首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Science最新文献

英文 中文
Intellectually Rigorous but Morally Tolerant: Exploring Moral Leniency as a Mediator Between Cognitive Style and “Utilitarian” Judgment 理性严谨但道德宽容:探索道德宽容作为认知风格与“功利”判断之间的中介
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-03 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.70024
Manon D. Gouiran, Florian Cova

Past research on people's moral judgments about moral dilemmas has revealed a connection between utilitarian judgment and reflective cognitive style. This has traditionally been interpreted as reflection is conducive to utilitarianism. However, recent research shows that the connection between reflective cognitive style and utilitarian judgments holds only when participants are asked whether the utilitarian option is permissible, and disappears when they are asked whether it is recommended. To explain this phenomenon, we propose that reflective cognitive style is associated with a greater moral leniency—that is, a greater tendency to be tolerant of moral violations, and that moral leniency predicts utilitarian judgment when utilitarian judgment is measured through permissibility. In Study 1 (N = 192), we design a set of vignettes to assess moral leniency. In Studies 2 and 3 (N = 455, 428), we show that reflective cognitive style is indeed associated with greater moral leniency, and that moral leniency mediates the connection between cognitive style and utilitarian judgment. We discuss the implication of our results for the interpretation of the relationship between utilitarianism and reflective cognitive style.

以往关于道德困境道德判断的研究揭示了功利判断与反思认知风格之间的联系。这在传统上被解释为反思有利于功利主义。然而,最近的研究表明,只有当参与者被问及功利主义选择是否被允许时,反思性认知风格和功利主义判断之间的联系才成立,而当他们被问及功利主义选择是否被推荐时,这种联系就消失了。为了解释这一现象,我们提出反思性认知风格与更大的道德宽容有关——也就是说,更倾向于容忍违反道德的行为,而道德宽容预示着功利主义判断,当功利主义判断是通过容忍度来衡量的。在研究1 (N = 192)中,我们设计了一组小插曲来评估道德宽容。在研究2和研究3中(N = 455,428),我们发现反映性认知风格确实与更大的道德宽容有关,而道德宽容在认知风格和功利判断之间起到中介作用。我们讨论了我们的结果对解释功利主义和反思性认知风格之间关系的意义。
{"title":"Intellectually Rigorous but Morally Tolerant: Exploring Moral Leniency as a Mediator Between Cognitive Style and “Utilitarian” Judgment","authors":"Manon D. Gouiran,&nbsp;Florian Cova","doi":"10.1111/cogs.70024","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.70024","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Past research on people's moral judgments about moral dilemmas has revealed a connection between utilitarian judgment and reflective cognitive style. This has traditionally been interpreted as reflection is conducive to utilitarianism. However, recent research shows that the connection between reflective cognitive style and utilitarian judgments holds only when participants are asked whether the utilitarian option is permissible, and disappears when they are asked whether it is recommended. To explain this phenomenon, we propose that reflective cognitive style is associated with a greater moral leniency—that is, a greater tendency to be tolerant of moral violations, and that moral leniency predicts utilitarian judgment when utilitarian judgment is measured through permissibility. In Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 192), we design a set of vignettes to assess moral leniency. In Studies 2 and 3 (<i>N</i> = 455, 428), we show that reflective cognitive style is indeed associated with greater moral leniency, and that moral leniency mediates the connection between cognitive style and utilitarian judgment. We discuss the implication of our results for the interpretation of the relationship between utilitarianism and reflective cognitive style.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-12-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.70024","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142764051","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Constant Error, Revisited: A New Explanation of the Halo Effect 一个持续的错误,重新审视:对光环效应的新解释
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-03 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.70022
Chris Westbury, Daniel King

Judgments of character traits tend to be overcorrelated, a bias known as the halo effect. We conducted two studies to test an explanation of the effect based on shared lexical context and connotation. Study 1 tested whether the context similarity of trait names could explain 39 participants’ ratings of the probability that two traits would co-occur. Over 126 trait pairs, cosine similarity between the word2vec vectors of the two words was a reliable predictor of the human judgments of trait co-occurrence probability (cross-validated r2 = .19, p < .001). Two measures related to word similarity increased the variation accounted for in the human judgments to 45%, cross-validated (p < .001). In Experiment 2, 40 different participants judged similarity of word meaning within the pairs, confirming that the word pairs were not simply synonymous (Average [SD] = 40.8/100 [13.1/100]). Shared lexical context and word connotation play a role in shaping the halo effect.

