首页 > 最新文献

Infant Behavior & Development最新文献

英文 中文
Mother-infant self- and interactive contingency at four months and infant cognition at one year: A view from microanalysis 四个月时的母婴自我和互动偶然性与一岁时的婴儿认知:微观分析视角
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101920
Beatrice Beebe , Gavkhar Abdurokhmonova , Sang Han Lee , Georgios Dougalis , Frances Champagne , Virginia Rauh , Molly Algermissen , Julie Herbstman , Amy E. Margolis

Although a considerable literature documents associations between early mother-infant interaction and cognitive outcomes in the first years of life, few studies examine the contributions of contingently coordinated mother-infant interaction to infant cognitive development. This study examined associations between the temporal dynamics of the contingent coordination of mother-infant face-to-face interaction at 4 months and cognitive performance on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development at age one year in a sample of (N = 100) Latina mother-infant pairs. Split-screen videotaped interactions were coded on a one second time base for the communication modalities of infant and mother gaze and facial affect, infant vocal affect, and mother touch. Multi-level time-series models evaluated self- and interactive contingent processes in these modalities and revealed 4-month patterns of interaction associated with higher one-year cognitive performance, not identified in prior studies. Infant and mother self-contingency, the moment-to-moment probability that the individual’s prior behavior predicts the individual’s future behavior, was the most robust measure associated with infant cognitive performance. Self-contingency findings showed that more varying infant behavior was optimal for higher infant cognitive performance, namely, greater modulation of negative affect; more stable maternal behavior was optimal for higher infant cognitive performance, namely, greater likelihood of sustaining positive facial affect. Although interactive contingency findings were sparse, they showed that, when mothers looked away, or dampened their faces to interest or mild negative facial affect, infants with higher 12-month cognitive performance were less likely to show negative vocal affect. We suggest that infant ability to modulate negative affect, and maternal ability to sustain positive affect, may be mutually reinforcing, together creating a dyadic climate that is associated with more optimal infant cognitive development.

尽管有大量文献记录了早期母婴互动与婴儿出生后最初几年的认知结果之间的关系,但很少有研究探讨偶然协调的母婴互动对婴儿认知发展的贡献。本研究以(N = 100)拉丁裔母婴对为样本,考察了 4 个月时母婴面对面互动的偶然协调的时间动态与 1 岁时贝利婴儿发展量表(Bayley Scales of Infant Development)认知表现之间的关系。分屏互动录像以一秒为基准,对婴儿和母亲的注视、面部情感、婴儿的声音情感和母亲的抚摸等交流方式进行编码。多层次时间序列模型评估了这些模式中的自我和互动或然过程,并揭示了 4 个月的互动模式与更高的一年期认知表现相关,这在之前的研究中是没有发现的。婴儿和母亲的自我权变,即个体先前行为预测个体未来行为的瞬间概率,是与婴儿认知表现相关的最可靠的测量指标。自我权变研究结果表明,婴儿行为变化越多,婴儿认知能力越强,即对负面情绪的调节能力越强;母亲行为越稳定,婴儿认知能力越强,即维持面部积极情绪的可能性越大。虽然交互式或然性研究结果很少,但它们表明,当母亲将目光移开,或将脸部表情减弱到感兴趣或轻微的负面面部情绪时,12 个月认知能力较高的婴儿较少表现出负面的声音情绪。我们认为,婴儿调节消极情绪的能力和母亲维持积极情绪的能力可能是相辅相成的,它们共同创造了一种与婴儿认知发展更理想相关的家庭氛围。
{"title":"Mother-infant self- and interactive contingency at four months and infant cognition at one year: A view from microanalysis","authors":"Beatrice Beebe ,&nbsp;Gavkhar Abdurokhmonova ,&nbsp;Sang Han Lee ,&nbsp;Georgios Dougalis ,&nbsp;Frances Champagne ,&nbsp;Virginia Rauh ,&nbsp;Molly Algermissen ,&nbsp;Julie Herbstman ,&nbsp;Amy E. Margolis","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101920","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101920","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>Although a considerable literature documents associations between early mother-infant interaction and cognitive outcomes in the first years of life, few studies examine the contributions of contingently coordinated mother-infant interaction to infant cognitive development. This study examined associations between the temporal dynamics of the contingent coordination of mother-infant face-to-face interaction at 4 months and cognitive performance on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development at age one year in a sample of (N = 100) Latina mother-infant pairs. Split-screen videotaped interactions were coded on a one second time base for the communication modalities of infant and mother gaze and facial affect, infant vocal affect, and mother touch. Multi-level time-series models evaluated self- and interactive contingent processes in these modalities and revealed 4-month patterns of interaction associated with higher one-year cognitive performance, not identified in prior studies. Infant and mother </span><em>self-contingency</em><span>, the moment-to-moment probability that the individual’s prior behavior<span> predicts the individual’s future behavior, was the most robust measure associated with infant cognitive performance. Self-contingency findings showed that more varying infant behavior was optimal for higher infant cognitive performance, namely, greater modulation of negative affect; more stable maternal behavior was optimal for higher infant cognitive performance, namely, greater likelihood of sustaining positive facial affect. Although </span></span><em>interactive contingency</em> findings were sparse, they showed that, when mothers looked away, or dampened their faces to interest or mild negative facial affect, infants with higher 12-month cognitive performance were less likely to show negative vocal affect. We suggest that infant ability to modulate negative affect, and maternal ability to sustain positive affect, may be mutually reinforcing, together creating a dyadic climate that is associated with more optimal infant cognitive development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101920"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139480238","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The United States reference values of the Bayley III motor scale are suitable in Suriname 贝利 III 运动量表的美国参考值适用于苏里南
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101922
Maria JAJ Fleurkens-Peeters , Wilco CWR Zijlmans , Reinier P. Akkermans , Maria WG Nijhuis-van der Sanden , Anjo JWM Janssen

To determine if the United States reference values of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, version III motor scale are suitable for Surinamese infants, we assessed 151 healthy infants at 3, 12, 24 and 36 months of age. The mean fine motor, gross motor, and composite scores of the total group did not significantly differ from the US norms, although some significant but not clinically relevant differences were found (lower fine motor scores at 12 months, lower gross motor and total composite scores at 24 months, and higher scores for gross motor and composite scores at 3 months).

