首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Science最新文献

英文 中文
High-Pitched Sound is Open and Low-Pitched Sound is Closed: Representing the Spatial Meaning of Pitch Height 高音是开放的,低音是封闭的:表现音高的空间意义。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13486
Lari Vainio, Ida-Lotta Myllylä, Alexandra Wikström, Martti Vainio

Research shows that high- and low-pitch sounds can be associated with various meanings. For example, high-pitch sounds are associated with small concepts, whereas low-pitch sounds are associated with large concepts. This study presents three experiments revealing that high-pitch sounds are also associated with open concepts and opening hand actions, while low-pitch sounds are associated with closed concepts and closing hand actions. In Experiment 1, this sound-meaning correspondence effect was shown using the two-alternative forced-choice task, while Experiments 2 and 3 used reaction time tasks to show this interaction. In Experiment 2, high-pitch vocalizations were found to facilitate opening hand gestures, and low-pitch vocalizations were found to facilitate closing hand gestures, when performed simultaneously. In Experiment 3, high-pitched vocalizations were produced particularly rapidly when the visual target stimulus presented an open object, and low-pitched vocalizations were produced particularly rapidly when the target presented a closed object. These findings are discussed concerning the meaning of intonational cues. They are suggested to be based on cross-modally representing conceptual spatial knowledge in sensory, motor, and affective systems. Additionally, this pitch-opening effect might share cognitive processes with other pitch-meaning effects.

研究表明,高音调和低音调的声音可以与不同的含义相关联。例如,高音调的声音与小概念相关,而低音调的声音与大概念相关。本研究通过三个实验揭示了高音调声音也与开放概念和张手动作相关,而低音调声音则与封闭概念和收手动作相关。在实验 1 中,这种音义对应效应是通过双选项强迫选择任务来体现的,而实验 2 和实验 3 则是通过反应时间任务来体现这种交互作用。在实验 2 中,发现同时进行高音调发声和低音调发声时,高音调发声有利于张开手势,而低音调发声有利于收拢手势。在实验 3 中,当视觉目标刺激呈现开放物体时,高音调发声尤其迅速,而当目标呈现封闭物体时,低音调发声尤其迅速。这些发现与语调线索的意义有关。这些发现被认为是基于在感觉、运动和情感系统中跨模态表现概念空间知识。此外,这种音调开阔效应可能与其他音调意义效应共享认知过程。
{"title":"High-Pitched Sound is Open and Low-Pitched Sound is Closed: Representing the Spatial Meaning of Pitch Height","authors":"Lari Vainio,&nbsp;Ida-Lotta Myllylä,&nbsp;Alexandra Wikström,&nbsp;Martti Vainio","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13486","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13486","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Research shows that high- and low-pitch sounds can be associated with various meanings. For example, high-pitch sounds are associated with small concepts, whereas low-pitch sounds are associated with large concepts. This study presents three experiments revealing that high-pitch sounds are also associated with open concepts and opening hand actions, while low-pitch sounds are associated with closed concepts and closing hand actions. In Experiment 1, this sound-meaning correspondence effect was shown using the two-alternative forced-choice task, while Experiments 2 and 3 used reaction time tasks to show this interaction. In Experiment 2, high-pitch vocalizations were found to facilitate opening hand gestures, and low-pitch vocalizations were found to facilitate closing hand gestures, when performed simultaneously. In Experiment 3, high-pitched vocalizations were produced particularly rapidly when the visual target stimulus presented an open object, and low-pitched vocalizations were produced particularly rapidly when the target presented a closed object. These findings are discussed concerning the meaning of intonational cues. They are suggested to be based on cross-modally representing conceptual spatial knowledge in sensory, motor, and affective systems. Additionally, this pitch-opening effect might share cognitive processes with other pitch-meaning effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13486","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142001072","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring How Generating Metaphor Via Insight Versus Analysis Affects Metaphor Quality and Learning Outcomes 探索通过洞察和分析产生隐喻如何影响隐喻质量和学习效果。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13488
Yuhua Yu, Lindsay Krebs, Mark Beeman, Vicky T. Lai