人们对性格特征的判断往往是过度相关的,这种偏见被称为光环效应。我们进行了两项研究来检验基于共享词汇语境和内涵的效应解释。研究1测试了性状名称的上下文相似性是否可以解释39名参与者对两个性状同时出现的概率的评分。在126对特征对中,两个词的word2vec向量之间的余弦相似度是人类判断特征共现概率的可靠预测因子(交叉验证r2 = 0.19, p <;措施)。两项与单词相似度相关的测量将人类判断中的差异增加到45%,交叉验证(p <;措施)。在实验2中,40名不同的参与者判断单词对内的词义相似度,证实单词对不是简单的同义(平均[SD] = 40.8/100[13.1/100])。共同的词汇语境和词汇内涵在光环效应的形成中起着重要作用。
{"title":"A Constant Error, Revisited: A New Explanation of the Halo Effect","authors":"Chris Westbury,&nbsp;Daniel King","doi":"10.1111/cogs.70022","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.70022","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Judgments of character traits tend to be overcorrelated, a bias known as <i>the halo effect</i>. We conducted two studies to test an explanation of the effect based on shared lexical context and connotation. Study 1 tested whether the context similarity of trait names could explain 39 participants’ ratings of the probability that two traits would co-occur. Over 126 trait pairs, cosine similarity between the word2vec vectors of the two words was a reliable predictor of the human judgments of trait co-occurrence probability (cross-validated <i>r</i><sup>2</sup> = .19, <i>p</i> &lt; .001). Two measures related to word similarity increased the variation accounted for in the human judgments to 45%, cross-validated (<i>p</i> &lt; .001). In Experiment 2, 40 different participants judged similarity of word meaning within the pairs, confirming that the word pairs were not simply synonymous (Average [SD] = 40.8/100 [13.1/100]). Shared lexical context and word connotation play a role in shaping the halo effect.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-12-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.70022","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142764052","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Task and Timing Effects in Argument Role Sensitivity: Evidence From Production, EEG, and Computational Modeling 争论角色敏感性中的任务和时间效应:来自生产、脑电图和计算模型的证据
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-03 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.70023
Masato Nakamura, Shota Momma, Hiromu Sakai, Colin Phillips

Comprehenders generate expectations about upcoming lexical items in language processing using various types of contextual information. However, a number of studies have shown that argument roles do not impact neural and behavioral prediction measures. Despite these robust findings, some prior studies have suggested that lexical prediction might be sensitive to argument roles in production tasks such as the cloze task or in comprehension tasks when additional time is available for prediction. This study demonstrates that both the task and additional time for prediction independently influence lexical prediction using argument roles, via evidence from closely matched electroencephalogram (EEG) and speeded cloze experiments. In order to investigate the timing effect, our EEG experiment used maximally simple Japanese stimuli such as Bee-nom/acc sting, and it manipulated the time for prediction by changing the temporal interval between the context noun and the target verb without adding any further linguistic content. In order to investigate the task effect, we conducted a speeded cloze study that was matched with our EEG study both in terms of stimuli and the time available for prediction. We found that both the EEG study with additional time for prediction and the speeded cloze study with matched timing showed clear sensitivity to argument roles, while the EEG conditions with less time for prediction replicated the standard pattern of argument role insensitivity. Based on these findings, we propose that lexical prediction is initially insensitive to argument roles but a monitoring mechanism serially inhibits role-inappropriate candidates. This monitoring process operates quickly in production tasks, where it is important to quickly select a single candidate to produce, whereas it may operate more slowly in comprehension tasks, where multiple candidates can be maintained until a continuation is perceived. Computational simulations demonstrate that this mechanism can successfully explain the task and timing effects observed in our experiments.

