contrary to early findings, a number of studies have observed cueing effects at short durations for arrow cues. For gaze cues however, the evidence is more mixed. The present study therefore aims to directly compare the cueing effects of arrow and gaze cues at short and long durations. Participants (N = 30) performed a discrimination task and were presented with arrow and gaze cues for short or long durations. Cueing effects were measured at each duration for each cue type. Significant cueing effects were observed for both cue types at both short and long duration. Moreover, for both cue types, no difference was observed in the magnitude of cueing effects at short and long duration. The results suggest that both cue types cues can efficiently orient attention even at short cue durations, and that the biological relevance of gaze direction cues do not provide any advantage over arrows.
摘要相对于非提示位置,提示位置的信息处理效率更高。许多研究已经检验了非预测性凝视线索是否由于其生物学相关性而具有特殊性。虽然大多数研究表明,凝视线索和箭头线索的提示效果相似,但有一个方面有待研究——提示持续时间。与早期发现相反,许多研究已经观察到箭头提示在短时间内的提示效应。然而,对于凝视暗示,证据更为复杂。因此,本研究旨在直接比较箭头和凝视线索在短时间和长时间内的提示效果。参与者(N=30)执行辨别任务,并在短时间或长时间内获得箭头和凝视提示。在每种提示类型的每个持续时间测量提示效果。在短时间和长时间两种提示类型中都观察到了显著的提示效应。此外,对于这两种线索类型,在短时间和长时间的线索效应幅度上没有观察到差异。结果表明,即使在短的线索持续时间内,这两种线索类型的线索也能有效地引导注意力,而且凝视方向线索的生物学相关性与箭头相比没有任何优势。
{"title":"Gaze cues vs. arrow cues at short vs. long durations","authors":"Tarini Singh, Lars-Michael Schöpper, G. Domes, C. Frings","doi":"10.1080/13506285.2022.2154878","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2022.2154878","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Information processing is more efficient at cued relative to non-cued locations. A number of studies have examined whether non-predictive gaze cues are special due to their biological relevance. While most studies indicate that cueing effects of gaze cues and arrow cues are similar, one aspect remains to be examined – cue duration. Contrary to early findings, a number of studies have observed cueing effects at short durations for arrow cues. For gaze cues however, the evidence is more mixed. The present study therefore aims to directly compare the cueing effects of arrow and gaze cues at short and long durations. Participants (N = 30) performed a discrimination task and were presented with arrow and gaze cues for short or long durations. Cueing effects were measured at each duration for each cue type. Significant cueing effects were observed for both cue types at both short and long duration. Moreover, for both cue types, no difference was observed in the magnitude of cueing effects at short and long duration. The results suggest that both cue types cues can efficiently orient attention even at short cue durations, and that the biological relevance of gaze direction cues do not provide any advantage over arrows.","PeriodicalId":47961,"journal":{"name":"VISUAL COGNITION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2022-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43721549","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Working memory for movement rhythms given spatial relevance: Effects of sequence length and maintenance delay 给定空间相关性的运动节奏的工作记忆:序列长度和维持延迟的影响
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-10-21 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2022.2162173
Shiau-Chuen Chiou, T. Schack
ABSTRACT Temporal information is an essential component of human movements. However, it is still unclear how the temporal information is extracted from complex whole-body movements through observation and how it is encoded and retained in working memory. In the current study, we investigated how the sequence length and maintenance delay influence working memory for movement rhythms (i.e., temporal structures of movement sequences) after considering the task-relevance of the corresponding spatial information and the sensitivity difference between spatial and temporal processing in visual perception. We found that the sequence length – in the sense of information load more than temporal duration – may act as the first bottleneck in the processing of movement rhythms, deciding whether temporal information can be encoded as individual units in high precision or it might be encoded as an ensemble “whole” in relatively low precision. In addition, the maintenance delay may act as the second bottleneck, determining to what extent the encoded information can be retained in memory.
