首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Science最新文献

英文 中文
The Effects of Linear Order in Category Learning: Some Replications of Ramscar et al. (2010) and Their Implications for Replicating Training Studies 类别学习中线性顺序的影响:拉姆斯卡尔等人(2010)的一些研究成果及其对重复训练研究的启示。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-22 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13445
Eva Viviani, Michael Ramscar, Elizabeth Wonnacott

Ramscar, Yarlett, Dye, Denny, and Thorpe (2010) showed how, consistent with the predictions of error-driven learning models, the order in which stimuli are presented in training can affect category learning. Specifically, learners exposed to artificial language input where objects preceded their labels learned the discriminating features of categories better than learners exposed to input where labels preceded objects. We sought to replicate this finding in two online experiments employing the same tests used originally: A four pictures test (match a label to one of four pictures) and a four labels test (match a picture to one of four labels). In our study, only findings from the four pictures test were consistent with the original result. Additionally, the effect sizes observed were smaller, and participants over-generalized high-frequency category labels more than in the original study. We suggest that although Ramscar, Yarlett, Dye, Denny, and Thorpe (2010) feature-label order predictions were derived from error-driven learning, they failed to consider that this mechanism also predicts that performance in any training paradigm must inevitably be influenced by participant prior experience. We consider our findings in light of these factors, and discuss implications for the generalizability and replication of training studies.

Ramscar、Yarlett、Dye、Denny 和 Thorpe(2010 年)的研究表明,与错误驱动学习模型的预测一致,训练中刺激物的呈现顺序会影响类别学习。具体来说,学习者在接受人工语言输入时,对象先于标签,比接受输入时标签先于对象的学习者能更好地学习类别的辨别特征。我们试图在两个在线实验中重复这一发现,并采用了最初使用的相同测试:四幅图片测试(将一个标签与四幅图片中的一幅匹配)和四个标签测试(将一幅图片与四个标签中的一个匹配)。在我们的研究中,只有四幅图片测试的结果与最初的结果一致。此外,观察到的效应大小较小,而且与原始研究相比,参与者对高频类别标签的过度概括程度更高。我们认为,虽然 Ramscar、Yarlett、Dye、Denny 和 Thorpe(2010 年)的特征标签顺序预测是由错误驱动学习得出的,但他们没有考虑到这一机制也预测了在任何训练范式中的表现都不可避免地会受到参与者先前经验的影响。我们将根据这些因素来考虑我们的发现,并讨论对训练研究的推广和复制的影响。
{"title":"The Effects of Linear Order in Category Learning: Some Replications of Ramscar et al. (2010) and Their Implications for Replicating Training Studies","authors":"Eva Viviani,&nbsp;Michael Ramscar,&nbsp;Elizabeth Wonnacott","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13445","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13445","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Ramscar, Yarlett, Dye, Denny, and Thorpe (2010) showed how, consistent with the predictions of error-driven learning models, the order in which stimuli are presented in training can affect category learning. Specifically, learners exposed to artificial language input where objects preceded their labels learned the discriminating features of categories better than learners exposed to input where labels preceded objects. We sought to replicate this finding in two online experiments employing the same tests used originally: A four pictures test (match a label to one of four pictures) and a four labels test (match a picture to one of four labels). In our study, only findings from the four pictures test were consistent with the original result. Additionally, the effect sizes observed were smaller, and participants over-generalized high-frequency category labels more than in the original study. We suggest that although Ramscar, Yarlett, Dye, Denny, and Thorpe (2010) feature-label order predictions were derived from error-driven learning, they failed to consider that this mechanism also predicts that performance in any training paradigm must inevitably be influenced by participant prior experience. We consider our findings in light of these factors, and discuss implications for the generalizability and replication of training studies.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13445","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141082740","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do They Know It's Christmash? Lexical Knowledge Directly Impacts Speech Perception 他们知道这是圣诞节吗?词汇知识直接影响语音感知
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-21 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13449
Sahil Luthra, Anne Marie Crinnion, David Saltzman, James S. Magnuson

We recently reported strong, replicable (i.e., replicated) evidence for lexically mediated compensation for coarticulation (LCfC; Luthra et al., 2021), whereby lexical knowledge influences a prelexical process. Critically, evidence for LCfC provides robust support for interactive models of cognition that include top-down feedback and is inconsistent with autonomous models that allow only feedforward processing. McQueen, Jesse, and Mitterer (2023) offer five counter-arguments against our interpretation; we respond to each of those arguments here and conclude that top-down feedback provides the most parsimonious explanation of extant data.