在语言处理过程中,理解者利用不同类型的语境信息对即将到来的词汇产生期望。然而,许多研究表明,争论角色并不影响神经和行为预测措施。尽管有这些强有力的发现,一些先前的研究表明,当有额外的时间用于预测时,词汇预测可能对诸如完形填空任务或理解任务中的论点角色很敏感。本研究通过密切匹配的脑电图(EEG)和快速填空实验的证据表明,预测任务和额外的预测时间都独立地影响使用论点角色的词汇预测。为了研究时间效应,我们的脑电图实验使用了最简单的日语刺激,如Bee-nom/acc sting,并通过改变上下文名词和目标动词之间的时间间隔来操纵预测时间,而不添加任何进一步的语言内容。为了研究任务效应,我们进行了一项快速填空研究,该研究与我们的脑电图研究在刺激和预测可用时间方面相匹配。我们发现,增加预测时间的脑电图研究和匹配时间的快速完形填空研究都对争论角色表现出明显的敏感性,而减少预测时间的脑电图条件则复制了争论角色不敏感的标准模式。基于这些发现,我们提出词汇预测最初对论点角色不敏感,但监控机制会逐渐抑制角色不合适的候选人。这种监控过程在生产任务中运行得很快,在生产任务中,快速选择要生成的单个候选任务很重要,而在理解任务中,它可能运行得较慢,在理解任务中,可以维护多个候选任务,直到感知到延续。计算模拟表明,该机制可以很好地解释实验中观察到的任务效应和时序效应。
{"title":"Task and Timing Effects in Argument Role Sensitivity: Evidence From Production, EEG, and Computational Modeling","authors":"Masato Nakamura,&nbsp;Shota Momma,&nbsp;Hiromu Sakai,&nbsp;Colin Phillips","doi":"10.1111/cogs.70023","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.70023","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Comprehenders generate expectations about upcoming lexical items in language processing using various types of contextual information. However, a number of studies have shown that argument roles do not impact neural and behavioral prediction measures. Despite these robust findings, some prior studies have suggested that lexical prediction might be sensitive to argument roles in production tasks such as the cloze task or in comprehension tasks when additional time is available for prediction. This study demonstrates that both the task and additional time for prediction independently influence lexical prediction using argument roles, via evidence from closely matched electroencephalogram (EEG) and speeded cloze experiments. In order to investigate the timing effect, our EEG experiment used maximally simple Japanese stimuli such as <i>Bee-nom/acc sting</i>, and it manipulated the time for prediction by changing the temporal interval between the context noun and the target verb without adding any further linguistic content. In order to investigate the task effect, we conducted a speeded cloze study that was matched with our EEG study both in terms of stimuli and the time available for prediction. We found that both the EEG study with additional time for prediction and the speeded cloze study with matched timing showed clear sensitivity to argument roles, while the EEG conditions with less time for prediction replicated the standard pattern of argument role insensitivity. Based on these findings, we propose that lexical prediction is initially insensitive to argument roles but a monitoring mechanism serially inhibits role-inappropriate candidates. This monitoring process operates quickly in production tasks, where it is important to quickly select a single candidate to produce, whereas it may operate more slowly in comprehension tasks, where multiple candidates can be maintained until a continuation is perceived. Computational simulations demonstrate that this mechanism can successfully explain the task and timing effects observed in our experiments.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-12-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.70023","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142764048","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Iconicity Emerges From Language Experience: Evidence From Japanese Ideophones and Their English Equivalents. 象似性产生于语言经验:来自日语意指音及其英语对等体的证据。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-01 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.70031
Hinano Iida, Kimi Akita

Iconicity is a relationship of resemblance between the form and meaning of a sign. Compelling evidence from diverse areas of the cognitive sciences suggests that iconicity plays a pivotal role in the processing, memory, learning, and evolution of both spoken and signed language, indicating that iconicity is a general property of language. However, the language-specific aspect of iconicity, illustrated by the fact that the meanings of ideophones in an unfamiliar language are hard to guess (e.g., shigeshige 'staring at something' in Japanese), remains to be fully investigated. In the present study, native speakers of Japanese and English rated the iconicity and familiarity of Japanese ideophones (e.g., gatagata 'rattling', butsubutsu 'murmuring') and their English equivalents (e.g., rattle, murmur). Two main findings emerged: (1) individuals generally perceived their native language as more iconic than their non-native language, replicating the previous findings in signed language, and (2) the familiarity of words in their native language boosted their perceived iconicity. These findings shed a light on the language-specific, subjective, and acquired nature of iconicity.

象似性是符号的形式和意义之间的一种相似关系。来自认知科学不同领域的令人信服的证据表明,象似性在口语和手语的加工、记忆、学习和进化中起着关键作用,表明象似性是语言的普遍属性。然而,象似性在语言方面的特殊性,如在不熟悉的语言中,意指词的含义很难猜测(例如,在日语中,shigeshige表示“盯着某物看”),仍有待充分研究。在本研究中,以日语和英语为母语的人对日语音标(例如,gatagata“嘎嘎作响”,butsubutsu“喃喃自语”)和英语音标(例如,rattle,杂音)的象似性和熟悉度进行了评分。主要发现有两个:(1)个体普遍认为母语比非母语更具象征意义,这与之前在手语中的发现相同;(2)对母语词汇的熟悉度提高了他们的象征意义感知。这些发现揭示了象似性的语言特殊性、主观性和后天性。
{"title":"Iconicity Emerges From Language Experience: Evidence From Japanese Ideophones and Their English Equivalents.","authors":"Hinano Iida, Kimi Akita","doi":"10.1111/cogs.70031","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.70031","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Iconicity is a relationship of resemblance between the form and meaning of a sign. Compelling evidence from diverse areas of the cognitive sciences suggests that iconicity plays a pivotal role in the processing, memory, learning, and evolution of both spoken and signed language, indicating that iconicity is a general property of language. However, the language-specific aspect of iconicity, illustrated by the fact that the meanings of ideophones in an unfamiliar language are hard to guess (e.g., shigeshige 'staring at something' in Japanese), remains to be fully investigated. In the present study, native speakers of Japanese and English rated the iconicity and familiarity of Japanese ideophones (e.g., gatagata 'rattling', butsubutsu 'murmuring') and their English equivalents (e.g., rattle, murmur). Two main findings emerged: (1) individuals generally perceived their native language as more iconic than their non-native language, replicating the previous findings in signed language, and (2) the familiarity of words in their native language boosted their perceived iconicity. These findings shed a light on the language-specific, subjective, and acquired nature of iconicity.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 12","pages":"e70031"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11670811/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142899404","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Linguistic-Sensorimotor Model of the Basic-Level Advantage in Category Verification. 类别验证中基本水平优势的语言-感觉运动模型。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-01 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.70025
Cai Wingfield, Rens van Hoef, Louise Connell