摘要时间信息是人类运动的重要组成部分。然而,目前尚不清楚如何通过观察从复杂的全身运动中提取时间信息,以及如何将其编码并保留在工作记忆中。在当前的研究中,我们在考虑了相应空间信息的任务相关性以及视觉感知中空间和时间处理之间的敏感性差异后,研究了序列长度和维持延迟如何影响运动节奏(即运动序列的时间结构)的工作记忆。我们发现,序列长度——从信息负载而非持续时间的角度来看——可能是运动节奏处理的第一个瓶颈,决定了时间信息是可以高精度编码为单个单元,还是可以相对低精度编码为整体“整体”。此外,维护延迟可以充当第二瓶颈,从而确定编码信息可以在多大程度上保留在存储器中。
{"title":"Working memory for movement rhythms given spatial relevance: Effects of sequence length and maintenance delay","authors":"Shiau-Chuen Chiou, T. Schack","doi":"10.1080/13506285.2022.2162173","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2022.2162173","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Temporal information is an essential component of human movements. However, it is still unclear how the temporal information is extracted from complex whole-body movements through observation and how it is encoded and retained in working memory. In the current study, we investigated how the sequence length and maintenance delay influence working memory for movement rhythms (i.e., temporal structures of movement sequences) after considering the task-relevance of the corresponding spatial information and the sensitivity difference between spatial and temporal processing in visual perception. We found that the sequence length – in the sense of information load more than temporal duration – may act as the first bottleneck in the processing of movement rhythms, deciding whether temporal information can be encoded as individual units in high precision or it might be encoded as an ensemble “whole” in relatively low precision. In addition, the maintenance delay may act as the second bottleneck, determining to what extent the encoded information can be retained in memory.","PeriodicalId":47961,"journal":{"name":"VISUAL COGNITION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2022-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44824391","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Sources and mechanisms of modality-specific distraction in visual short-term memory 视觉短期记忆中模态特异性分心的来源和机制
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-10-21 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2022.2162174
Tom Mercer, R. Shaw, Luke P Fisher
ABSTRACT Visual short-term and working memory can be disrupted by irrelevant, distracting input occurring after encoding. Distractors similar to the original memory are known to be interfering, but it is unclear whether dissimilar distractors have the same disruptive effect. The presence of dissimilar distraction would be problematic for views of similarity-based interference, hence the present study investigated modality-specific distraction using a procedure that required participants to compare single target and probe objects over a delay. An irrelevant distractor could be presented during the delay separating the target and probe, but it varied in its similarity to the target. In four experiments, recognition was disrupted by the presence of a distractor, even when the distractors were highly dissimilar to the target. Furthermore, the interference effect was not reduced when the same distractors were repeatedly used throughout the experiment, and interference from dissimilar distractors was only lessened when it was extremely predictable. These findings indicate that susceptibility to dissimilar distraction is a persistent limitation in visual short-term memory.
摘要编码后出现的不相关的、分散注意力的输入可能会扰乱视觉短期记忆和工作记忆。已知与原始记忆相似的干扰因素会产生干扰,但尚不清楚不同的干扰因素是否具有相同的干扰效果。不同分心的存在对于基于相似性的干扰的观点来说是有问题的,因此本研究使用了一种程序来研究特定模态的分心,该程序要求参与者在一段时间内比较单个目标和探测对象。在目标和探针分离的延迟过程中,可能会出现不相关的干扰物,但它与目标的相似性不同。在四个实验中,即使干扰物与目标高度不同,识别也会因干扰物的存在而中断。此外,当在整个实验中重复使用相同的干扰物时,干扰效应并没有减少,而来自不同干扰物的干扰只有在非常可预测的情况下才会减少。这些发现表明,对不同分心的易感性是视觉短期记忆的一个持续限制。
{"title":"Sources and mechanisms of modality-specific distraction in visual short-term memory","authors":"Tom Mercer, R. Shaw, Luke P Fisher","doi":"10.1080/13506285.2022.2162174","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2022.2162174","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Visual short-term and working memory can be disrupted by irrelevant, distracting input occurring after encoding. Distractors similar to the original memory are known to be interfering, but it is unclear whether dissimilar distractors have the same disruptive effect. The presence of dissimilar distraction would be problematic for views of similarity-based interference, hence the present study investigated modality-specific distraction using a procedure that required participants to compare single target and probe objects over a delay. An irrelevant distractor could be presented during the delay separating the target and probe, but it varied in its similarity to the target. In four experiments, recognition was disrupted by the presence of a distractor, even when the distractors were highly dissimilar to the target. Furthermore, the interference effect was not reduced when the same distractors were repeatedly used throughout the experiment, and interference from dissimilar distractors was only lessened when it was extremely predictable. These findings indicate that susceptibility to dissimilar distraction is a persistent limitation in visual short-term memory.","PeriodicalId":47961,"journal":{"name":"VISUAL COGNITION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2022-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49491151","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Impact of simulated target blur on the preparation and execution of aiming movements 模拟目标模糊对瞄准动作准备和执行的影响
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-09-14 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2022.2140730
J. Roberts, James Maiden, S. Bennett
ABSTRACT While visual information directly influences the preparation and control of aiming movements, less is known about the influence of a degraded visual context such as target blur. Participants aimed as quickly and accurately as possible within a virtual aiming environment to clear or blurred targets using a stylus on a digitizing board. Findings showed a more prolonged time to initiate movements for blurred compared to clear targets, but no influence for movement time, or end-point accuracy and precision. The adaptation in movement preparation may reflect an initial uncertainty surrounding the visual context; namely, the visual target characteristics that are typically needed to avoid any error. Meanwhile, the absence of any influence within movement reflects the processing of the coarse and dynamic visual characteristics of the limb, which was independent of the degraded visual context of the target. These findings may contribute further insights toward low vision and possible functional adaptations.