我们最近报告了词汇介导的共同发音补偿(LCfC;Luthra et al.至关重要的是,LCfC 的证据为包含自上而下反馈的互动认知模型提供了强有力的支持,而与只允许前馈处理的自主模型并不一致。麦奎恩、杰西和米特勒(2023)针对我们的解释提出了五个反驳论点;我们在此一一回应,并得出结论:自上而下的反馈为现有数据提供了最合理的解释。
{"title":"Do They Know It's Christmash? Lexical Knowledge Directly Impacts Speech Perception","authors":"Sahil Luthra,&nbsp;Anne Marie Crinnion,&nbsp;David Saltzman,&nbsp;James S. Magnuson","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13449","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13449","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We recently reported strong, replicable (i.e., replicated) evidence for <i>lexically mediated compensation for coarticulation</i> (LCfC; Luthra et al., 2021), whereby lexical knowledge influences a prelexical process. Critically, evidence for LCfC provides robust support for <i>interactive</i> models of cognition that include <i>top-down feedback</i> and is inconsistent with <i>autonomous</i> models that allow only feedforward processing. McQueen, Jesse, and Mitterer (2023) offer five counter-arguments against our interpretation; we respond to each of those arguments here and conclude that top-down feedback provides the most parsimonious explanation of extant data.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141077180","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Tracking the Cognitive Band in an Open-Ended Task 追踪开放式任务中的认知带
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-21 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13454
John R. Anderson, Shawn Betts, Daniel Bothell, Cvetomir M. Dimov, Jon M. Fincham

Open-ended tasks can be decomposed into the three levels of Newell's Cognitive Band: the Unit-Task level, the Operation level, and the Deliberate-Act level. We analyzed the video game Co-op Space Fortress at these levels, reporting both the match of a cognitive model to subject behavior and the use of electroencephalogram (EEG) to track subject cognition. The Unit Task level in this game involves coordinating with a partner to kill a fortress. At this highest level of the Cognitive Band, there is a good match between subject behavior and the model. The EEG signals were also strong enough to track when Unit Tasks succeeded or failed. The intermediate Operation level in this task involves legs of flight to achieve a kill. The EEG signals associated with these operations are much weaker than the signals associated with the Unit Tasks. Still, it was possible to reconstruct subject play with much better than chance success. There were significant differences in the leg behavior of subjects and models. Model behavior did not provide a good basis for interpreting a subject's behavior at this level. At the lowest Deliberate-Act level, we observed overlapping key actions, which the model did not display. Such overlapping key actions also frustrated efforts to identify EEG signals of motor actions. We conclude that the Unit-task level is the appropriate level both for understanding open-ended tasks and for using EEG to track the performance of open-ended tasks.

开放式任务可分解为纽厄尔认知带的三个层次:单元任务层次、操作层次和慎重行动层次。我们从这些层面分析了视频游戏《合作太空堡垒》,报告了认知模型与受试者行为的匹配情况,以及使用脑电图(EEG)追踪受试者认知的情况。该游戏中的 "单位任务 "关卡涉及与伙伴协调击杀堡垒。在这一最高级别的认知带中,受试者的行为与模型十分吻合。脑电信号的强度也足以追踪单元任务成功或失败的时间。该任务的中间操作级别涉及双腿飞行以实现击杀。与这些操作相关的脑电信号要比与单元任务相关的信号弱得多。尽管如此,仍有可能比偶然成功率高得多地重建受试者的游戏。受试者和模型的腿部行为存在明显差异。在这个水平上,模型行为并不能为解释受试者的行为提供良好的基础。在最低的 "故意行动 "水平上,我们观察到了重叠的按键动作,而模型并没有表现出这些动作。这些重叠的关键动作也阻碍了识别运动动作脑电信号的努力。我们的结论是,单元任务级别是理解开放式任务和使用脑电图跟踪开放式任务执行情况的适当级别。
{"title":"Tracking the Cognitive Band in an Open-Ended Task","authors":"John R. Anderson,&nbsp;Shawn Betts,&nbsp;Daniel Bothell,&nbsp;Cvetomir M. Dimov,&nbsp;Jon M. Fincham","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13454","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13454","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Open-ended tasks can be decomposed into the three levels of Newell's Cognitive Band: the Unit-Task level, the Operation level, and the Deliberate-Act level. We analyzed the video game Co-op Space Fortress at these levels, reporting both the match of a cognitive model to subject behavior and the use of electroencephalogram (EEG) to track subject cognition. The Unit Task level in this game involves coordinating with a partner to kill a fortress. At this highest level of the Cognitive Band, there is a good match between subject behavior and the model. The EEG signals were also strong enough to track when Unit Tasks succeeded or failed. The intermediate Operation level in this task involves legs of flight to achieve a kill. The EEG signals associated with these operations are much weaker than the signals associated with the Unit Tasks. Still, it was possible to reconstruct subject play with much better than chance success. There were significant differences in the leg behavior of subjects and models. Model behavior did not provide a good basis for interpreting a subject's behavior at this level. At the lowest Deliberate-Act level, we observed overlapping key actions, which the model did not display. Such overlapping key actions also frustrated efforts to identify EEG signals of motor actions. We conclude that the Unit-task level is the appropriate level both for understanding open-ended tasks and for using EEG to track the performance of open-ended tasks.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13454","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141077181","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
One Cue's Loss Is Another Cue's Gain—Learning Morphophonology Through Unlearning 一个线索的损失是另一个线索的收获--通过不学习来学习语音学
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-15 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13450
Erdin Mujezinović, Vsevolod Kapatsinski, Ruben van de Vijver