People are generally more accurate at categorizing objects at the basic level (e.g., dog) than at more general, superordinate categories (e.g., animal). Recent research has suggested that this basic-level advantage emerges from the linguistic-distributional and sensorimotor relationship between a category concept and object concept, but the proposed mechanisms have not been subject to a formal computational test. In this paper, we present a computational model of category verification that allows linguistic distributional information and sensorimotor experience to interact in a grounded implementation of a full-size adult conceptual system. In simulations across multiple datasets, we demonstrate that the model performs the task of category verification at a level comparable to human participants, and-critically-that its operation naturally gives rise to the basic-level-advantage phenomenon. That is, concepts are easier to categorize when there is a high degree of overlap in sensorimotor experience and/or linguistic distributional knowledge between category and member concepts, and the basic-level advantage emerges as an overall behavioral artifact of this linguistic and sensorimotor overlap. Findings support the linguistic-sensorimotor preparation account of the basic-level advantage and, more broadly, linguistic-sensorimotor theories of the conceptual system.

一般来说,人们在基本层面(如狗)对物体的分类比在更一般、更高级的类别(如动物)上更准确。最近的研究表明,这种基本水平的优势来自于类别概念和对象概念之间的语言分布和感觉运动关系,但所提出的机制尚未经过正式的计算测试。在本文中,我们提出了一个类别验证的计算模型,该模型允许语言分布信息和感觉运动经验在全尺寸成人概念系统的基础实现中相互作用。在跨多个数据集的模拟中,我们证明了该模型在与人类参与者相当的水平上执行类别验证任务,并且至关重要的是,其操作自然会产生基本水平优势现象。也就是说,当类别和成员概念之间在感觉运动经验和/或语言分布知识上存在高度重叠时,概念更容易分类,并且基本水平的优势作为这种语言和感觉运动重叠的整体行为产物出现。研究结果支持基础水平优势的语言-感觉运动准备解释,更广泛地说,支持概念系统的语言-感觉运动理论。
{"title":"A Linguistic-Sensorimotor Model of the Basic-Level Advantage in Category Verification.","authors":"Cai Wingfield, Rens van Hoef, Louise Connell","doi":"10.1111/cogs.70025","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.70025","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People are generally more accurate at categorizing objects at the basic level (e.g., dog) than at more general, superordinate categories (e.g., animal). Recent research has suggested that this basic-level advantage emerges from the linguistic-distributional and sensorimotor relationship between a category concept and object concept, but the proposed mechanisms have not been subject to a formal computational test. In this paper, we present a computational model of category verification that allows linguistic distributional information and sensorimotor experience to interact in a grounded implementation of a full-size adult conceptual system. In simulations across multiple datasets, we demonstrate that the model performs the task of category verification at a level comparable to human participants, and-critically-that its operation naturally gives rise to the basic-level-advantage phenomenon. That is, concepts are easier to categorize when there is a high degree of overlap in sensorimotor experience and/or linguistic distributional knowledge between category and member concepts, and the basic-level advantage emerges as an overall behavioral artifact of this linguistic and sensorimotor overlap. Findings support the linguistic-sensorimotor preparation account of the basic-level advantage and, more broadly, linguistic-sensorimotor theories of the conceptual system.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 12","pages":"e70025"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11666073/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142883344","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can Language Models Trained on Written Monologue Learn to Predict Spoken Dialogue? 在书面独白中训练的语言模型能否学会预测口语对话?
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-26 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.70013
Muhammad Umair, Julia B. Mertens, Lena Warnke, Jan P. de Ruiter

Transformer-based Large Language Models (LLMs) have recently increased in popularity, in part due to their impressive performance on a number of language tasks. While LLMs can produce human-like writing, the extent to which these models can learn to predict spoken language in natural interaction remains unclear. This is a nontrivial question, as spoken and written language differ in syntax, pragmatics, and norms that interlocutors follow. Previous work suggests that while LLMs may develop an understanding of linguistic rules based on statistical regularities, they fail to acquire the knowledge required for language use. This implies that LLMs may not learn the normative structure underlying interactive spoken language, but may instead only model superficial regularities in speech. In this paper, we aim to evaluate LLMs as models of spoken dialogue. Specifically, we investigate whether LLMs can learn that the identity of a speaker in spoken dialogue influences what is likely to be said. To answer this question, we first fine-tuned two variants of a specific LLM (GPT-2) on transcripts of natural spoken dialogue in English. Then, we used these models to compute surprisal values for two-turn sequences with the same first-turn but different second-turn speakers and compared the output to human behavioral data. While the predictability of words in all fine-tuned models was influenced by speaker identity information, the models did not replicate humans' use of this information. Our findings suggest that although LLMs may learn to generate text conforming to normative linguistic structure, they do not (yet) faithfully replicate human behavior in natural conversation.