虽然视觉信息直接影响瞄准运动的准备和控制,但人们对目标模糊等视觉环境退化的影响知之甚少。参与者在虚拟瞄准环境中尽可能快速准确地瞄准,使用数字化板上的触控笔来清除或模糊目标。研究结果显示,与清晰目标相比,模糊目标启动运动的时间更长,但对运动时间、终点准确性和精度没有影响。动作准备中的适应可能反映了最初围绕视觉环境的不确定性;也就是说,通常需要视觉目标特征来避免任何错误。同时,在运动中不受任何影响反映了肢体粗糙和动态的视觉特征的加工,这与目标视觉环境的退化无关。这些发现可能有助于进一步了解低视力和可能的功能适应。
{"title":"Impact of simulated target blur on the preparation and execution of aiming movements","authors":"J. Roberts, James Maiden, S. Bennett","doi":"10.1080/13506285.2022.2140730","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2022.2140730","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT While visual information directly influences the preparation and control of aiming movements, less is known about the influence of a degraded visual context such as target blur. Participants aimed as quickly and accurately as possible within a virtual aiming environment to clear or blurred targets using a stylus on a digitizing board. Findings showed a more prolonged time to initiate movements for blurred compared to clear targets, but no influence for movement time, or end-point accuracy and precision. The adaptation in movement preparation may reflect an initial uncertainty surrounding the visual context; namely, the visual target characteristics that are typically needed to avoid any error. Meanwhile, the absence of any influence within movement reflects the processing of the coarse and dynamic visual characteristics of the limb, which was independent of the degraded visual context of the target. These findings may contribute further insights toward low vision and possible functional adaptations.","PeriodicalId":47961,"journal":{"name":"VISUAL COGNITION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2022-09-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43388292","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does superior visual working memory capacity enable greater distractor suppression? 卓越的视觉工作记忆能力是否能更好地抑制干扰?
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-09-14 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2022.2145403
Christopher Hauck, M. Lien, E. Ruthruff
ABSTRACT We asked whether individuals high in working memory capacity have a superior ability to proactively suppress features. If so, it would help explain why these individuals are more resistant to attention capture. We tested this hypothesis using the capture-probe paradigm employed in Lien et al. (2022. On preventing attention capture: Is singleton suppression actually singleton suppression? Psychological Research, 86(6), 1958–1971). Participants (N = 112) performed a colour change detection task, assessing visual working memory capacity. They then performed a visual search task (70% of the trials) intermixed with probe tasks (30% of the trials). For the visual search task, either a salient colour singleton distractor or non-salient distractor (a triplet) appeared with the target object. For the probe recall task, participants reported probe letters that briefly appeared inside each object. Replicating Lien et al., a suppression effect on probe recall accuracy was observed for both salient singletons and non-salient triplets. Critically, high and low visual working memory capacity individuals showed statistically equivalent ability to suppress colour distractors. These findings suggest that proactive suppression is not the mechanism by which high-capacity individuals achieve greater resistance to capture. Proactive suppression may be an implicit process that does not require special working memory capabilities.