为了确定美国贝利婴幼儿发展量表(Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development)第三版运动量表的参考值是否适合苏里南婴儿,我们对 151 名健康婴儿进行了 3、12、24 和 36 个月大时的评估。尽管发现了一些显著但与临床无关的差异(12 个月时精细运动得分较低、24 个月时粗大运动得分和综合得分较低、3 个月时粗大运动得分和综合得分较高),但总组的精细运动、粗大运动和综合得分的平均值与美国标准值没有显著差异。
{"title":"The United States reference values of the Bayley III motor scale are suitable in Suriname","authors":"Maria JAJ Fleurkens-Peeters ,&nbsp;Wilco CWR Zijlmans ,&nbsp;Reinier P. Akkermans ,&nbsp;Maria WG Nijhuis-van der Sanden ,&nbsp;Anjo JWM Janssen","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101922","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101922","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>To determine if the United States reference values of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, version III motor scale are suitable for Surinamese infants, we assessed 151 healthy infants at 3, 12, 24 and 36 months of age. The mean fine motor, gross motor, and composite scores of the total group did not significantly differ from the US norms, although some significant but not clinically relevant differences were found (lower fine motor scores at 12 months, lower gross motor and total composite scores at 24 months, and higher scores for gross motor and composite scores at 3 months).</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101922"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638324000018/pdfft?md5=da7a645ccaf0b04cf70d1c36e8840437&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638324000018-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139436617","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of caregiver inhibitory control on infant visual working memory 照顾者的抑制控制对婴儿视觉工作记忆的影响
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-11 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101921
Christina Davidson, Aimee Theyer, Ghada Amaireh, Sobanawartiny Wijeakumar

Visual working memory (VWM) emerges in the first year of life and has far-reaching implications for academic and later life outcomes. Given that caregivers play a significant role in shaping cognitive function in children, it is important to understand how they might impact VWM development as early as infancy. The current study investigated whether caregivers’ efficiency of regulating inhibitory control was associated with VWM function in their infants. Eighty-eight caregivers were presented with a Go-NoGo task to assess inhibitory control. An efficiency score was calculated using their behavioural responses. Eighty-six 6-to-10-month-old infants were presented with a preferential looking task to assess VWM function. VWM load was manipulated across one (low load), two (medium load) and three (high load) items. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy was used to record brain activation from caregivers and their infants. We found no direct association between caregiver efficiency and infant VWM behaviour. However, we found an indirect association - caregiver efficiency was linked to infant VWM through left-lateralized fronto-parietal engagement. Specifically, infants with low efficiency caregivers showed decreasing left-lateralized parietal engagement with increasing VWM performance at the medium and high loads compared to infants with high efficiency caregivers, who did not show any load- or performance-dependent modulation. Our findings contribute to a growing body of literature examining the role that caregivers play in early neurocognitive development.

视觉工作记忆(VWM)出现于儿童出生后的第一年,对其学业和日后的生活有着深远的影响。鉴于照顾者在塑造儿童认知功能方面扮演着重要角色,了解他们如何影响婴儿时期的视觉工作记忆发展就显得尤为重要。本研究调查了照顾者调节抑制控制的效率是否与婴儿的VWM功能有关。研究人员向 88 名照顾者布置了一项 Go-NoGo 任务,以评估其抑制控制能力。根据他们的行为反应计算出效率分数。86 名 6 至 10 个月大的婴儿接受了优先寻找任务,以评估 VWM 功能。VWM 负荷在一个(低负荷)、两个(中等负荷)和三个(高负荷)项目中进行调节。我们使用功能性近红外光谱记录了照顾者及其婴儿的大脑激活情况。我们发现,照顾者的效率与婴儿的 VWM 行为之间没有直接联系。但是,我们发现了一种间接的关联--照顾者的效率通过左侧前顶叶的参与与婴儿的暴力记忆行为相关联。具体来说,在中负荷和高负荷下,低效能照料者的婴儿表现出左侧顶叶参与度的下降,而高能效照料者的婴儿则没有表现出任何与负荷或表现相关的调节。越来越多的文献研究了照料者在婴儿早期神经认知发展中所扮演的角色,我们的研究结果为这些文献的研究做出了贡献。
{"title":"The impact of caregiver inhibitory control on infant visual working memory","authors":"Christina Davidson,&nbsp;Aimee Theyer,&nbsp;Ghada Amaireh,&nbsp;Sobanawartiny Wijeakumar","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101921","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101921","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Visual working memory (VWM) emerges in the first year of life and has far-reaching implications for academic and later life outcomes. Given that caregivers play a significant role in shaping cognitive function in children, it is important to understand how they might impact VWM development as early as infancy. The current study investigated whether caregivers’ efficiency of regulating inhibitory control was associated with VWM function in their infants. Eighty-eight caregivers were presented with a Go-NoGo task to assess inhibitory control. An efficiency score was calculated using their behavioural responses. Eighty-six 6-to-10-month-old infants were presented with a preferential looking task to assess VWM function. VWM load was manipulated across one (low load), two (medium load) and three (high load) items. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy was used to record brain activation from caregivers and their infants. We found no direct association between caregiver efficiency and infant VWM behaviour. However, we found an indirect association - caregiver efficiency was linked to infant VWM through left-lateralized fronto-parietal engagement. Specifically, infants with low efficiency caregivers showed decreasing left-lateralized parietal engagement with increasing VWM performance at the medium and high loads compared to infants with high efficiency caregivers, who did not show any load- or performance-dependent modulation. Our findings contribute to a growing body of literature examining the role that caregivers play in early neurocognitive development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101921"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638323001133/pdfft?md5=0c3ba700c3bd37373bd6f652babbfd64&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638323001133-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139419070","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Finding a secure base: Exploring children’s attachment behaviors with professional caregivers during the first months of daycare 寻找安全基地探索儿童在入托前几个月与专业保育员的依恋行为
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101919
Alessia Macagno, Paola Molina