Metaphor generation is both a creative act and a means of learning. When learning a new concept, people often create a metaphor to connect the new concept to existing knowledge. Does the manner in which people generate a metaphor, via sudden insight (Aha! moment) or deliberate analysis, influence the quality of generation and subsequent learning outcomes? According to some research, deliberate processing enhances knowledge retention; hence, generation via analysis likely leads to better concept learning. However, other research has shown that solutions generated via insight are better remembered. In the current study, participants were presented with science concepts and descriptions, then generated metaphors for the concepts. They also indicated how they generated each metaphor and rated their metaphor for novelty and aptness. We assessed participants’ learning outcomes with a memory test and evaluated the creative quality of the metaphors based on self- and crowd-sourced ratings. Consistent with the deliberate processing benefit, participants became more familiar with the target science concept if they previously generated a metaphor for the concept via analysis compared to via insight. We also found that metaphors generated via analysis did not differ from metaphors generated via insight in quality (aptness or novelty) nor in how well they were remembered. However, participants’ self-evaluations of metaphors generated via insight showed more agreement with independent raters, suggesting the role of insight in modulating the creative ideation process. These preliminary findings have implications for understanding the nature of insight during idea generation and its impact on learning.

隐喻的产生既是一种创造行为,也是一种学习手段。在学习一个新概念时,人们通常会创造一个隐喻,将新概念与现有知识联系起来。人们产生隐喻的方式,是通过突然的洞察力("啊哈!"时刻)还是经过深思熟虑的分析,是否会影响隐喻产生的质量和随后的学习效果?一些研究表明,深思熟虑的处理方式会增强知识的保留;因此,通过分析产生隐喻可能会带来更好的概念学习。然而,其他研究表明,通过洞察力生成的解决方案记忆效果更好。在当前的研究中,研究人员向参与者展示了科学概念和描述,然后为概念生成隐喻。他们还说明了自己是如何产生每个隐喻的,并对隐喻的新颖性和恰当性进行了评分。我们通过记忆测试评估了参与者的学习成果,并根据自我评分和群众评分评估了隐喻的创意质量。与刻意加工的益处相一致的是,与洞察力相比,如果参与者之前通过分析为目标科学概念生成了隐喻,那么他们对该概念就会更加熟悉。我们还发现,通过分析产生的隐喻与通过洞察产生的隐喻在质量(恰当性或新颖性)和记忆程度上并无不同。然而,参与者对通过洞察力产生的隐喻的自我评价与独立评分者的一致程度更高,这表明洞察力在调节创造性构思过程中的作用。这些初步研究结果对于理解创意产生过程中洞察力的性质及其对学习的影响具有重要意义。
{"title":"Exploring How Generating Metaphor Via Insight Versus Analysis Affects Metaphor Quality and Learning Outcomes","authors":"Yuhua Yu,&nbsp;Lindsay Krebs,&nbsp;Mark Beeman,&nbsp;Vicky T. Lai","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13488","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13488","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Metaphor generation is both a creative act and a means of learning. When learning a new concept, people often create a metaphor to connect the new concept to existing knowledge. Does the manner in which people generate a metaphor, via sudden insight (Aha! moment) or deliberate analysis, influence the quality of generation and subsequent learning outcomes? According to some research, deliberate processing enhances knowledge retention; hence, generation via analysis likely leads to better concept learning. However, other research has shown that solutions generated via insight are better remembered. In the current study, participants were presented with science concepts and descriptions, then generated metaphors for the concepts. They also indicated how they generated each metaphor and rated their metaphor for novelty and aptness. We assessed participants’ learning outcomes with a memory test and evaluated the creative quality of the metaphors based on self- and crowd-sourced ratings. Consistent with the deliberate processing benefit, participants became more familiar with the target science concept if they previously generated a metaphor for the concept via analysis compared to via insight. We also found that metaphors generated via analysis did not differ from metaphors generated via insight in quality (aptness or novelty) nor in how well they were remembered. However, participants’ self-evaluations of metaphors generated via insight showed more agreement with independent raters, suggesting the role of insight in modulating the creative ideation process. These preliminary findings have implications for understanding the nature of insight during idea generation and its impact on learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13488","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142001071","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can Large Language Models Counter the Recent Decline in Literacy Levels? An Important Role for Cognitive Science 大型语言模型能否应对近期识字水平的下降?认知科学的重要作用。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13487
Falk Huettig, Morten H. Christiansen