摘要:我们研究工作记忆容量高的个体是否具有更强的主动抑制特征的能力。如果是这样,这将有助于解释为什么这些人对注意力吸引更有抵抗力。我们使用Lien等人(2022)采用的捕获-探针范式检验了这一假设。关于防止注意力捕获:单例抑制真的是单例抑制吗?心理研究,86(6),1958-1971。参与者(N = 112)执行颜色变化检测任务,评估视觉工作记忆容量。然后他们执行视觉搜索任务(70%的试验)和探测任务(30%的试验)。在视觉搜索任务中,一个显著的单一颜色分心物或一个非显著的分心物(三组)与目标物体一起出现。对于探针回忆任务,参与者报告在每个物体中短暂出现的探针字母。重复Lien等人的研究,在显著单子和非显著三子中都观察到探针回忆准确性的抑制效应。重要的是,高和低视觉工作记忆能力的个体在统计上显示出相同的抑制颜色干扰的能力。这些发现表明,主动抑制并不是高能力个体获得更大抵抗捕获的机制。主动抑制可能是一种不需要特殊工作记忆能力的内隐过程。
{"title":"Does superior visual working memory capacity enable greater distractor suppression?","authors":"Christopher Hauck, M. Lien, E. Ruthruff","doi":"10.1080/13506285.2022.2145403","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2022.2145403","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT We asked whether individuals high in working memory capacity have a superior ability to proactively suppress features. If so, it would help explain why these individuals are more resistant to attention capture. We tested this hypothesis using the capture-probe paradigm employed in Lien et al. (2022. On preventing attention capture: Is singleton suppression actually singleton suppression? Psychological Research, 86(6), 1958–1971). Participants (N = 112) performed a colour change detection task, assessing visual working memory capacity. They then performed a visual search task (70% of the trials) intermixed with probe tasks (30% of the trials). For the visual search task, either a salient colour singleton distractor or non-salient distractor (a triplet) appeared with the target object. For the probe recall task, participants reported probe letters that briefly appeared inside each object. Replicating Lien et al., a suppression effect on probe recall accuracy was observed for both salient singletons and non-salient triplets. Critically, high and low visual working memory capacity individuals showed statistically equivalent ability to suppress colour distractors. These findings suggest that proactive suppression is not the mechanism by which high-capacity individuals achieve greater resistance to capture. Proactive suppression may be an implicit process that does not require special working memory capabilities.","PeriodicalId":47961,"journal":{"name":"VISUAL COGNITION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2022-09-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42572449","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Visual detection of 3D mirror-symmetry and 3D rotational-symmetry 三维镜像对称和三维旋转对称的视觉检测
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-09-14 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2022.2139314
T. Sawada, M. Farshchi
ABSTRACT Detecting 3D symmetry is important for the human visual system because many objects in our everyday life are 3D symmetrical. Many are 3D mirror-symmetrical and others are 3D rotational-symmetrical. But note that their retinal images are 2D symmetrical only in degenerate views. It has been suggested that a human observer can detect 3D mirror-symmetry even from a 2D retinal image of a 3D mirror-symmetrical pair of contours. There are model-based invariants of the 3D mirror-symmetrical pair of contours in the retinal image and there are additional invariant features when the contours are individually planar. There are also model-based invariants of a 3D rotational-symmetrical pair of contours. These invariant features of 3D mirror-symmetry and rotational-symmetry are analogous to one another but the features of 3D rotational-symmetry are computationally more difficult than the features of 3D mirror-symmetry. Experiment 1 showed that only 3D mirror-symmetry could be detected reliably while the detection of 3D rotational-symmetry was close to chance-level. Experiment 2 showed that the detection of 3D mirror-symmetry is partly based on the model-based invariants of 3D mirror-symmetry and the planarity of the contours. These results show that the visual system has evolved to favour the perception of 3D mirror-symmetry.
由于我们日常生活中的许多物体都是三维对称的,因此检测三维对称性对人类视觉系统非常重要。许多是三维镜像对称的,其他是三维旋转对称的。但请注意,他们的视网膜图像仅在退化视图中是二维对称的。有人认为,人类观察者甚至可以从3D镜像对称轮廓对的2D视网膜图像中检测到3D镜像对称。视网膜图像中存在基于模型的镜面对称对轮廓的不变量,并且当轮廓单独为平面时存在额外的不变量特征。也有基于模型的三维旋转对称轮廓对的不变量。三维镜像对称和旋转对称的这些不变特征彼此相似,但三维旋转对称的特征在计算上比三维镜像对称的特征更困难。实验1表明,只有三维镜像对称能够被可靠地检测出来,而三维旋转对称的检测接近机会水平。实验2表明,三维镜像对称的检测部分是基于模型的三维镜像对称不变量和轮廓的平面性。这些结果表明,视觉系统已经进化到有利于三维镜像对称的感知。
{"title":"Visual detection of 3D mirror-symmetry and 3D rotational-symmetry","authors":"T. Sawada, M. Farshchi","doi":"10.1080/13506285.2022.2139314","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2022.2139314","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Detecting 3D symmetry is important for the human visual system because many objects in our everyday life are 3D symmetrical. Many are 3D mirror-symmetrical and others are 3D rotational-symmetrical. But note that their retinal images are 2D symmetrical only in degenerate views. It has been suggested that a human observer can detect 3D mirror-symmetry even from a 2D retinal image of a 3D mirror-symmetrical pair of contours. There are model-based invariants of the 3D mirror-symmetrical pair of contours