Recent decades have seen a major rise in demand for daycare services for children aged 0 to 3 years, and this has increased research interest in the child-professional caregiver relationship at daycare centers: How does the relationship between children and their new caregivers develop over time? How long does it take for children to settle in at daycare? What variables can influence the settling-in process? These questions are all of the utmost salience and bear crucial implications for children, parents, and daycare practitioners. In this study, we set out to explore the relationship between infants and their new caregivers over the first two months in daycare, using the Professional Caregiver Attachment Diary. The study involved seven Italian daycare centres and 55 professional caregivers, who observed 148 children (M=17.8 months). The children’s attachment behaviors were assessed at three time-points: when the children started attending daycare (T1), one month later (T2), and two months later (T3). We found that positive attachment behaviors (Secure and Non-Distressed) increased over time, whereas insecure behaviors (Avoidant and Resistant) decreased. Most of the change took place during the first month. Furthermore, children who had attended more daycare more regularly (with fewer days of absence) displayed fewer avoidant behaviors and a more rapid decrease in resistant behaviors than did children who were absent more frequently. The findings suggest that the PCAD may be usefully deployed to observe and analyze children while they are settling into a new daycare setting, especially in relation to their exploratory behaviors.

近几十年来,0 至 3 岁儿童对日托服务的需求大幅增加,这也增加了对日托中心儿童与专业保育员关系的研究兴趣:随着时间的推移,儿童与新保育员之间的关系如何发展?儿童在日托中心安顿下来需要多长时间?哪些变量会影响适应过程?这些问题都极为重要,对儿童、家长和托儿所从业人员都有重要影响。在这项研究中,我们使用 "专业保育员依恋日记"(Professional Caregiver Attachment Diary)来探讨婴儿在入托的头两个月中与新保育员之间的关系。这项研究涉及 7 个意大利日托中心和 55 名专业保育员,他们观察了 148 名儿童(M=17.8 个月)。在三个时间点对儿童的依恋行为进行了评估:儿童开始上托儿所时(T1)、一个月后(T2)和两个月后(T3)。我们发现,随着时间的推移,积极的依恋行为(安全型和非受压型)有所增加,而不安全的行为(回避型和抗拒型)则有所减少。大部分变化发生在第一个月。此外,与缺席次数较多的儿童相比,经常参加日托(缺席天数较少)的儿童表现出的回避行为较少,抵触行为减少得更快。研究结果表明,PCAD 可用于观察和分析儿童在适应新托儿所环境时的表现,尤其是他们的探索行为。
{"title":"Finding a secure base: Exploring children’s attachment behaviors with professional caregivers during the first months of daycare","authors":"Alessia Macagno,&nbsp;Paola Molina","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101919","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101919","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Recent decades have seen a major rise in demand for daycare services for children aged 0 to 3 years, and this has increased research interest in the child-professional caregiver relationship at daycare centers: How does the relationship between children and their new caregivers develop over time? How long does it take for children to settle in at daycare? What variables can influence the settling-in process? These questions are all of the utmost salience and bear crucial implications for children, parents, and daycare practitioners. In this study, we set out to explore the relationship between infants and their new caregivers over the first two months in daycare, using the <em>Professional Caregiver Attachment Diary</em>. The study involved seven Italian daycare centres and 55 professional caregivers, who observed 148 children (<em>M</em>=17.8 months). The children’s attachment behaviors were assessed at three time-points: when the children started attending daycare (T1), one month later (T2), and two months later (T3). We found that positive attachment behaviors (<em>Secure</em> and <em>Non-Distressed</em>) increased over time, whereas insecure behaviors (<em>Avoidant</em> and <em>Resistant</em>) decreased. Most of the change took place during the first month. Furthermore, children who had attended more daycare more regularly (with fewer days of absence) displayed fewer avoidant behaviors and a more rapid decrease in resistant behaviors than did children who were absent more frequently. The findings suggest that the PCAD may be usefully deployed to observe and analyze children while they are settling into a new daycare setting, especially in relation to their exploratory behaviors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101919"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S016363832300111X/pdfft?md5=e08774491853108c84b6bf9dcaa2e477&pid=1-s2.0-S016363832300111X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139399317","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When crying turns to hitting: Examining maternal responses to negative affect 当哭泣变成殴打研究母亲对负面情绪的反应
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101918
Brooke Edelman , Tamara Del Vecchio

Physical aggression in toddlerhood is empirically linked to anger and often conceptualized as a byproduct of frustration and related negative affect. Further, parenting is the major environmental construct implicated in the development of aggressive behaviors. Given parents’ role as “external regulators,” parents’ responses to their toddlers’ negative affect may serve to escalate or de-escalate their toddlers’ affective experience, thereby impacting the likelihood of subsequent aggression. In the present study, we examined whether parents’ negative affect, harsh, soothing, and distracting responses to their toddlers’ negative affect mediated the relation between toddlers’ negative affect and their aggressive behavior in brief conflict episodes. During a laboratory visit, a community sample of 69 mother-toddler dyads was observed in a structured interaction task. We found that child negative affect is associated with subsequent aggressive behavior by way of maternal harsh responses to negative affect. Negative emotional expression, soothing, and distraction neither facilitated or hindered children’s escalation from negative affect to aggression. Our findings support a dyadic intervention in which patterns of coercive parent-child interactions are targets for prevention and intervention of toddler aggression.