Literacy is in decline in many parts of the world, accompanied by drops in associated cognitive skills (including IQ) and an increasing susceptibility to fake news. It is possible that the recent explosive growth and widespread deployment of Large Language Models (LLMs) might exacerbate this trend, but there is also a chance that LLMs can help turn things around. We argue that cognitive science is ideally suited to help steer future literacy development in the right direction by challenging and informing current educational practices and policy. Cognitive scientists have the right interdisciplinary skills to study, analyze, evaluate, and change LLMs to facilitate their critical use, to encourage turn-taking that promotes rather than hinders literacy, to support literacy acquisition in diverse and equitable ways, and to scaffold potential future changes in what it means to be literate. We urge cognitive scientists to take up this mantle—the future impact of LLMs on human literacy skills is too important to be left to the large, predominately U.S.-based tech companies.

在世界许多地方,识字率正在下降,伴随而来的是相关认知技能(包括智商)的下降和对假新闻的日益敏感。最近大语言模型(LLM)的爆炸式增长和广泛应用有可能加剧这一趋势,但也有可能帮助扭转局面。我们认为,认知科学非常适合通过挑战和指导当前的教育实践和政策,帮助引导未来的扫盲朝着正确的方向发展。认知科学家拥有适当的跨学科技能来研究、分析、评估和改变识字方法,以促进其批判性使用,鼓励促进而不是阻碍识字的轮流学习,以多样化和公平的方式支持识字学习,并为未来识字方法的潜在变革提供支架。我们敦促认知科学家承担起这一重任--未来扫盲对人类读写能力的影响太重要了,不能留给以美国为主的大型科技公司。
{"title":"Can Large Language Models Counter the Recent Decline in Literacy Levels? An Important Role for Cognitive Science","authors":"Falk Huettig,&nbsp;Morten H. Christiansen","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13487","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13487","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Literacy is in decline in many parts of the world, accompanied by drops in associated cognitive skills (including IQ) and an increasing susceptibility to fake news. It is possible that the recent explosive growth and widespread deployment of Large Language Models (LLMs) might exacerbate this trend, but there is also a chance that LLMs can help turn things around. We argue that cognitive science is ideally suited to help steer future literacy development in the right direction by challenging and informing current educational practices and policy. Cognitive scientists have the right interdisciplinary skills to study, analyze, evaluate, and change LLMs to facilitate their critical use, to encourage turn-taking that promotes rather than hinders literacy, to support literacy acquisition in diverse and equitable ways, and to scaffold potential future changes in what it means to be literate. We urge cognitive scientists to take up this mantle—the future impact of LLMs on human literacy skills is too important to be left to the large, predominately U.S.-based tech companies.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142001070","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lexical and Social Effects on the Learning and Integration of Inflectional Morphology 词汇和社会对学习和整合词法的影响
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-30 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13483
Péter Rácz, Ágnes Lukács

People learn language variation through exposure to linguistic interactions. The way we take part in these interactions is shaped by our lexical representations, the mechanisms of language processing, and the social context. Existing work has looked at how we learn and store variation in the ambient language. How this is mediated by the social context is less understood.

We report on the results of an innovative experimental battery designed to test how learning variation is affected by a variable's social indexicality. Hungarian native speakers played a co-operative game involving verb nonwords. These were built on existing inflectional variation in Hungarian. Participant behavior shifted in response to an automated co-player's preferences, and this reflected a change in the overall lexical patterns of the players, affected by the particular verbs introduced by the co-player. Patterns persisted in subsequent testing. Learning was similar for variables with or without social meaning.

Results show that participants can learn and retain a range of variable morphological patterns in a simulated interaction. Participants seem to have equal capacity to pick up variables with and without social meaning. This suggests that the social meaning of a pattern does not clearly constrain learning morphological variation and becomes relevant downstream in learning.