in the retinal image and there are additional invariant features when the contours are individually planar. There are also model-based invariants of a 3D rotational-symmetrical pair of contours. These invariant features of 3D mirror-symmetry and rotational-symmetry are analogous to one another but the features of 3D rotational-symmetry are computationally more difficult than the features of 3D mirror-symmetry. Experiment 1 showed that only 3D mirror-symmetry could be detected reliably while the detection of 3D rotational-symmetry was close to chance-level. Experiment 2 showed that the detection of 3D mirror-symmetry is partly based on the model-based invariants of 3D mirror-symmetry and the planarity of the contours. These results show that the visual system has evolved to favour the perception of 3D mirror-symmetry.","PeriodicalId":47961,"journal":{"name":"VISUAL COGNITION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2022-09-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43547445","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Found in translation: The role of response mappings for observing binding effects in localization tasks 在翻译中发现:在本地化任务中观察绑定效果的响应映射的作用
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-09-14 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2022.2139033
Lars-Michael Schöpper, M. Lappe, C. Frings
ABSTRACT According to action control theories, response and stimulus’ features are integrated into event files. Repeating any of an event file’s components retrieves the previously bound information, causing benefits for full repetition, but interference for partial repetition. Yet, such “binding effects” are absent in localization performance. By assuming sequential processing steps until response execution as assumed in visual search, we hypothesized that, for localization, participants can execute their response without the need to process target features. Hence, post-selective processing might be crucial for binding effects to emerge. Here, participants localized coloured targets appearing on one of four corners of a touchpad in two response conditions, namely, directly tapping on the target (direct response mapping), and tapping on the corner diagonal opposite to the target (translational response mapping). Only the translational response mapping yielded binding effects between localization response and colour. The direct response mapping instead showed an effect that is better explained by (non-spatial) Inhibition of Return or related change benefit effects. We conclude that an arbitrary response mapping – based on a translation of a spatial feature into a non-direct spatial response – can lead to binding effects even in localization tasks.
摘要根据动作控制理论,将反应和刺激的特征集成到事件文件中。重复事件文件的任何组件都会检索以前绑定的信息,这有利于完全重复,但会干扰部分重复。然而,这种“绑定效应”在本地化性能中是不存在的。通过假设在视觉搜索中假设的顺序处理步骤直到响应执行,我们假设,对于定位,参与者可以在不需要处理目标特征的情况下执行他们的响应。因此,后选择性加工可能对结合效应的出现至关重要。在这里,参与者在两种响应条件下定位出现在触摸板四个角之一上的彩色目标,即直接敲击目标(直接响应映射)和敲击与目标相反的角对角线(平移响应映射)。只有翻译反应映射产生了定位反应和颜色之间的结合效应。相反,直接反应映射显示了一种效果,这种效果可以通过(非空间)抑制回报或相关的变化效益效应来更好地解释。我们得出的结论是,基于将空间特征转换为非直接空间响应的任意响应映射,即使在定位任务中也会导致绑定效应。
{"title":"Found in translation: The role of response mappings for observing binding effects in localization tasks","authors":"Lars-Michael Schöpper, M. Lappe, C. Frings","doi":"10.1080/13506285.2022.2139033","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2022.2139033","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT According to action control theories, response and stimulus’ features are integrated into event files. Repeating any of an event file’s components retrieves the previously bound information, causing benefits for full repetition, but interference for partial repetition. Yet, such “binding effects” are absent in localization performance. By assuming sequential processing steps until response execution as assumed in visual search, we hypothesized that, for localization, participants can execute their response without the need to process target features. Hence, post-selective processing might be crucial for binding effects to emerge. Here, participants localized coloured targets appearing on one of four corners of a touchpad in two response conditions, namely, directly tapping on the target (direct response mapping), and tapping on the corner diagonal opposite to the target (translational response mapping). Only the translational response mapping yielded binding effects between localization response and colour. The direct response mapping instead showed an effect that is better explained by (non-spatial) Inhibition of Return or related change benefit effects. We conclude that an arbitrary response mapping – based on a translation of a spatial feature into a non-direct spatial response – can lead to binding effects even in localization tasks.","PeriodicalId":47961,"journal":{"name":"VISUAL COGNITION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2022-09-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43795102","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
期刊
VISUAL COGNITION
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1