根据经验,幼儿期的身体攻击行为与愤怒有关,通常被认为是挫折和相关负面情绪的副产品。此外,父母的教养方式是与攻击行为的发展相关的主要环境因素。鉴于父母作为 "外部调节者 "的角色,父母对幼儿负面情绪的反应可能会使幼儿的情绪体验升级或降级,从而影响其随后发生攻击行为的可能性。在本研究中,我们考察了父母对幼儿负面情绪的反应、严厉反应、安抚反应和分散注意力反应是否对幼儿的负面情绪与他们在短暂冲突事件中的攻击行为之间的关系起到了中介作用。在一次实验室访问中,我们对 69 个社区样本中的母亲和幼儿组合进行了结构化互动任务观察。我们发现,通过母亲对负面情绪的严厉反应,儿童的负面情绪与随后的攻击行为有关。负面情绪表达、安抚和转移注意力既不会促进也不会阻碍儿童从负面情绪升级到攻击行为。我们的研究结果支持以胁迫性亲子互动模式作为预防和干预幼儿攻击行为的目标的二元干预。
{"title":"When crying turns to hitting: Examining maternal responses to negative affect","authors":"Brooke Edelman ,&nbsp;Tamara Del Vecchio","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101918","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101918","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Physical aggression in toddlerhood is empirically linked to anger and often conceptualized as a byproduct of frustration and related negative affect. Further, parenting is the major environmental construct implicated in the development of aggressive behaviors. Given parents’ role as “external regulators,” parents’ responses to their toddlers’ negative affect may serve to escalate or de-escalate their toddlers’ affective experience, thereby impacting the likelihood of subsequent aggression. In the present study, we examined whether parents’ negative affect, harsh, soothing, and distracting responses to their toddlers’ negative affect mediated the relation between toddlers’ negative affect and their aggressive behavior in brief conflict episodes. During a laboratory visit, a community sample of 69 mother-toddler dyads was observed in a structured interaction task. We found that child negative affect is associated with subsequent aggressive behavior by way of maternal harsh responses to negative affect. Negative emotional expression, soothing, and distraction neither facilitated or hindered children’s escalation from negative affect to aggression. Our findings support a dyadic intervention in which patterns of coercive parent-child interactions are targets for prevention and intervention of toddler aggression.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101918"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139399318","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preterm toddlers’ joint attention characteristics during dyadic interactions with their mothers and fathers compared to full-term toddlers at age 2 years 早产幼儿与两岁足月幼儿在与母亲和父亲的二人互动中的共同注意特点比较
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101915
Merve Ataman-Devrim , Jean Quigley , Elizabeth Nixon

The current study investigates Joint Attention (JA) characteristics (duration, frequency, source of initiation, type of JA, agent of termination, missed and unsuccessful episodes) in preterm and full-term toddlers’ interactions with their mothers and fathers, separately. Thirty-one singleton full-term (Mage = 24.07 months, SD = 1.45; 13 boys) and 17 singleton preterm toddlers (Madjustedage = 24.72 months, SD = 3.39; 12 boys) participated in the study with both parents. JA episodes were examined during dyadic five-minute free play sessions, were coded second-by-second, and were analysed using two-way mixed ANOVAs. Although the total amount of time spent in JA was not significantly different between the preterm and the full-term groups, JA episodes were more frequent, specifically supported JA episodes, and were more often terminated by the child during parent-preterm toddler interactions. Moreover, preterm toddlers missed their fathers’ attempts for JA more often than their mothers’ and more often than full-term toddlers missed their fathers’ and mothers’ bids for JA. Further, regardless of the birth status, toddlers initiated more JA with mothers than fathers, and fathers redirected their child’s attention to initiate JA more than mothers. Findings indicate that preterm toddlers may struggle to respond to JA bids, especially with their fathers, and to sustain their attention on a specific object or event during interactions. Preterm toddlers may need more support to engage in JA relative to their full-term peers, and redirecting attention strategy may not be optimal for them. Also, toddlers’ JA interactions may be different with their mothers and fathers. Findings contribute to the literature by demonstrating preterm toddlers’ JA characteristics with both parents compared to full-term toddlers at age two.