人们通过语言互动学习语言变体。我们参与这些互动的方式是由我们的词汇表征、语言处理机制和社会环境决定的。现有的工作研究了我们如何学习和存储环境语言中的变化。至于如何通过社会环境进行调解,目前还不太清楚。我们报告了一项创新实验的结果,该实验旨在测试学习变异如何受到变量社会索引性的影响。以匈牙利语为母语的人玩了一个涉及动词非词的合作游戏。这些非词建立在匈牙利语现有的词性变异基础上。参与者的行为会随着自动合作者的偏好而改变,这反映了参与者整体词汇模式的变化,并受到合作者引入的特定动词的影响。这种模式在随后的测试中持续存在。对于有无社会意义的变量,学习效果相似。结果表明,参与者可以在模拟互动中学习并保留一系列可变词形模式。参与者在学习具有和不具有社会意义的变量时似乎具有同等的能力。这表明,形态模式的社会意义并不会明显限制形态变异的学习,而是会在学习的下游发挥作用。
{"title":"Lexical and Social Effects on the Learning and Integration of Inflectional Morphology","authors":"Péter Rácz,&nbsp;Ágnes Lukács","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13483","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13483","url":null,"abstract":"<p>People learn language variation through exposure to linguistic interactions. The way we take part in these interactions is shaped by our lexical representations, the mechanisms of language processing, and the social context. Existing work has looked at how we learn and store variation in the ambient language. How this is mediated by the social context is less understood.</p><p>We report on the results of an innovative experimental battery designed to test how learning variation is affected by a variable's social indexicality. Hungarian native speakers played a co-operative game involving verb nonwords. These were built on existing inflectional variation in Hungarian. Participant behavior shifted in response to an automated co-player's preferences, and this reflected a change in the overall lexical patterns of the players, affected by the particular verbs introduced by the co-player. Patterns persisted in subsequent testing. Learning was similar for variables with or without social meaning.</p><p>Results show that participants can learn and retain a range of variable morphological patterns in a simulated interaction. Participants seem to have equal capacity to pick up variables with and without social meaning. This suggests that the social meaning of a pattern does not clearly constrain learning morphological variation and becomes relevant downstream in learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13483","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141856845","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Surface Cues Explain the Logic-Liking Effect in Disjunctions 表面线索解释了分词中的逻辑喜好效应
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13482
Constantin G. Meyer-Grant, Dorothea Poggel, Karl Christoph Klauer

The finding that people tend to prefer logically valid conclusions over invalid ones is known in the literature as the logic-liking effect and has traditionally been interpreted as evidence for the notion of so-called logical intuitions. Results of more recent empirical studies investigating conditional and categorical syllogisms suggest, however, that previous instances of the logic-liking effect can be accounted for by a confound in terms of surface-feature atmosphere. But the true nature of this atmosphere effect has so far remained largely elusive. Here, we address this issue and introduce two variants of disjunctive syllogisms that enable us to deconfound validity, possibility of the conclusion, and surface-feature atmosphere, which has been impossible with simple disjunctive syllogisms used in earlier studies. Three experiments, in which participants were asked to provide liking and logic ratings for these arguments, revealed that the logic-liking effect in disjunctive syllogisms can be explained by an atmosphere confound in combination with implied demand to consider logicality when judging likability. We also observed a strong atmosphere effect in logic ratings over and above an effect of logical validity per se. Furthermore, atmosphere effects appear to be induced only by specific surface features, namely those that are ecologically valid, if fallible, predictors for logicality. We conclude that acquired atmosphere heuristics provide proxies for logical validity that reasoners often take at face value. A comparison of the present results with previous findings from experiments that focused on conditional and categorical syllogisms additionally indicates that these atmosphere heuristics are used irrespective of an argument's complexity.