A word often expresses many different morphological functions. Which part of a word contributes to which part of the overall meaning is not always clear, which raises the question as to how such functions are learned. While linguistic studies tacitly assume the co-occurrence of cues and outcomes to suffice in learning these functions (Baer-Henney, Kügler, & van de Vijver, 2015; Baer-Henney & van de Vijver, 2012), error-driven learning suggests that contingency rather than contiguity is crucial (Nixon, 2020; Ramscar, Yarlett, Dye, Denny, & Thorpe, 2010). In error-driven learning, cues gain association strength if they predict a certain outcome, and they lose strength if the outcome is absent. This reduction of association strength is called unlearning. So far, it is unclear if such unlearning has consequences for cue–outcome associations beyond the ones that get reduced. To test for such consequences of unlearning, we taught participants morphophonological patterns in an artificial language learning experiment. In one block, the cues to two morphological outcomes—plural and diminutive—co-occurred within the same word forms. In another block, a single cue to only one of these two outcomes was presented in a different set of word forms. We wanted to find out, if participants unlearn this cue's association with the outcome that is not predicted by the cue alone, and if this allows the absent cue to be associated with the absent outcome. Our results show that if unlearning was possible, participants learned that the absent cue predicts the absent outcome better than if no unlearning was possible. This effect was stronger if the unlearned cue was more salient. This shows that unlearning takes place even if no alternative cues to an absent outcome are provided, which highlights that learners take both positive and negative evidence into account—as predicted by domain general error-driven learning.

一个词往往表达许多不同的形态功能。一个词的哪个部分对整体意义的哪个部分起作用并不总是很清楚,这就提出了如何学习这些功能的问题。虽然语言学研究默认线索和结果的共同出现足以帮助学习这些功能(Baer-Henney, Kügler, & van de Vijver, 2015; Baer-Henney & van de Vijver, 2012),但错误驱动学习表明偶然性而非连续性才是关键(Nixon, 2020; Ramscar, Yarlett, Dye, Denny, & Thorpe, 2010)。在错误驱动学习中,如果线索能预测出某种结果,它们就会获得关联强度,而如果预测不到结果,它们就会失去关联强度。这种关联强度的降低被称为 "非学习"。到目前为止,还不清楚这种 "解除学习 "是否会对线索与结果之间的关联产生影响。为了测试这种解除学习的后果,我们在人工语言学习实验中向参与者教授了形态音素模式。在一个区块中,两个词形结果的线索--大写和小写--出现在相同的词形中。在另一个区块中,在一组不同的词形中只出现了这两种结果中的一种。我们想知道,如果被试没有学习到这一线索与结果之间的关联,而这一线索又不能单独预测结果,那么被试是否能将缺失的线索与缺失的结果联系起来。我们的研究结果表明,如果可以取消学习,那么与无法取消学习的情况相比,参与者能更好地学习到缺失线索对缺失结果的预测作用。如果未学习的线索更加突出,这种效果会更强。这表明,即使没有提供缺失结果的替代线索,学习者也会进行解除学习,这突出表明学习者会同时考虑积极和消极的证据--正如一般错误驱动学习领域所预测的那样。
{"title":"One Cue's Loss Is Another Cue's Gain—Learning Morphophonology Through Unlearning","authors":"Erdin Mujezinović,&nbsp;Vsevolod Kapatsinski,&nbsp;Ruben van de Vijver","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13450","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13450","url":null,"abstract":"<p>A word often expresses many different morphological functions. Which part of a word contributes to which part of the overall meaning is not always clear, which raises the question as to how such functions are learned. While linguistic studies tacitly assume the co-occurrence of cues and outcomes to suffice in learning these functions (Baer-Henney, Kügler, &amp; van de Vijver, 2015; Baer-Henney &amp; van de Vijver, 2012), error-driven learning suggests that contingency rather than contiguity is crucial (Nixon, 2020; Ramscar, Yarlett, Dye, Denny, &amp; Thorpe, 2010). In error-driven learning, cues gain association strength if they predict a certain outcome, and they lose strength if the outcome is absent. This reduction of association strength is called unlearning. So far, it is unclear if such unlearning has consequences for cue–outcome associations beyond the ones that get reduced. To test for such consequences of unlearning, we taught participants morphophonological patterns in an artificial language learning experiment. In one block, the cues to two morphological outcomes—plural and diminutive—co-occurred within the same word forms. In another block, a single cue to only one of these two outcomes was presented in a different set of word forms. We wanted to find out, if participants unlearn this cue's association with the outcome that is not predicted by the cue alone, and if this allows the absent cue to be associated with the absent outcome. Our results show that if unlearning was possible, participants learned that the absent cue predicts the absent outcome better than if no unlearning was possible. This effect was stronger if the unlearned cue was more salient. This shows that unlearning takes place even if no alternative cues to an absent outcome are provided, which highlights that learners take both positive and negative evidence into account—as predicted by domain general error-driven learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13450","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140923539","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Learning the Meanings of Function Words From Grounded Language Using a Visual Question Answering Model 利用视觉问题解答模型从基础语言中学习功能词的含义
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-14 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13448
Eva Portelance, Michael C. Frank, Dan Jurafsky