本研究分别调查了早产儿和足月幼儿在与母亲和父亲互动时的联合注意(JA)特征(持续时间、频率、启动源、JA类型、终止因素、错过和不成功事件)。31名单胎足月儿(Mage = 24.07个月,SD = 1.45;13名男孩)和17名单胎早产儿(Madjustedage = 24.72个月,SD = 3.39;12名男孩)与父母双方一起参与了研究。JA 事件是在 5 分钟的双亲自由游戏过程中进行的,逐秒进行编码,并使用双向混合方差分析。虽然早产儿和足月儿在联合行动中所花费的总时间没有显著差异,但在父母与早产儿的互动中,早产儿的联合行动更频繁,特别是支持性联合行动,而且更经常被孩子终止。此外,早产幼儿错过父亲尝试 JA 的次数多于母亲,错过父亲和母亲请求 JA 的次数也多于足月幼儿。此外,无论出生状况如何,幼儿主动与母亲进行的联合行动多于主动与父亲进行的联合行动,而且父亲比母亲更多地将孩子的注意力转向主动与母亲进行的联合行动。研究结果表明,早产幼儿可能很难对联合行动的请求做出反应,尤其是对父亲的请求,也很难在互动过程中将注意力持续放在特定的物体或事件上。与足月幼儿相比,早产幼儿可能需要更多的支持才能参与联合行动,而重新引导注意力的策略对他们来说可能不是最佳选择。此外,幼儿与母亲和父亲之间的联合行动互动也可能不同。研究结果表明,与足月幼儿相比,早产幼儿在两岁时与父母双方的联合行动具有不同的特点,从而为相关文献做出了贡献。
{"title":"Preterm toddlers’ joint attention characteristics during dyadic interactions with their mothers and fathers compared to full-term toddlers at age 2 years","authors":"Merve Ataman-Devrim ,&nbsp;Jean Quigley ,&nbsp;Elizabeth Nixon","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101915","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101915","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The current study investigates Joint Attention (JA) characteristics (duration, frequency, source of initiation, type of JA, agent of termination, missed and unsuccessful episodes) in preterm and full-term toddlers’ interactions with their mothers and fathers, separately. Thirty-one singleton full-term (<em>M</em>age = 24.07 months, <em>SD</em> = 1.45; 13 boys) and 17 singleton preterm toddlers (<em>M</em>adjustedage = 24.72 months, <em>SD</em> = 3.39; 12 boys) participated in the study with both parents. JA episodes were examined during dyadic five-minute free play sessions, were coded second-by-second, and were analysed using two-way mixed ANOVAs. Although the total amount of time spent in JA was not significantly different between the preterm and the full-term groups, JA episodes were more frequent, specifically supported JA episodes, and were more often terminated by the child during parent-preterm toddler interactions. Moreover, preterm toddlers missed their fathers’ attempts for JA more often than their mothers’ and more often than full-term toddlers missed their fathers’ and mothers’ bids for JA. Further, regardless of the birth status, toddlers initiated more JA with mothers than fathers, and fathers redirected their child’s attention to initiate JA more than mothers. Findings indicate that preterm toddlers may struggle to respond to JA bids, especially with their fathers, and to sustain their attention on a specific object or event during interactions. Preterm toddlers may need more support to engage in JA relative to their full-term peers, and redirecting attention strategy may not be optimal for them. Also, toddlers’ JA interactions may be different with their mothers and fathers. Findings contribute to the literature by demonstrating preterm toddlers’ JA characteristics with both parents compared to full-term toddlers at age two.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101915"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638323001078/pdfft?md5=9f4c857d956cc031de35ec5e16b6a7c7&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638323001078-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139072446","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of maternal gaze responsiveness on infants’ gaze following and later vocabulary development 母亲的凝视反应能力对婴儿的凝视跟踪和日后词汇发展的影响。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101917
Eugenia Wildt, Katharina J. Rohlfing

Research has shown that infants’ language development is influenced by their gaze following—an ability linked to their cognitive and social development. Following social learning approaches, this pilot study explored whether variations in gaze following and later vocabulary scores relate to early mother–infant interactions by focusing on the role of mothers’ gaze responsiveness in infants’ attentional and language development. We recruited 15 mother–child pairs in Poland and assessed their engagement in joint attention episodes. Results indicate that mothers foster their infants' gaze-following ability by providing them with numerous opportunities to participate in the task. We also confirmed a correlation between infants’ gaze-following ability at 6 months and their vocabulary scores at 24 months. However, combining both infants’ gaze following and mothers’ gaze monitoring as predictors in one model revealed that maternal gaze monitoring was a stronger predictor of infants’ later vocabulary growth. Overall, this study emphasizes that mothers’ gaze responsiveness is a crucial feature of scaffolding that impacts on infants’ gaze following and language development.

研究表明,婴儿的语言发展受其目光追随能力的影响--这种能力与婴儿的认知和社会发展息息相关。根据社会学习方法,这项试验性研究通过关注母亲的注视反应能力在婴儿的注意力和语言发展中的作用,探讨了注视跟随的变化和日后的词汇得分是否与早期的母婴互动有关。我们在波兰招募了 15 对母婴,并评估了他们参与联合注意力事件的情况。结果表明,母亲通过为婴儿提供大量参与任务的机会来培养他们的目光追随能力。我们还证实,婴儿 6 个月时的注视跟踪能力与他们 24 个月时的词汇量得分之间存在相关性。然而,将婴儿的注视跟踪能力和母亲的注视监控能力作为一个预测因素结合在一个模型中,结果显示,母亲的注视监控能力对婴儿日后的词汇量增长有更强的预测作用。总之,本研究强调,母亲的注视反应能力是影响婴儿注视跟随和语言发展的重要支架特征。
{"title":"The impact of maternal gaze responsiveness on infants’ gaze following and later vocabulary development","authors":"Eugenia Wildt,&nbsp;Katharina J. Rohlfing","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101917","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101917","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Research has shown that infants’ language development is influenced by their gaze following—an ability linked to their cognitive and social development. Following social learning approaches, this pilot study explored whether variations in gaze following and later vocabulary scores relate to early mother–infant interactions by focusing on the role of mothers’ gaze responsiveness in infants’ attentional and language development. We recruited 15 mother–child pairs in Poland and assessed their engagement in joint attention episodes. Results indicate that mothers foster their infants' gaze-following ability by providing them with numerous opportunities to participate in the task. We also confirmed a correlation between infants’ gaze-following ability at 6 months and their vocabulary scores at 24 months. However, combining both infants’ gaze following and mothers’ gaze monitoring as predictors in one model revealed that maternal gaze monitoring was a stronger predictor of infants’ later vocabulary growth. Overall, this study emphasizes that mothers’ gaze responsiveness is a crucial feature of scaffolding that impacts on infants’ gaze following and language development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101917"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638323001091/pdfft?md5=46fe8f4eae451d764b608b54f4e9b1a7&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638323001091-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138886828","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Predictors of executive function among 2 years old from a Thai birth cohort 泰国出生队列中两岁儿童执行功能的预测因素
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101916
Pimjuta Nimmapirat , Nancy Fiedler , Panrapee Suttiwan , Margaret Wolan Sullivan , Pamela Ohman-Strickland , Parinya Panuwet , Dana Boyd Barr , Tippawan Prapamontol , Warangkana Naksen , for the SAWASDEE birth cohort investigative team