人们倾向于选择逻辑上有效的结论而不是无效的结论,这一发现在文献中被称为逻辑喜好效应,传统上被解释为所谓逻辑直觉概念的证据。然而,最近对条件式和分类式三段论的实证研究结果表明,以前的逻辑-喜欢效应可以通过表面-特征氛围的混淆来解释。但是,这种氛围效应的真正本质迄今为止仍然难以捉摸。在此,我们针对这一问题,引入了两种变体的分句三段论,使我们能够解除对有效性、结论的可能性和表面特征氛围的困惑,而这在以前的研究中使用简单的分句三段论是不可能实现的。在三个实验中,我们要求参与者对这些论证进行喜欢度和逻辑性评分,结果表明,逻辑性-喜欢度效应可以通过氛围混淆与在判断喜欢度时考虑逻辑性的隐含要求相结合来解释。我们还观察到,在逻辑评分中,除了逻辑有效性本身的影响之外,还有一种强烈的氛围效应。此外,气氛效应似乎只由特定的表面特征引起,即那些在生态学上有效的逻辑性预测因素,尽管这些预测因素有误。我们的结论是,习得的氛围启发法提供了逻辑有效性的代用指标,推理者通常会将其视为表面价值。将本研究结果与之前针对条件式和分类式三段论的实验结果进行比较,还表明无论论证的复杂程度如何,这些氛围启发法都会被使用。
{"title":"Surface Cues Explain the Logic-Liking Effect in Disjunctions","authors":"Constantin G. Meyer-Grant,&nbsp;Dorothea Poggel,&nbsp;Karl Christoph Klauer","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13482","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13482","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The finding that people tend to prefer logically valid conclusions over invalid ones is known in the literature as the logic-liking effect and has traditionally been interpreted as evidence for the notion of so-called logical intuitions. Results of more recent empirical studies investigating conditional and categorical syllogisms suggest, however, that previous instances of the logic-liking effect can be accounted for by a confound in terms of surface-feature atmosphere. But the true nature of this atmosphere effect has so far remained largely elusive. Here, we address this issue and introduce two variants of disjunctive syllogisms that enable us to deconfound validity, possibility of the conclusion, and surface-feature atmosphere, which has been impossible with simple disjunctive syllogisms used in earlier studies. Three experiments, in which participants were asked to provide liking and logic ratings for these arguments, revealed that the logic-liking effect in disjunctive syllogisms can be explained by an atmosphere confound in combination with implied demand to consider logicality when judging likability. We also observed a strong atmosphere effect in logic ratings over and above an effect of logical validity per se. Furthermore, atmosphere effects appear to be induced only by specific surface features, namely those that are ecologically valid, if fallible, predictors for logicality. We conclude that acquired atmosphere heuristics provide proxies for logical validity that reasoners often take at face value. A comparison of the present results with previous findings from experiments that focused on conditional and categorical syllogisms additionally indicates that these atmosphere heuristics are used irrespective of an argument's complexity.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13482","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141724795","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Introduction to Progress and Puzzles of Cognitive Science 认知科学的进步与困惑》导言:Wiley 虚拟期刊简介。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-12 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13480
Rick Dale, Ruth M. J. Byrne, Emma Cohen, Ophelia Deroy, Samuel J. Gershman, Janet H. Hsiao, Ping Li, Padraic Monaghan, David C. Noelle, Iris van Rooij, Priti Shah, Michael J. Spivey, Sashank Varma
{"title":"Introduction to Progress and Puzzles of Cognitive Science","authors":"Rick Dale,&nbsp;Ruth M. J. Byrne,&nbsp;Emma Cohen,&nbsp;Ophelia Deroy,&nbsp;Samuel J. Gershman,&nbsp;Janet H. Hsiao,&nbsp;Ping Li,&nbsp;Padraic Monaghan,&nbsp;David C. Noelle,&nbsp;Iris van Rooij,&nbsp;Priti Shah,&nbsp;Michael J. Spivey,&nbsp;Sashank Varma","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13480","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13480","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141601921","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Hierarchical Bayesian Model of Adaptive Teaching 自适应教学的层次贝叶斯模型
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-09 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13477
Alicia M. Chen, Andrew Palacci, Natalia Vélez, Robert D. Hawkins, Samuel J. Gershman

How do teachers learn about what learners already know? How do learners aid teachers by providing them with information about their background knowledge and what they find confusing? We formalize this collaborative reasoning process using a hierarchical Bayesian model of pedagogy. We then evaluate this model in two online behavioral experiments (N = 312 adults). In Experiment 1, we show that teachers select examples that account for learners' background knowledge, and adjust their examples based on learners' feedback. In Experiment 2, we show that learners strategically provide more feedback when teachers' examples deviate from their background knowledge. These findings provide a foundation for extending computational accounts of pedagogy to richer interactive settings.