基于变换器的大型语言模型(LLMs)最近越来越受欢迎,部分原因是它们在一些语言任务中表现出色。虽然 LLM 可以生成类似人类的书写,但这些模型能在多大程度上学会预测自然交互中的口语仍不清楚。这是一个非同小可的问题,因为口语和书面语在句法、语用学和对话者遵循的规范方面存在差异。以往的研究表明,虽然语言学习者可以根据统计规律理解语言规则,但他们无法获得语言使用所需的知识。这意味着 LLMs 可能无法学习交互式口语背后的规范结构,而只能模拟言语中的表面规律。本文旨在评估作为口语对话模型的 LLMs。具体来说,我们研究 LLMs 是否能够学习到,在口语对话中,说话者的身份会影响可能会说的话。为了回答这个问题,我们首先在英语自然口语对话的记录本上对特定 LLM(GPT-2)的两个变体进行了微调。然后,我们使用这些模型计算了具有相同第一轮但不同第二轮说话人的两轮序列的意外值,并将输出结果与人类行为数据进行了比较。虽然所有微调模型中单词的可预测性都受到说话者身份信息的影响,但这些模型并没有复制人类对这一信息的使用。我们的研究结果表明,尽管 LLMs 可以学习生成符合规范语言结构的文本,但它们(目前)还不能忠实地复制人类在自然对话中的行为。
{"title":"Can Language Models Trained on Written Monologue Learn to Predict Spoken Dialogue?","authors":"Muhammad Umair,&nbsp;Julia B. Mertens,&nbsp;Lena Warnke,&nbsp;Jan P. de Ruiter","doi":"10.1111/cogs.70013","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.70013","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Transformer-based Large Language Models (LLMs) have recently increased in popularity, in part due to their impressive performance on a number of language tasks. While LLMs can produce human-like writing, the extent to which these models can learn to predict <i>spoken</i> language in natural interaction remains unclear. This is a nontrivial question, as spoken and written language differ in syntax, pragmatics, and norms that interlocutors follow. Previous work suggests that while LLMs may develop an understanding of linguistic rules based on statistical regularities, they fail to acquire the knowledge required for language use. This implies that LLMs may not learn the normative structure underlying interactive spoken language, but may instead only model superficial regularities in speech. In this paper, we aim to evaluate LLMs as models of spoken dialogue. Specifically, we investigate whether LLMs can learn that the <i>identity</i> of a speaker in spoken dialogue influences what is likely to be said. To answer this question, we first fine-tuned two variants of a specific LLM (GPT-2) on transcripts of natural spoken dialogue in English. Then, we used these models to compute surprisal values for two-turn sequences with the same first-turn but different second-turn speakers and compared the output to human behavioral data. While the predictability of words in all fine-tuned models was influenced by speaker identity information, the models did not replicate humans' use of this information. Our findings suggest that although LLMs may learn to generate text conforming to normative linguistic structure, they do not (yet) faithfully replicate human behavior in natural conversation.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142717401","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dissociable Contributions of Goal-Relevant Evidence and Goal-Irrelevant Familiarity to Individual and Developmental Differences in Conflict Recognition 目标相关证据和目标相关熟悉程度对冲突识别中的个体差异和发展差异的不同贡献。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-26 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.70019
Alexander Weigard, Takakuni Suzuki, Lena J. Skalaban, May Conley, Alexandra O. Cohen, Hugh Garavan, Mary M. Heitzeg, B. J. Casey, Chandra Sripada, Andrew Heathcote

Recent studies using the diffusion decision model find that performance across many cognitive control tasks can be largely attributed to a task-general efficiency of evidence accumulation (EEA) factor that reflects individuals’ ability to selectively gather evidence relevant to task goals. However, estimates of EEA from an n-back “conflict recognition” paradigm in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) Study, a large, diverse sample of youth, appear to contradict these findings. EEA estimates from “lure” trials—which present stimuli that are familiar (i.e., presented previously) but do not meet formal criteria for being a target—show inconsistent relations with EEA estimates from other trials and display atypical v-shaped bivariate distributions, suggesting many individuals are responding based largely on stimulus familiarity rather than goal-relevant stimulus features. We present a new formal model of evidence integration in conflict recognition tasks that distinguishes individuals’ EEA for goal-relevant evidence from their use of goal-irrelevant familiarity. We then investigate developmental, cognitive, and clinical correlates of these novel parameters. Parameters for EEA and goal-irrelevant familiarity-based processing showed strong correlations across levels of n-back load, suggesting they are task-general dimensions that influence individuals’ performance regardless of working memory demands. Only EEA showed large, robust developmental differences in the ABCD sample and an independent age-diverse sample. EEA also exhibited higher test-retest reliability and uniquely meaningful associations with clinically relevant dimensions. These findings establish a principled modeling framework for characterizing conflict recognition mechanisms and have several broader implications for research on individual and developmental differences in cognitive control.