Executive function (EF) is a critical skill for academic achievement. Research on the psychosocial and environmental predictors of EF, particularly among Southeast Asian, agricultural, and low income/rural populations, is limited. Our longitudinal study explored the influence of agricultural environmental, psychosocial, and temperamental factors on children’s emerging EF. Three-hundred and nine farm worker women were recruited during the first trimester of pregnancy. We evaluated the effects of prenatal insecticide exposure and psychosocial factors on “cool” (i.e., cognitive: A-not-B task, looking version) and “hot” EF (i.e., affective, response inhibition) measures of emerging EF. Maternal urine samples were collected monthly during pregnancy, composited, and analyzed for dialkylphosphate (DAP) metabolites of organophosphate insecticides. Psychosocial factors included socioeconomic status, maternal psychological factors, and quality of mother-child behavioral interactions. Backward stepwise regressions evaluated predictors of children’s EF at 12 (N = 288), 18 (N = 277) and 24 (N = 280) months of age. We observed different predictive models for cool EF, as measured by A-not-B task, vs. hot EF, as measured by response inhibition tasks. Report of housing quality as a surrogate for income was a significant predictor of emerging EF. However, these variables had opposite effects for cool vs. hot EF. More financial resources predicted better cool EF performance but poorer hot EF performance. Qualitative findings indicate that homes with fewer resources were in tribal areas where children must remain close to an adult for safety reasons. This finding suggests that challenging physical environments (e.g., an elevated bamboo home with no electricity or running water), may contribute to development of higher levels of response inhibition through parental socialization methods that emphasize compliance. Children who tended to show more arousal and excitability, and joy reactivity as young infants in the laboratory setting had better cognitive performance. In contrast, maternal emotional availability was a significant predictor of hot EF. As expected, increased maternal exposure to pesticides during pregnancy was associated with worse cognitive performance but was not associated with inhibitory control. Identifying risk factors contributing to the differential developmental pathways of cool and hot EF will inform prevention strategies to promote healthy development in this and other unstudied rural, low income Southeast Asian farming communities.

执行功能(EF)是学术成就的关键技能。关于EF的社会心理和环境预测因素的研究,特别是在东南亚、农业和低收入/农村人口中,是有限的。我们的纵向研究探讨了农业环境、社会心理和气质因素对儿童出现EF的影响。在怀孕的前三个月招募了309名农场女工。我们评估了产前杀虫剂暴露和社会心理因素对新兴EF的“冷”(即认知:A-not-B任务,外观版本)和“热”(即情感,反应抑制)测量的影响。妊娠期间每月采集产妇尿液样本,合成并分析有机磷杀虫剂的二烷基磷酸(DAP)代谢物。社会心理因素包括社会经济地位、母亲心理因素和母子行为互动质量。通过反向逐步回归评估儿童在12 (N = 288)、18 (N = 277)和24 (N = 280)月龄时EF的预测因子。我们观察到冷EF和热EF的不同预测模型,前者通过a -非- b任务测量,后者通过反应抑制任务测量。报告住房质量作为收入的替代品是新兴EF的重要预测因子。然而,这些变量对冷EF和热EF有相反的影响。更多的财政资源预示着较好的冷EF绩效,但较差的热EF绩效。定性调查结果表明,资源较少的家庭位于部落地区,那里的儿童出于安全原因必须与成年人待在一起。这一发现表明,具有挑战性的物理环境(例如,没有电或自来水的高架竹屋)可能有助于通过强调依从性的父母社会化方法发展更高水平的反应抑制。那些在实验室环境中表现出更多兴奋、兴奋和快乐反应的儿童,他们的认知表现更好。母亲情绪可得性是热EF的显著预测因子。正如预期的那样,孕妇在怀孕期间接触杀虫剂的增加与认知能力下降有关,但与抑制控制无关。确定导致冷热EF不同发展途径的风险因素将为预防策略提供信息,以促进该地区和其他未研究的农村低收入东南亚农业社区的健康发展。
{"title":"Predictors of executive function among 2 years old from a Thai birth cohort","authors":"Pimjuta Nimmapirat ,&nbsp;Nancy Fiedler ,&nbsp;Panrapee Suttiwan ,&nbsp;Margaret Wolan Sullivan ,&nbsp;Pamela Ohman-Strickland ,&nbsp;Parinya Panuwet ,&nbsp;Dana Boyd Barr ,&nbsp;Tippawan Prapamontol ,&nbsp;Warangkana Naksen ,&nbsp;for the SAWASDEE birth cohort investigative team","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101916","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101916","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span><span>Executive function (EF) is a critical skill for academic achievement. Research on the psychosocial and environmental predictors of EF, particularly among </span>Southeast Asian, agricultural, and low income/rural populations, is limited. Our longitudinal study explored the influence of agricultural environmental, psychosocial, and temperamental factors on children’s emerging EF. Three-hundred and nine farm worker women were recruited during the first trimester of pregnancy. We evaluated the effects of prenatal insecticide exposure and psychosocial factors on “cool” (i.e., cognitive: A-not-B task, looking version) and “hot” EF (i.e., affective, response inhibition) measures of emerging EF. Maternal urine samples were collected monthly during pregnancy, composited, and analyzed for dialkylphosphate (DAP) metabolites of </span>organophosphate insecticides<span>. Psychosocial factors included socioeconomic status, maternal psychological factors, and quality of mother-child behavioral interactions. Backward stepwise regressions evaluated predictors of children’s EF at 12 (N = 288), 18 (N = 277) and 24 (N = 280) months of age. We observed different predictive models for cool EF, as measured by A-not-B task, vs. hot EF, as measured by response inhibition tasks. Report of housing quality as a surrogate for income was a significant predictor of emerging EF. However, these variables had opposite effects for cool vs. hot EF. More financial resources predicted better cool EF performance but poorer hot EF performance. Qualitative findings indicate that homes with fewer resources were in tribal areas where children must remain close to an adult for safety reasons. This finding suggests that challenging physical environments (e.g., an elevated bamboo home with no electricity or running water), may contribute to development of higher levels of response inhibition through parental socialization<span> methods that emphasize compliance. Children who tended to show more arousal and excitability, and joy reactivity as young infants in the laboratory setting had better cognitive performance. In contrast, maternal emotional availability was a significant predictor of hot EF. As expected, increased maternal exposure to pesticides during pregnancy was associated with worse cognitive performance but was not associated with inhibitory control. Identifying risk factors contributing to the differential developmental pathways of cool and hot EF will inform prevention strategies to promote healthy development in this and other unstudied rural, low income Southeast Asian farming communities.</span></span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101916"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138656670","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Touchscreens can promote infant object-interlocutor reference switching 触摸屏可促进婴儿的对象-对话者参照切换
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101914
Kimberley M. Hudspeth, Charlie Lewis