Interpreting a seemingly simple function word like “or,” “behind,” or “more” can require logical, numerical, and relational reasoning. How are such words learned by children? Prior acquisition theories have often relied on positing a foundation of innate knowledge. Yet recent neural-network-based visual question answering models apparently can learn to use function words as part of answering questions about complex visual scenes. In this paper, we study what these models learn about function words, in the hope of better understanding how the meanings of these words can be learned by both models and children. We show that recurrent models trained on visually grounded language learn gradient semantics for function words requiring spatial and numerical reasoning. Furthermore, we find that these models can learn the meanings of logical connectives and and or without any prior knowledge of logical reasoning as well as early evidence that they are sensitive to alternative expressions when interpreting language. Finally, we show that word learning difficulty is dependent on the frequency of models' input. Our findings offer proof-of-concept evidence that it is possible to learn the nuanced interpretations of function words in a visually grounded context by using non-symbolic general statistical learning algorithms, without any prior knowledge of linguistic meaning.

解释 "或"、"在......后面 "或 "更多 "等看似简单的功能词需要逻辑、数字和关系推理。儿童是如何学会这些词的呢?先前的习得理论往往依赖于先天知识基础的假设。然而,最近基于神经网络的视觉问题解答模型显然可以学会使用功能词,作为回答复杂视觉场景问题的一部分。在本文中,我们研究了这些模型对功能词的学习情况,希望能更好地理解模型和儿童是如何学习这些词的含义的。我们的研究表明,在视觉语言基础上训练的递归模型可以学习需要空间和数字推理的功能词的梯度语义。此外,我们还发现,这些模型可以在没有任何逻辑推理知识的情况下学习逻辑连接词和和或的含义,而且有早期证据表明,这些模型在解释语言时对替代表达方式很敏感。最后,我们还发现单词学习的难度取决于模型输入的频率。我们的研究结果提供了概念证明,即在没有任何语言意义知识的前提下,通过使用非符号的一般统计学习算法,在视觉基础的语境中学习功能词的细微解释是可能的。
{"title":"Learning the Meanings of Function Words From Grounded Language Using a Visual Question Answering Model","authors":"Eva Portelance,&nbsp;Michael C. Frank,&nbsp;Dan Jurafsky","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13448","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13448","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Interpreting a seemingly simple function word like “or,” “behind,” or “more” can require logical, numerical, and relational reasoning. How are such words learned by children? Prior acquisition theories have often relied on positing a foundation of innate knowledge. Yet recent neural-network-based visual question answering models apparently can learn to use function words as part of answering questions about complex visual scenes. In this paper, we study what these models learn about function words, in the hope of better understanding how the meanings of these words can be learned by both models and children. We show that recurrent models trained on visually grounded language learn gradient semantics for function words requiring spatial and numerical reasoning. Furthermore, we find that these models can learn the meanings of logical connectives <i>and</i> and <i>or</i> without any prior knowledge of logical reasoning as well as early evidence that they are sensitive to alternative expressions when interpreting language. Finally, we show that word learning difficulty is dependent on the frequency of models' input. Our findings offer proof-of-concept evidence that it is possible to learn the nuanced interpretations of function words in a visually grounded context by using non-symbolic general statistical learning algorithms, without any prior knowledge of linguistic meaning.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13448","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140919246","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Improved Perception of Aggression Under (un)Related Threat of Shock 在(不)相关的冲击威胁下提高对攻击行为的感知。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-14 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13451
Fábio Silva, Marta I. Garrido, Sandra C. Soares