最近使用扩散决策模型进行的研究发现,许多认知控制任务的表现在很大程度上可归因于任务一般的证据积累效率(EEA)因素,该因素反映了个体选择性地收集与任务目标相关的证据的能力。然而,在青少年脑认知发展研究(ABCD)(一项大型、多样化的青少年样本研究)中,通过n-back "冲突识别 "范式得出的EEA估计值似乎与这些发现相矛盾。引诱 "试验中的EEA估计值与其他试验中的EEA估计值的关系并不一致,而且显示出不典型的V型双变量分布,这表明许多个体主要是根据刺激物的熟悉程度而不是与目标相关的刺激物特征来做出反应的。我们提出了冲突识别任务中证据整合的新形式模型,该模型将个体对目标相关证据的 EEA 与他们对目标无关熟悉度的使用区分开来。然后,我们对这些新参数的发展、认知和临床相关性进行了研究。EEA参数和基于目标相关熟悉度的处理参数在不同的n-back负荷水平上显示出很强的相关性,这表明它们是任务的一般维度,无论工作记忆需求如何都会影响个体的表现。在 ABCD 样本和一个独立的不同年龄样本中,只有 EEA 显示出巨大而稳健的发展差异。此外,EEA 还表现出更高的测试再测可靠性以及与临床相关维度的独特关联。这些发现为描述冲突识别机制建立了一个原则性的建模框架,并对认知控制的个体差异和发展差异研究产生了一些更广泛的影响。
{"title":"Dissociable Contributions of Goal-Relevant Evidence and Goal-Irrelevant Familiarity to Individual and Developmental Differences in Conflict Recognition","authors":"Alexander Weigard,&nbsp;Takakuni Suzuki,&nbsp;Lena J. Skalaban,&nbsp;May Conley,&nbsp;Alexandra O. Cohen,&nbsp;Hugh Garavan,&nbsp;Mary M. Heitzeg,&nbsp;B. J. Casey,&nbsp;Chandra Sripada,&nbsp;Andrew Heathcote","doi":"10.1111/cogs.70019","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.70019","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Recent studies using the diffusion decision model find that performance across many cognitive control tasks can be largely attributed to a task-general efficiency of evidence accumulation (EEA) factor that reflects individuals’ ability to selectively gather evidence relevant to task goals. However, estimates of EEA from an n-back “conflict recognition” paradigm in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development<sup>SM</sup> (ABCD) Study, a large, diverse sample of youth, appear to contradict these findings. EEA estimates from “lure” trials—which present stimuli that are familiar (i.e., presented previously) but do not meet formal criteria for being a target—show inconsistent relations with EEA estimates from other trials and display atypical v-shaped bivariate distributions, suggesting many individuals are responding based largely on stimulus familiarity rather than goal-relevant stimulus features. We present a new formal model of evidence integration in conflict recognition tasks that distinguishes individuals’ EEA for goal-relevant evidence from their use of goal-irrelevant familiarity. We then investigate developmental, cognitive, and clinical correlates of these novel parameters. Parameters for EEA and goal-irrelevant familiarity-based processing showed strong correlations across levels of n-back load, suggesting they are task-general dimensions that influence individuals’ performance regardless of working memory demands. Only EEA showed large, robust developmental differences in the ABCD sample and an independent age-diverse sample. EEA also exhibited higher test-retest reliability and uniquely meaningful associations with clinically relevant dimensions. These findings establish a principled modeling framework for characterizing conflict recognition mechanisms and have several broader implications for research on individual and developmental differences in cognitive control.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.70019","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142717412","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Semantic Content in Face Representation: Essential for Proficient Recognition of Unfamiliar Faces by Good Recognizers 人脸表征中的语义内容:优秀识别器熟练识别不熟悉面孔的必要条件
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-26 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.70020
Tong Jiang, Guomei Zhou

Face recognition is adapted to achieve goals of social interactions, which rely on further processing of the semantic information of faces, beyond visual computations. Here, we explored the semantic content of face representation apart from visual component, and tested their relations to face recognition performance. Specifically, we propose that enhanced visual or semantic coding could underlie the advantage of familiar over unfamiliar faces recognition, as well as the superior recognition of skilled face recognizers. We asked participants to freely describe familiar/unfamiliar faces using words or phrases, and converted these descriptions into semantic vectors. Face semantics were transformed into quantifiable face vectors by aggregating these word/phrase vectors. We also extracted visual features from a deep convolutional neural network and obtained the visual representation of familiar/unfamiliar faces. Semantic and visual representations were used to predict perceptual representation generated from a behavior rating task separately in different groups (bad/good face recognizers in familiar-face/unfamiliar-face conditions). Comparisons revealed that although long-term memory facilitated visual feature extraction for familiar faces compared to unfamiliar faces, good recognizers compensated for this disparity by incorporating more semantic information for unfamiliar faces, a strategy not observed in bad recognizers. This study highlights the significance of semantics in recognizing unfamiliar faces.