We re-examine whether the type of object played with influences parent-infant joint attention. A within-participants comparison of 24 parent-9-month-old dyads, used head-mounted eye-tracking to measure parental naming and infant attention during play with touchscreen apps on a touchscreen tablet or matched interactive toys. Infants engaged in sustained attention more to the toy than the tablet. Parents named objects less in toy play. Infants exhibited more gaze shifts between the object and their parent during tablet play. Contrasting previous studies, these findings suggest that joint tablet play can be more interactive than with toys, and raise questions about the recommendation that infants should not be exposed at all to such technology.

我们重新研究了玩耍对象的类型是否会影响父母和婴儿的共同注意力。我们对 24 名父母和 9 个月大的婴儿进行了参与者内比较,使用头戴式眼动跟踪仪测量了父母在触摸屏平板电脑上玩触摸屏应用程序或玩匹配的互动玩具时的命名和婴儿的注意力。婴儿对玩具的持续注意力高于平板电脑。在玩具游戏中,父母对物体的命名较少。在平板电脑游戏中,婴儿在物体和父母之间表现出更多的视线转移。与之前的研究相反,这些研究结果表明,与玩具相比,共同玩平板电脑的互动性更强,这也让人们对 "完全不应该让婴儿接触此类技术 "的建议产生了疑问。
{"title":"Touchscreens can promote infant object-interlocutor reference switching","authors":"Kimberley M. Hudspeth,&nbsp;Charlie Lewis","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101914","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101914","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We re-examine whether the type of object played with influences parent-infant joint attention. A within-participants comparison of 24 parent-9-month-old dyads, used head-mounted eye-tracking to measure parental naming and infant attention during play with touchscreen apps on a touchscreen tablet or matched interactive toys. Infants engaged in sustained attention more to the toy than the tablet. Parents named objects less in toy play. Infants exhibited more gaze shifts between the object and their parent during tablet play. Contrasting previous studies, these findings suggest that joint tablet play can be more interactive than with toys, and raise questions about the recommendation that infants should not be exposed at all to such technology.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101914"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138549703","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
It takes a village: Caregiver diversity and language contingency in the UK and rural Gambia 需要一个村庄英国和冈比亚农村地区护理人员的多样性和语言应急能力
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101913
Laura Katus , Maria M. Crespo-Llado , Bosiljka Milosavljevic , Mariama Saidykhan , Omar Njie , Tijan Fadera , Samantha McCann , Lena Acolatse , Marta Perapoch Amadó , Maria Rozhko , Sophie E. Moore , Clare E. Elwell , Sarah Lloyd-Fox , The BRIGHT Project Team

Introduction

There is substantial diversity within and between contexts globally in caregiving practices and family composition, which may have implications for the early interaction’s infants engage in. We draw on data from the [blinded] project, which longitudinally examined infants in the UK and in rural Gambia, West Africa. In The Gambia, households are commonly characterized by multigenerational, frequently polygamous family structures, which, in part, is reflected in the diversity of caregivers a child spends time with. In this paper, we aim to 1) evaluate and validate the Language Environment Analysis (LENA) for use in the Mandinka speaking families in The Gambia, 2) examine the nature (i.e., prevalence of turn taking) and amount (i.e., adult and child vocalizations) of conversation that infants are exposed to from 12 to 24 months of age and 3) investigate the link between caregiver diversity and child language outcomes, examining the mediating role of contingent turn taking.

Method

We obtained naturalistic seven-hour-long LENA recordings at 12, 18 and 24 months of age from a cohort of N = 204 infants from Mandinka speaking households in The Gambia and N = 61 infants in the UK. We examined developmental changes and site differences in LENA counts of adult word counts (AWC), contingent turn taking (CTT) and child vocalizations (CVC). In the larger and more heterogenous Gambian sample, we also investigated caregiver predictors of turn taking frequency. We hereby examined the number of caregivers present over the recording day and the consistency of caregivers across two subsequent days per age point. We controlled for children’s cognitive development via the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL).

Results

Our LENA validation showed high internal consistency between the human coders and automated LENA outputs (Cronbach’s alpha’s all >.8). All LENA counts were higher in the UK compared to the Gambian cohort. In The Gambia, controlling for overall neurodevelopment via the MSEL, CTT at 12 and 18 months predicted CVC at 18 and 24 months. Caregiver consistency was associated with CTT counts at 18 and 24 months. The number of caregivers and CTT counts showed an inverted u-shape relationship at 18 and 24 months, with an intermediate number of caregivers being associated with the highest CTT frequencies. Mediation analyses showed a partial mediation by number of caregivers and CTT and 24-month CVC.