Anxiety shifts visual attention and perceptual mechanisms, preparing oneself to detect potentially threatening information more rapidly. Despite being demonstrated for threat-related social stimuli, such as fearful expressions, it remains unexplored if these effects encompass other social cues of danger, such as aggressive gestures/actions. To this end, we recruited a total of 65 participants and asked them to identify, as quickly and accurately as possible, potentially aggressive actions depicted by an agent. By introducing and manipulating the occurrence of electric shocks, we induced safe and threatening conditions. In addition, the association between electric shocks and aggression was also manipulated. Our result showed that participants have improved sensitivity, with no changes to criterion, when detecting aggressive gestures during threat compared to safe conditions. Furthermore, drift diffusion model analysis showed that under threat participants exhibited faster evidence accumulation toward the correct perceptual decision. Lastly, the relationship between threat source and aggression appeared to not impact any of the effects described above. Overall, our results indicate that the benefits gained from states of anxiety, such as increased sensitivity toward threat and greater evidence accumulation, are transposable to social stimuli capable of signaling danger other than facial expressions.

焦虑会转移视觉注意力和感知机制,使自己准备好更快地发现潜在的威胁信息。尽管与威胁相关的社会刺激(如恐惧的表情)已经得到证实,但这些效应是否包括其他危险的社会线索(如攻击性手势/动作),仍有待探索。为此,我们招募了 65 名参与者,要求他们尽可能快速、准确地识别代理人所描述的潜在攻击性动作。通过引入和操纵电击的发生,我们诱发了安全和威胁条件。此外,我们还操纵了电击与攻击性之间的关联。我们的结果表明,与安全条件相比,受试者在威胁条件下检测攻击性手势时的灵敏度有所提高,但标准没有变化。此外,漂移扩散模型分析表明,在威胁条件下,参与者表现出更快的证据积累,从而做出正确的感知决策。最后,威胁源和攻击性之间的关系似乎不会影响上述任何效应。总之,我们的研究结果表明,焦虑状态下获得的益处,如对威胁的敏感性提高和证据积累增加,可以移植到面部表情以外的能够发出危险信号的社会刺激上。
{"title":"Improved Perception of Aggression Under (un)Related Threat of Shock","authors":"Fábio Silva,&nbsp;Marta I. Garrido,&nbsp;Sandra C. Soares","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13451","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13451","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Anxiety shifts visual attention and perceptual mechanisms, preparing oneself to detect potentially threatening information more rapidly. Despite being demonstrated for threat-related social stimuli, such as fearful expressions, it remains unexplored if these effects encompass other social cues of danger, such as aggressive gestures/actions. To this end, we recruited a total of 65 participants and asked them to identify, as quickly and accurately as possible, potentially aggressive actions depicted by an agent. By introducing and manipulating the occurrence of electric shocks, we induced safe and threatening conditions. In addition, the association between electric shocks and aggression was also manipulated. Our result showed that participants have improved sensitivity, with no changes to criterion, when detecting aggressive gestures during threat compared to safe conditions. Furthermore, drift diffusion model analysis showed that under threat participants exhibited faster evidence accumulation toward the correct perceptual decision. Lastly, the relationship between threat source and aggression appeared to not impact any of the effects described above. Overall, our results indicate that the benefits gained from states of anxiety, such as increased sensitivity toward threat and greater evidence accumulation, are transposable to social stimuli capable of signaling danger other than facial expressions.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13451","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140915751","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reviving Bistable Perception in Patients With Depression by Decreasing the Overestimation of Prior Precision 通过降低对先前精确度的高估,恢复抑郁症患者的双稳态感知。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-14 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13452
Wenbo Wang, Changbo Zhu, Ting Jia, Meidan Zu, Yandong Tang, Liqin Zhou, Yanghua Tian, Bailu Si, Ke Zhou

Slower perceptual alternations, a notable perceptual effect observed in psychiatric disorders, can be alleviated by antidepressant therapies that affect serotonin levels in the brain. While these phenomena have been well documented, the underlying neurocognitive mechanisms remain to be elucidated. Our study bridges this gap by employing a computational cognitive approach within a Bayesian predictive coding framework to explore these mechanisms in depression. We fitted a prediction error (PE) model to behavioral data from a binocular rivalry task, uncovering that significantly higher initial prior precision and lower PE led to a slower switch rate in patients with depression. Furthermore, serotonin-targeting antidepressant treatments significantly decreased the prior precision and increased PE, both of which were predictive of improvements in the perceptual alternation rate of depression patients. These findings indicated that the substantially slower perception switch rate in patients with depression was caused by the greater reliance on top-down priors and that serotonin treatment's efficacy was in its recalibration of these priors and enhancement of PE. Our study not only elucidates the cognitive underpinnings of depression, but also suggests computational modeling as a potent tool for integrating cognitive science with clinical psychology, advancing our understanding and treatment of cognitive impairments in depression.