人脸识别是为了实现社会交往的目标,而社会交往的目标依赖于视觉计算之外对人脸语义信息的进一步处理。在此,我们探讨了除视觉成分之外的人脸表征的语义内容,并测试了它们与人脸识别成绩的关系。具体来说,我们认为视觉或语义编码的增强可能是熟悉人脸识别优于陌生人脸识别的原因,也是熟练的人脸识别者识别能力更强的原因。我们要求参与者用单词或短语自由描述熟悉/不熟悉的面孔,并将这些描述转换成语义向量。通过汇总这些单词/短语向量,将人脸语义转化为可量化的人脸向量。我们还从深度卷积神经网络中提取了视觉特征,并获得了熟悉/不熟悉人脸的视觉表征。语义表征和视觉表征分别用于预测不同组别(熟悉面孔/不熟悉面孔条件下的坏/好面孔识别者)的行为评级任务所产生的知觉表征。比较结果表明,虽然与不熟悉的面孔相比,长期记忆有利于熟悉面孔的视觉特征提取,但好的人脸识别者通过为不熟悉的面孔纳入更多语义信息来弥补这种差异,这种策略在差的人脸识别者身上没有观察到。这项研究强调了语义在识别陌生面孔中的重要性。
{"title":"Semantic Content in Face Representation: Essential for Proficient Recognition of Unfamiliar Faces by Good Recognizers","authors":"Tong Jiang,&nbsp;Guomei Zhou","doi":"10.1111/cogs.70020","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.70020","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Face recognition is adapted to achieve goals of social interactions, which rely on further processing of the semantic information of faces, beyond visual computations. Here, we explored the semantic content of face representation apart from visual component, and tested their relations to face recognition performance. Specifically, we propose that enhanced visual or semantic coding could underlie the advantage of familiar over unfamiliar faces recognition, as well as the superior recognition of skilled face recognizers. We asked participants to freely describe familiar/unfamiliar faces using words or phrases, and converted these descriptions into semantic vectors. Face semantics were transformed into quantifiable face vectors by aggregating these word/phrase vectors. We also extracted visual features from a deep convolutional neural network and obtained the visual representation of familiar/unfamiliar faces. Semantic and visual representations were used to predict perceptual representation generated from a behavior rating task separately in different groups (bad/good face recognizers in familiar-face/unfamiliar-face conditions). Comparisons revealed that although long-term memory facilitated visual feature extraction for familiar faces compared to unfamiliar faces, good recognizers compensated for this disparity by incorporating more semantic information for unfamiliar faces, a strategy not observed in bad recognizers. This study highlights the significance of semantics in recognizing unfamiliar faces.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142717428","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beyond the Positivity Bias: The Processing and Integration of Self-Relevant Feedback Is Driven by Its Alignment With Pre-Existing Self-Views 超越积极性偏差:自我相关反馈的处理和整合受其与预先存在的自我观点的一致性所驱动
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.70017
Josué García-Arch, Solenn Friedrich, Xiongbo Wu, David Cucurell, Lluís Fuentemilla

Our self-concept is constantly faced with self-relevant information. Prevailing research suggests that information's valence plays a central role in shaping our self-views. However, the need for stability within the self-concept structure and the inherent alignment of positive feedback with the pre-existing self-views of healthy individuals might mask valence and congruence effects. In this study (N = 30, undergraduates), we orthogonalized feedback valence and self-congruence effects to examine the behavioral and electrophysiological signatures of self-relevant feedback processing and self-concept updating. We found that participants had a preference for integrating self-congruent and dismissing self-incongruent feedback, regardless of its valence. Consistently, electroencephalography results revealed that feedback congruence, but not feedback valence, is rapidly detected during early processing stages. Our findings diverge from the accepted notion that self-concept updating is based on the selective incorporation of positive information. These findings offer novel insights into self-concept dynamics, with implications for the understanding of psychopathological conditions.