教师如何了解学习者已经掌握的知识?学习者如何通过向教师提供他们的背景知识信息和他们感到困惑的知识来帮助教师?我们使用分层贝叶斯教学法模型将这一合作推理过程正规化。然后,我们在两个在线行为实验(N = 312 名成人)中对这一模型进行了评估。在实验 1 中,我们发现教师在选择例子时会考虑到学习者的背景知识,并根据学习者的反馈调整他们的例子。在实验 2 中,我们发现当教师的示例偏离学习者的背景知识时,学习者会有策略地提供更多反馈。这些发现为将计算教学法扩展到更丰富的互动环境奠定了基础。
{"title":"A Hierarchical Bayesian Model of Adaptive Teaching","authors":"Alicia M. Chen,&nbsp;Andrew Palacci,&nbsp;Natalia Vélez,&nbsp;Robert D. Hawkins,&nbsp;Samuel J. Gershman","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13477","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13477","url":null,"abstract":"<p>How do teachers learn about what learners already know? How do learners aid teachers by providing them with information about their background knowledge and what they find confusing? We formalize this collaborative reasoning process using a hierarchical Bayesian model of pedagogy. We then evaluate this model in two online behavioral experiments (<i>N</i> = 312 adults). In Experiment 1, we show that teachers select examples that account for learners' background knowledge, and adjust their examples based on learners' feedback. In Experiment 2, we show that learners strategically provide more feedback when teachers' examples deviate from their background knowledge. These findings provide a foundation for extending computational accounts of pedagogy to richer interactive settings.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13477","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141564840","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Word Forms Reflect Trade-Offs Between Speaker Effort and Robust Listener Recognition 词形反映了说话者的努力与强大的听者识别之间的权衡。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-09 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13478
Stephan C. Meylan, Thomas L. Griffiths

How do cognitive pressures shape the lexicons of natural languages? Here, we reframe George Kingsley Zipf's proposed “law of abbreviation” within a more general framework that relates it to cognitive pressures that affect speakers and listeners. In this new framework, speakers' drive to reduce effort (Zipf's proposal) is counteracted by the need for low-frequency words to have word forms that are sufficiently distinctive to allow for accurate recognition by listeners. To support this framework, we replicate and extend recent work using the prevalence of subword phonemic sequences (phonotactic probability) to measure speakers' production effort in place of Zipf's measure of length. Across languages and corpora, phonotactic probability is more strongly correlated with word frequency than word length. We also show this measure of ease of speech production (phonotactic probability) is strongly correlated with a measure of perceptual difficulty that indexes the degree of competition from alternative interpretations in word recognition. This is consistent with the claim that there must be trade-offs between these two factors, and is inconsistent with a recent proposal that phonotactic probability facilitates both perception and production. To our knowledge, this is the first work to offer an explanation why long, phonotactically improbable word forms remain in the lexicons of natural languages.

认知压力是如何塑造自然语言词典的?在此,我们将乔治-金斯利-齐普夫(George Kingsley Zipf)提出的 "缩略法则 "重新纳入一个更广泛的框架,将其与影响说话者和听话者的认知压力联系起来。在这一新的框架中,由于低频词需要有足够独特的词形才能被听者准确识别,这就抵消了说话者减少努力的动力(齐普夫的提议)。为了支持这一框架,我们复制并扩展了最近的研究成果,使用子词音位序列的普遍性(音位概率)来衡量说话者的生产努力,以取代齐普夫的长度衡量方法。在各种语言和语料库中,音位概率与词频的相关性比与词长的相关性更强。我们还表明,这种衡量语音生成难易程度的方法(语音战术概率)与衡量感知难度的方法密切相关,而感知难度则反映了在单词识别过程中来自其他解释的竞争程度。这与 "这两个因素之间必须有所权衡 "的说法是一致的,而与最近提出的 "语音战术概率同时促进感知和语音生成 "的说法不一致。据我们所知,这是第一项能解释为什么长的、语音学上不可能的词形会保留在自然语言的词典中的研究。
{"title":"Word Forms Reflect Trade-Offs Between Speaker Effort and Robust Listener Recognition","authors":"Stephan C. Meylan,&nbsp;Thomas L. Griffiths","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13478","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13478","url":null,"abstract":"<p>How do cognitive pressures shape the lexicons of natural languages? Here, we reframe George Kingsley Zipf's proposed “law of abbreviation” within a more general framework that relates it to cognitive pressures that affect speakers and listeners. In this new framework, speakers' drive to reduce effort (Zipf's proposal) is counteracted by the need for low-frequency words to have word forms that are sufficiently distinctive to allow for accurate recognition by listeners. To support this framework, we replicate and extend recent work using the prevalence of subword phonemic sequences (phonotactic probability) to measure speakers' production effort in place of Zipf's measure of length. Across languages and corpora, phonotactic probability is more strongly correlated with word frequency than word length. We also show this measure of ease of speech production (phonotactic probability) is strongly correlated with a measure of perceptual difficulty that indexes the degree of competition from alternative interpretations in word recognition. This is consistent with the claim that there must be trade-offs between these two factors, and is inconsistent with a recent proposal that phonotactic probability facilitates both perception and production. To our knowledge, this is the first work to offer an explanation why long, phonotactically improbable word forms remain in the lexicons of natural languages.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13478","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141564844","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Learners’ Spontaneous Gesture Before a Math Lesson Predicts the Efficacy of Seeing Versus Doing Gesture During the Lesson 学习者在数学课前的自发手势可预测上课时 "看 "与 "做 "手势的效果。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-09 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13479
Eliza L. Congdon, Elizabeth M. Wakefield, Miriam A. Novack, Naureen Hemani-Lopez, Susan Goldin-Meadow