Discussion

The LENA provided reliable estimates for the Mandinka language in the home recording context. We showed that turn taking is associated with subsequent child vocalizations and explored contextual caregiving factors contributing to turn taking in the Gambian cohort.

导言:在全球范围内,不同环境下的照顾方式和家庭组成存在很大差异,这可能会对婴儿参与的早期互动产生影响。我们借鉴了[盲法]项目的数据,该项目对英国和西非冈比亚农村地区的婴儿进行了纵向研究。在冈比亚,多代同堂、一夫多妻制的家庭结构是常见的家庭结构特征,这在一定程度上反映在与孩子相处的照顾者的多样性上。在本文中,我们的目标是:1)评估和验证在冈比亚曼丁卡语家庭中使用的语言环境分析(LENA);2)研究谈话的性质(即轮流发言的普遍性)和数量(即成人和儿童的发声)、我们从冈比亚曼丁卡语家庭的 N = 204 名婴儿和英国的 N = 61 名婴儿中获得了 12、18 和 24 个月大时长达 7 小时的 LENA 自然录音。我们研究了成人词数 (AWC)、应急轮流 (CTT) 和儿童发声 (CVC) 的 LENA 计数的发展变化和地点差异。在规模更大、异质性更强的冈比亚样本中,我们还调查了轮流发言频率的预测因素。在此,我们考察了记录当天在场的照料者人数,以及每个年龄点的照料者在随后两天中的一致性。我们通过穆伦早期学习量表(MSEL)对儿童的认知发展进行了控制。结果我们的 LENA 验证表明,人工编码者和自动 LENA 输出之间具有很高的内部一致性(Cronbach's alpha's all >.8)。与冈比亚队列相比,英国的所有 LENA 计数都更高。在冈比亚,在通过 MSEL 控制整体神经发育的情况下,12 个月和 18 个月的 CTT 预测了 18 个月和 24 个月的 CVC。照护者的一致性与 18 和 24 个月时的 CTT 计数有关。在 18 和 24 个月时,照顾者人数与 CTT 计数呈倒 U 型关系,照顾者人数居中与 CTT 频率最高相关。中介分析表明,照顾者人数和 CTT 与 24 个月的 CVC 存在部分中介关系。我们的研究表明,轮流发声与儿童随后的发声有关,并探讨了在冈比亚队列中导致轮流发声的护理因素。
{"title":"It takes a village: Caregiver diversity and language contingency in the UK and rural Gambia","authors":"Laura Katus ,&nbsp;Maria M. Crespo-Llado ,&nbsp;Bosiljka Milosavljevic ,&nbsp;Mariama Saidykhan ,&nbsp;Omar Njie ,&nbsp;Tijan Fadera ,&nbsp;Samantha McCann ,&nbsp;Lena Acolatse ,&nbsp;Marta Perapoch Amadó ,&nbsp;Maria Rozhko ,&nbsp;Sophie E. Moore ,&nbsp;Clare E. Elwell ,&nbsp;Sarah Lloyd-Fox ,&nbsp;The BRIGHT Project Team","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101913","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101913","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>There is substantial diversity within and between contexts globally in caregiving practices and family composition, which may have implications for the early interaction’s infants engage in. We draw on data from the [blinded] project, which longitudinally examined infants in the UK and in rural Gambia, West Africa. In The Gambia, households are commonly characterized by multigenerational, frequently polygamous family structures, which, in part, is reflected in the diversity of caregivers a child spends time with. In this paper, we aim to 1) evaluate and validate the Language Environment Analysis (LENA) for use in the Mandinka speaking families in The Gambia, 2) examine the nature (i.e., prevalence of turn taking) and amount (i.e., adult and child vocalizations) of conversation that infants are exposed to from 12 to 24 months of age and 3) investigate the link between caregiver diversity and child language outcomes, examining the mediating role of contingent turn taking.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>We obtained naturalistic seven-hour-long LENA recordings at 12, 18 and 24 months of age from a cohort of N = 204 infants from Mandinka speaking households in The Gambia and N = 61 infants in the UK. We examined developmental changes and site differences in LENA counts of adult word counts (AWC), contingent turn taking (CTT) and child vocalizations (CVC). In the larger and more heterogenous Gambian sample, we also investigated caregiver predictors of turn taking frequency. We hereby examined the number of caregivers present over the recording day and the consistency of caregivers across two subsequent days per age point. We controlled for children’s cognitive development via the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL).</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Our LENA validation showed high internal consistency between the human coders and automated LENA outputs (Cronbach’s alpha’s all &gt;.8). All LENA counts were higher in the UK compared to the Gambian cohort. In The Gambia, controlling for overall neurodevelopment via the MSEL, CTT at 12 and 18 months predicted CVC at 18 and 24 months. Caregiver consistency was associated with CTT counts at 18 and 24 months. The number of caregivers and CTT counts showed an inverted u-shape relationship at 18 and 24 months, with an intermediate number of caregivers being associated with the highest CTT frequencies. Mediation analyses showed a partial mediation by number of caregivers and CTT and 24-month CVC.</p></div><div><h3>Discussion</h3><p>The LENA provided reliable estimates for the Mandinka language in the home recording context. We showed that turn taking is associated with subsequent child vocalizations and explored contextual caregiving factors contributing to turn taking in the Gambian cohort.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101913"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638323001054/pdfft?md5=6d39869a5d18eb72a123a4c5f85c87dd&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638323001054-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138490834","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infant Behavior & Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1