我们的自我概念经常面对与自我相关的信息。流行的研究表明,信息的价值在塑造我们的自我概念方面起着核心作用。然而,自我概念结构需要稳定,而且积极反馈与健康人原有的自我概念内在一致,这可能会掩盖情绪和一致性效应。在这项研究中(N = 30,本科生),我们将反馈情绪和自我一致性效应正交化,以检验自我相关反馈处理和自我概念更新的行为和电生理特征。我们发现,无论反馈的效价如何,参与者都倾向于整合与自我一致的反馈,而忽略与自我不一致的反馈。与此一致的是,脑电图结果显示,在早期处理阶段,反馈一致性(而非反馈价值)会被迅速检测到。我们的研究结果与公认的自我概念更新是基于对积极信息的选择性吸收的观点不同。这些发现为自我概念的动态变化提供了新的视角,对理解精神病理学状况具有重要意义。
{"title":"Beyond the Positivity Bias: The Processing and Integration of Self-Relevant Feedback Is Driven by Its Alignment With Pre-Existing Self-Views","authors":"Josué García-Arch,&nbsp;Solenn Friedrich,&nbsp;Xiongbo Wu,&nbsp;David Cucurell,&nbsp;Lluís Fuentemilla","doi":"10.1111/cogs.70017","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.70017","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Our self-concept is constantly faced with self-relevant information. Prevailing research suggests that information's valence plays a central role in shaping our self-views. However, the need for stability within the self-concept structure and the inherent alignment of positive feedback with the pre-existing self-views of healthy individuals might mask valence and congruence effects. In this study (<i>N</i> = 30, undergraduates), we orthogonalized feedback valence and self-congruence effects to examine the behavioral and electrophysiological signatures of self-relevant feedback processing and self-concept updating. We found that participants had a preference for integrating self-congruent and dismissing self-incongruent feedback, regardless of its valence. Consistently, electroencephalography results revealed that feedback congruence, but not feedback valence, is rapidly detected during early processing stages. Our findings diverge from the accepted notion that self-concept updating is based on the selective incorporation of positive information. These findings offer novel insights into self-concept dynamics, with implications for the understanding of psychopathological conditions.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.70017","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142666025","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lay Theories of Moral Progress 道德进步论
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.70018
Casey Lewry, Sana Asifriyaz, Tania Lombrozo

Many consider the world to be morally better today than it was in the past and expect moral improvement to continue. How do people explain what drives this change? In this paper, we identify two ways people might think about how moral progress occurs: that it is driven by human action (i.e., if people did not actively work to make the world better, moral progress would not occur) or that it is driven by an unspecified mechanism (i.e., that our world is destined to morally improve, but without specifying a role for human action). In Study 1 (N = 147), we find that those who more strongly believe that the mechanism of moral progress is human action are more likely to believe their own intervention is warranted to correct a moral setback. In Study 2 (N = 145), we find that this translates to intended action: those who more strongly believe moral progress is driven by human action report that they would donate more money to correct a moral setback. In Study 3 (N = 297), participants generate their own explanations for why moral progress occurs. We find that participants’ donation intentions are predicted by whether their explanations state that human action drives moral progress. Together, these studies suggest that beliefs about the mechanisms of moral progress have important implications for engaging in social action.

许多人认为当今世界的道德水平比过去有所提高,并期望道德水平的提高能够持续下去。人们如何解释这种变化的驱动力?在本文中,我们指出了人们思考道德进步的两种方式:道德进步是由人类行动驱动的(即如果人们不积极努力使世界变得更好,道德进步就不会发生),或者道德进步是由一种未指定的机制驱动的(即我们的世界注定会在道德上有所改善,但未指定人类行动的作用)。在研究 1(N = 147)中,我们发现那些更坚信道德进步的机制是人类行动的人,更有可能相信他们自己的干预是纠正道德挫折的正当理由。在研究 2(N = 145)中,我们发现这一点会转化为预期行动:那些更坚信道德进步是由人类行动推动的人表示,他们会捐出更多的钱来纠正道德挫折。在研究 3(N = 297)中,参与者对道德进步发生的原因做出了自己的解释。我们发现,参与者的捐款意向会受到他们的解释是否表明人类行动推动了道德进步的预测。这些研究共同表明,关于道德进步机制的信念对参与社会行动具有重要影响。
{"title":"Lay Theories of Moral Progress","authors":"Casey Lewry,&nbsp;Sana Asifriyaz,&nbsp;Tania Lombrozo","doi":"10.1111/cogs.70018","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.70018","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Many consider the world to be morally better today than it was in the past and expect moral improvement to continue. How do people explain what drives this change? In this paper, we identify two ways people might think about how moral progress occurs: that it is driven by human action (i.e., if people did not actively work to make the world better, moral progress would not occur) or that it is driven by an unspecified mechanism (i.e., that our world is destined to morally improve, but without specifying a role for human action). In Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 147), we find that those who more strongly believe that the mechanism of moral progress is human action are more likely to believe their own intervention is warranted to correct a moral setback. In Study 2 (<i>N</i> = 145), we find that this translates to intended action: those who more strongly believe moral progress is driven by human action report that they would donate more money to correct a moral setback. In Study 3 (<i>N</i> = 297), participants generate their own explanations for why moral progress occurs. We find that participants’ donation intentions are predicted by whether their explanations state that human action drives moral progress. Together, these studies suggest that beliefs about the mechanisms of moral progress have important implications for engaging in social action.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.70018","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142665101","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1