知觉交替变慢是精神疾病中观察到的一种明显的知觉效应,可以通过影响大脑中血清素水平的抗抑郁疗法来缓解。虽然这些现象已被充分记录,但其背后的神经认知机制仍有待阐明。我们的研究在贝叶斯预测编码框架内采用计算认知方法来探索抑郁症的这些机制,从而弥补了这一空白。我们将预测误差(PE)模型拟合到双目对抗任务的行为数据中,发现抑郁症患者的初始先验精确度显著较高,PE较低,从而导致其转换速度较慢。此外,以血清素为靶点的抗抑郁药物治疗能显著降低先验精确度并提高PE,这两点都能预测抑郁症患者知觉交替率的改善。这些研究结果表明,抑郁症患者的知觉转换速度之所以大大降低,是因为他们更依赖于自上而下的先验,而血清素治疗的疗效在于重新校准了这些先验并提高了PE。我们的研究不仅阐明了抑郁症的认知基础,还表明计算建模是将认知科学与临床心理学相结合的有效工具,能促进我们对抑郁症认知障碍的理解和治疗。
{"title":"Reviving Bistable Perception in Patients With Depression by Decreasing the Overestimation of Prior Precision","authors":"Wenbo Wang,&nbsp;Changbo Zhu,&nbsp;Ting Jia,&nbsp;Meidan Zu,&nbsp;Yandong Tang,&nbsp;Liqin Zhou,&nbsp;Yanghua Tian,&nbsp;Bailu Si,&nbsp;Ke Zhou","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13452","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13452","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Slower perceptual alternations, a notable perceptual effect observed in psychiatric disorders, can be alleviated by antidepressant therapies that affect serotonin levels in the brain. While these phenomena have been well documented, the underlying neurocognitive mechanisms remain to be elucidated. Our study bridges this gap by employing a computational cognitive approach within a Bayesian predictive coding framework to explore these mechanisms in depression. We fitted a prediction error (PE) model to behavioral data from a binocular rivalry task, uncovering that significantly higher initial prior precision and lower PE led to a slower switch rate in patients with depression. Furthermore, serotonin-targeting antidepressant treatments significantly decreased the prior precision and increased PE, both of which were predictive of improvements in the perceptual alternation rate of depression patients. These findings indicated that the substantially slower perception switch rate in patients with depression was caused by the greater reliance on top-down priors and that serotonin treatment's efficacy was in its recalibration of these priors and enhancement of PE. Our study not only elucidates the cognitive underpinnings of depression, but also suggests computational modeling as a potent tool for integrating cognitive science with clinical psychology, advancing our understanding and treatment of cognitive impairments in depression.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140915731","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response (ASMR) and the Functions of Consciousness 自主感觉经络反应(ASMR)与意识的功能。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-14 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13453
Dylan Ludwig, Muhammad Ali Khalidi

“Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response” (ASMR) refers to a sensory-emotional experience that was first explicitly identified and named within the past two decades in online discussion boards. Since then, there has been mounting psychological and neural evidence of a clustering of properties common to the phenomenon of ASMR, including convergence on the set of stimuli that trigger the experience, the properties of the experience itself, and its downstream effects. Moreover, psychological instruments have begun to be developed and employed in an attempt to measure it. Based on this empirical work, we make the case that despite its nonscientific origins, ASMR is a good candidate for being a real kind in the cognitive sciences. The phenomenon appears to have a robust causal profile and may also have an adaptive evolutionary history. We also argue that a more thorough understanding of the distinctive type of phenomenal experience involved in an ASMR episode can shed light on the functions of consciousness, and ultimately undermine certain “cognitive” theories of consciousness. We conclude that ASMR should be the subject of more extensive scientific investigation, particularly since it may also have the potential for therapeutic applications.