Gestures—hand movements that accompany speech and express ideas—can help children learn how to solve problems, flexibly generalize learning to novel problem-solving contexts, and retain what they have learned. But does it matter who is doing the gesturing? We know that producing gesture leads to better comprehension of a message than watching someone else produce gesture. But we do not know how producing versus observing gesture impacts deeper learning outcomes such as generalization and retention across time. Moreover, not all children benefit equally from gesture instruction, suggesting that there are individual differences that may play a role in who learns from gesture. Here, we consider two factors that might impact whether gesture leads to learning, generalization, and retention after mathematical instruction: (1) whether children see gesture or do gesture and (2) whether a child spontaneously gestures before instruction when explaining their problem-solving reasoning. For children who spontaneously gestured before instruction, both doing and seeing gesture led to better generalization and retention of the knowledge gained than a comparison manipulative action. For children who did not spontaneously gesture before instruction, doing gesture was less effective than the comparison action for learning, generalization, and retention. Importantly, this learning deficit was specific to gesture, as these children did benefit from doing the comparison manipulative action. Our findings are the first evidence that a child's use of a particular representational format for communication (gesture) directly predicts that child's propensity to learn from using the same representational format.

手势--伴随语言和表达想法的手部动作--可以帮助儿童学习如何解决问题,灵活地将所学知识运用到新的解决问题的情境中,并保持所学知识。但是,谁做手势重要吗?我们知道,做手势比看别人做手势能更好地理解信息。但我们还不知道,做出手势与观察手势对更深层次的学习成果(如泛化和跨时间保持)有什么影响。此外,并非所有儿童都能同样从手势教学中获益,这表明个体差异可能会对谁从手势中学习产生影响。在此,我们考虑了两个可能影响数学教学后手势是否导致学习、泛化和保持的因素:(1)儿童是看到手势还是做出手势;(2)儿童在教学前解释他们解决问题的推理时是否自发地做出手势。对于在教学前自发做手势的儿童来说,做手势和看手势都比比较操作动作能更好地概括和保留所学知识。而对于在教学前没有自发做出手势的儿童来说,做手势在学习、概括和保持方面的效果都不如对比动作。重要的是,这种学习缺陷是手势特有的,因为这些儿童确实从做对比操作动作中获益。我们的研究结果首次证明,儿童使用特定的表征形式(手势)进行交流可直接预测儿童使用相同表征形式进行学习的倾向。
{"title":"Learners’ Spontaneous Gesture Before a Math Lesson Predicts the Efficacy of Seeing Versus Doing Gesture During the Lesson","authors":"Eliza L. Congdon,&nbsp;Elizabeth M. Wakefield,&nbsp;Miriam A. Novack,&nbsp;Naureen Hemani-Lopez,&nbsp;Susan Goldin-Meadow","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13479","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13479","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Gestures—hand movements that accompany speech and express ideas—can help children learn how to solve problems, flexibly generalize learning to novel problem-solving contexts, and retain what they have learned. But does it matter who is doing the gesturing? We know that producing gesture leads to better comprehension of a message than watching someone else produce gesture. But we do not know how producing versus observing gesture impacts deeper learning outcomes such as generalization and retention across time. Moreover, not all children benefit equally from gesture instruction, suggesting that there are individual differences that may play a role in who learns from gesture. Here, we consider two factors that might impact whether gesture leads to learning, generalization, and retention after mathematical instruction: (1) whether children see gesture or do gesture and (2) whether a child spontaneously gestures before instruction when explaining their problem-solving reasoning. For children who spontaneously gestured before instruction, both doing and seeing gesture led to better generalization and retention of the knowledge gained than a comparison manipulative action. For children who did not spontaneously gesture before instruction, doing gesture was less effective than the comparison action for learning, generalization, and retention. Importantly, this learning deficit was specific to gesture, as these children did benefit from doing the comparison manipulative action. Our findings are the first evidence that a child's use of a particular representational format for communication (gesture) directly predicts that child's propensity to learn from using the same representational format.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141564842","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When the Ends Justify the Mean: The Endpoint Leverage Effect in Distribution Perception 当 "目的 "证明 "平均 "合理时:分配认知中的终点杠杆效应。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-09 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13455
Jonas Ebert, Roland Deutsch