"自主感觉经络反应"(ASMR)指的是一种感官情绪体验,在过去二十年间首次在网上讨论区被明确识别和命名。从那时起,越来越多的心理学和神经学证据表明,ASMR 现象具有一系列共同属性,包括触发体验的刺激集合、体验本身的属性及其下游效应。此外,人们已开始开发和使用心理学工具,试图对其进行测量。基于这些实证研究工作,我们认为,尽管 ASMR 起源于非科学领域,但它很有可能成为认知科学领域的一种真正的现象。这种现象似乎具有强大的因果关系,而且还可能具有适应性进化史。我们还认为,更透彻地了解 ASMR 事件所涉及的现象体验的独特类型,可以揭示意识的功能,并最终削弱某些意识的 "认知 "理论。我们的结论是,ASMR 应该成为更广泛科学研究的主题,特别是因为它还可能具有治疗应用的潜力。
{"title":"Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response (ASMR) and the Functions of Consciousness","authors":"Dylan Ludwig,&nbsp;Muhammad Ali Khalidi","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13453","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13453","url":null,"abstract":"<p>“Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response” (ASMR) refers to a sensory-emotional experience that was first explicitly identified and named within the past two decades in online discussion boards. Since then, there has been mounting psychological and neural evidence of a clustering of properties common to the phenomenon of ASMR, including convergence on the set of stimuli that trigger the experience, the properties of the experience itself, and its downstream effects. Moreover, psychological instruments have begun to be developed and employed in an attempt to measure it. Based on this empirical work, we make the case that despite its nonscientific origins, ASMR is a good candidate for being a real kind in the cognitive sciences. The phenomenon appears to have a robust causal profile and may also have an adaptive evolutionary history. We also argue that a more thorough understanding of the distinctive type of phenomenal experience involved in an ASMR episode can shed light on the functions of consciousness, and ultimately undermine certain “cognitive” theories of consciousness. We conclude that ASMR should be the subject of more extensive scientific investigation, particularly since it may also have the potential for therapeutic applications.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13453","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140915723","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Solving Bongard Problems With a Visual Language and Pragmatic Constraints 用视觉语言和实用限制解决邦加德问题
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-03 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13432
Stefan Depeweg, Contantin A. Rothkopf, Frank Jäkel

More than 50 years ago, Bongard introduced 100 visual concept learning problems as a challenge for artificial vision systems. These problems are now known as Bongard problems. Although they are well known in cognitive science and artificial intelligence, only very little progress has been made toward building systems that can solve a substantial subset of them. In the system presented here, visual features are extracted through image processing and then translated into a symbolic visual vocabulary. We introduce a formal language that allows representing compositional visual concepts based on this vocabulary. Using this language and Bayesian inference, concepts can be induced from the examples that are provided in each problem. We find a reasonable agreement between the concepts with high posterior probability and the solutions formulated by Bongard himself for a subset of 35 problems. While this approach is far from solving Bongard problems like humans, it does considerably better than previous approaches. We discuss the issues we encountered while developing this system and their continuing relevance for understanding visual cognition. For instance, contrary to other concept learning problems, the examples are not random in Bongard problems; instead they are carefully chosen to ensure that the concept can be induced, and we found it helpful to take the resulting pragmatic constraints into account.

50 多年前,Bongard 提出了 100 个视觉概念学习问题,作为对人工视觉系统的挑战。这些问题现在被称为 Bongard 问题。尽管这些问题在认知科学和人工智能领域已广为人知,但在构建可解决其中大部分问题的系统方面却进展甚微。在本文介绍的系统中,通过图像处理提取视觉特征,然后将其转化为符号视觉词汇。我们引入了一种形式语言,可以在此词汇的基础上表示组合视觉概念。利用这种语言和贝叶斯推理,可以从每个问题中提供的示例中诱导出概念。我们发现,在 35 个问题的子集中,后验概率较高的概念与 Bongard 自己制定的解决方案之间存在合理的一致性。虽然这种方法远不能像人类一样解决 Bongard 问题,但它比以前的方法要好得多。我们将讨论我们在开发该系统时遇到的问题,以及这些问题对于理解视觉认知的持续意义。例如,与其他概念学习问题不同,Bongard 问题中的示例不是随机的;相反,它们是经过精心挑选的,以确保能够诱导出概念,而且我们发现将由此产生的实用限制考虑在内很有帮助。
{"title":"Solving Bongard Problems With a Visual Language and Pragmatic Constraints","authors":"Stefan Depeweg,&nbsp;Contantin A. Rothkopf,&nbsp;Frank Jäkel","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13432","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13432","url":null,"abstract":"<p>More than 50 years ago, Bongard introduced 100 visual concept learning problems as a challenge for artificial vision systems. These problems are now known as Bongard problems. Although they are well known in cognitive science and artificial intelligence, only very little progress has been made toward building systems that can solve a substantial subset of them. In the system presented here, visual features are extracted through image processing and then translated into a symbolic visual vocabulary. We introduce a formal language that allows representing compositional visual concepts based on this vocabulary. Using this language and Bayesian inference, concepts can be induced from the examples that are provided in each problem. We find a reasonable agreement between the concepts with high posterior probability and the solutions formulated by Bongard himself for a subset of 35 problems. While this approach is far from solving Bongard problems like humans, it does considerably better than previous approaches. We discuss the issues we encountered while developing this system and their continuing relevance for understanding visual cognition. For instance, contrary to other concept learning problems, the examples are not random in Bongard problems; instead they are carefully chosen to ensure that the concept can be induced, and we found it helpful to take the resulting pragmatic constraints into account.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13432","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140820581","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pupils Dilate More to Harder Vocabulary Words than Easier Ones 瞳孔对较难词汇的放大程度大于较易词汇
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-24 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13446
Ishanti Gangopadhyay, Daniel Fulford, Kathleen Corriveau, Jessica Mow, Pearl Han Li, Sudha Arunachalam