Previous research described different cognitive processes on how individuals process distributional information. Based on these processes, the current research uncovered a novel phenomenon in distribution perception: the Endpoint Leverage Effect. Subjective endpoints influence distribution estimations not only locally around the endpoint but also influence estimations across the whole value range of the distribution. The influence is largest close to the respective endpoint and decreases in size toward the opposite end of the value range. Three experiments investigate this phenomenon: Experiment 1 provides correlational evidence for the Endpoint Leverage Effect after presenting participants with a numerical distribution. Experiment 2 demonstrates the Endpoint Leverage Effect by manipulating the subjective endpoints of a numerical distribution directly. Experiment 3 generalizes the phenomenon by investigating a general population sample and estimations regarding a real-world income distribution. In addition, quantitative model analysis examines the cognitive processes underlying the effect. Overall, the novel Endpoint Leverage Effect is found in all three experiments, inspiring further research in a wide area of contexts.

以往的研究描述了个人处理分布信息的不同认知过程。基于这些过程,目前的研究发现了分布感知中的一个新现象:端点杠杆效应。主观端点不仅会影响端点周围局部的分布估计,还会影响整个分布值范围的估计。这种影响在接近相应端点时最大,在数值范围的另一端则逐渐减小。三个实验对这一现象进行了研究:实验 1 向参与者展示数值分布后,提供了端点杠杆效应的相关证据。实验 2 通过直接操纵数值分布的主观端点来证明端点杠杆效应。实验 3 通过调查普通人群样本和对现实世界收入分布的估计,将这一现象普遍化。此外,定量模型分析还研究了该效应背后的认知过程。总之,在所有三个实验中都发现了新颖的端点杠杆效应,从而激发了在广泛领域的进一步研究。
{"title":"When the Ends Justify the Mean: The Endpoint Leverage Effect in Distribution Perception","authors":"Jonas Ebert,&nbsp;Roland Deutsch","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13455","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13455","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Previous research described different cognitive processes on how individuals process distributional information. Based on these processes, the current research uncovered a novel phenomenon in distribution perception: the Endpoint Leverage Effect. Subjective endpoints influence distribution estimations not only locally around the endpoint but also influence estimations across the whole value range of the distribution. The influence is largest close to the respective endpoint and decreases in size toward the opposite end of the value range. Three experiments investigate this phenomenon: Experiment 1 provides correlational evidence for the Endpoint Leverage Effect after presenting participants with a numerical distribution. Experiment 2 demonstrates the Endpoint Leverage Effect by manipulating the subjective endpoints of a numerical distribution directly. Experiment 3 generalizes the phenomenon by investigating a general population sample and estimations regarding a real-world income distribution. In addition, quantitative model analysis examines the cognitive processes underlying the effect. Overall, the novel Endpoint Leverage Effect is found in all three experiments, inspiring further research in a wide area of contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13455","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141564843","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1