Understanding cognitive effort expended during assessments is essential to improving efficiency, accuracy, and accessibility within these assessments. Pupil dilation is commonly used as a psychophysiological measure of cognitive effort, yet research on its relationship with effort expended specifically during language processing is limited. The present study adds to and expands on this literature by investigating the relationships among pupil dilation, trial difficulty, and accuracy during a vocabulary test. Participants (n = 63, Mage = 19.25) completed a subset of trials from the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test while seated at an eye-tracker monitor. During each trial, four colored images were presented on the monitor while a word was presented via audio recording. Participants verbally indicated which image they thought represented the target word. Words were categorized into Easy, Medium, and Hard difficulty. Pupil dilation during the Medium and Hard trials was significantly greater than during the Easy trials, though the Medium and Hard trials did not significantly differ from each other. Pupil dilation in comparison to trial accuracy presented a more complex pattern, with comparisons between accurate and inaccurate trials differing depending on the timing of the stimulus presentation. These results present further evidence that pupil dilation increases with cognitive effort associated with vocabulary tests, providing insights that could help refine vocabulary assessments and other related tests of language processing.

了解评估过程中花费的认知努力对于提高评估的效率、准确性和可及性至关重要。瞳孔放大通常被用作认知努力的心理生理学测量指标,但有关瞳孔放大与语言处理过程中所花费的努力之间关系的研究却十分有限。本研究通过调查词汇测试中瞳孔放大、试题难度和准确性之间的关系,对这些文献进行了补充和扩展。受试者(n = 63,Mage = 19.25)坐在眼动仪监视器前完成了皮博迪图片词汇测试的一部分试验。在每次试验中,监视器上会显示四幅彩色图片,同时通过录音显示一个单词。受试者口头指出他们认为哪张图片代表了目标单词。单词被分为简单、中等和困难三种难度。中等难度和高难度试验中的瞳孔放大程度明显高于简单难度试验,但中等难度和高难度试验之间的差异并不明显。瞳孔放大与试验准确性的比较呈现出一种更为复杂的模式,准确和不准确试验之间的比较因刺激呈现的时间而异。这些结果进一步证明,瞳孔放大会随着词汇测试中认知努力的增加而增加,从而有助于完善词汇评估和其他相关的语言处理测试。
{"title":"Pupils Dilate More to Harder Vocabulary Words than Easier Ones","authors":"Ishanti Gangopadhyay,&nbsp;Daniel Fulford,&nbsp;Kathleen Corriveau,&nbsp;Jessica Mow,&nbsp;Pearl Han Li,&nbsp;Sudha Arunachalam","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13446","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13446","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Understanding cognitive effort expended during assessments is essential to improving efficiency, accuracy, and accessibility within these assessments. Pupil dilation is commonly used as a psychophysiological measure of cognitive effort, yet research on its relationship with effort expended specifically during language processing is limited. The present study adds to and expands on this literature by investigating the relationships among pupil dilation, trial difficulty, and accuracy during a vocabulary test. Participants (<i>n</i> = 63, <i>M<sub>age</sub></i> = 19.25) completed a subset of trials from the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test while seated at an eye-tracker monitor. During each trial, four colored images were presented on the monitor while a word was presented via audio recording. Participants verbally indicated which image they thought represented the target word. Words were categorized into Easy, Medium, and Hard difficulty. Pupil dilation during the Medium and Hard trials was significantly greater than during the Easy trials, though the Medium and Hard trials did not significantly differ from each other. Pupil dilation in comparison to trial accuracy presented a more complex pattern, with comparisons between accurate and inaccurate trials differing depending on the timing of the stimulus presentation. These results present further evidence that pupil dilation increases with cognitive effort associated with vocabulary tests, providing insights that could help refine vocabulary assessments and other related tests of language processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140639506","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1