首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Science最新文献

英文 中文
Does Stimulus Category Coherence Influence Visual Working Memory? A Rational Analysis 刺激类别一致性会影响视觉工作记忆吗?理性分析
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-16 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13498
Ruoyang Hu, Robert A. Jacobs

Visual working memory (VWM) refers to the temporary storage and manipulation of visual information. Although visually different, objects we view and remember can share the same higher-level category information, such as an apple, orange, and banana all being classified as fruit. We study the influence of category information on VWM, focusing on the question of whether stimulus category coherence (i.e., whether all to-be-remembered items belong to the same semantic category) influences VWM performance. This question is addressed in two behavioral experiments using a change-detection paradigm and a rational analysis using an ideal observer based on a Bayesian model. Both experimental participants and the ideal observer often, but not always, performed numerically better on coherent trials (i.e., when all stimuli belonged to the same category). We hypothesize that the influence of category coherence information on VWM may be task-dependent and/or stimulus-dependent. In conditions when category coherence information is highly valuable for task performance, as indicated by the ideal observer, then participants tended to make use of it. However, when the ideal observer suggested this information was not crucial to performance, participants did not. In addition, both participants and the ideal observer showed a bias toward responding “same,” and often showed a stronger influence of category coherence on change trials. The consistencies between participant and ideal observer responses suggest participants often behaved as they did because these behaviors are optimal (or approximately so) for maximizing task performance. This may help explain conflicting results reported in the scientific literature.

视觉工作记忆(VWM)是指对视觉信息的临时存储和处理。虽然视觉效果不同,但我们观看和记忆的对象可以共享相同的高层次类别信息,例如苹果、橙子和香蕉都被归类为水果。我们研究了类别信息对 VWM 的影响,重点是刺激类别的一致性(即是否所有要记忆的项目都属于同一语义类别)是否会影响 VWM 的表现。这个问题在两个行为实验中得到了解决,实验中使用了变化检测范式,并使用基于贝叶斯模型的理想观察者进行了理性分析。实验参与者和理想观察者在连贯试验(即所有刺激物都属于同一类别时)中的表现通常(但不总是)在数字上都更好。我们假设,类别一致性信息对VWM的影响可能与任务相关和/或与刺激相关。在理想观察者所指出的类别一致性信息对任务表现非常有价值的条件下,被试倾向于利用这种信息。然而,当理想观察者认为这种信息对任务表现并不重要时,参与者就不会利用这种信息。此外,参与者和理想观察者都表现出了对 "相同 "的偏向,而且在变化试验中,类别一致性的影响往往更大。参与者和理想观察者反应的一致性表明,参与者的行为往往是因为这些行为是最大化任务表现的最佳行为(或近似行为)。这可能有助于解释科学文献中报道的相互矛盾的结果。
{"title":"Does Stimulus Category Coherence Influence Visual Working Memory? A Rational Analysis","authors":"Ruoyang Hu,&nbsp;Robert A. Jacobs","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13498","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13498","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Visual working memory (VWM) refers to the temporary storage and manipulation of visual information. Although visually different, objects we view and remember can share the same higher-level category information, such as an apple, orange, and banana all being classified as fruit. We study the influence of category information on VWM, focusing on the question of whether stimulus category coherence (i.e., whether all to-be-remembered items belong to the same semantic category) influences VWM performance. This question is addressed in two behavioral experiments using a change-detection paradigm and a rational analysis using an ideal observer based on a Bayesian model. Both experimental participants and the ideal observer often, but not always, performed numerically better on coherent trials (i.e., when all stimuli belonged to the same category). We hypothesize that the influence of category coherence information on VWM may be task-dependent and/or stimulus-dependent. In conditions when category coherence information is highly valuable for task performance, as indicated by the ideal observer, then participants tended to make use of it. However, when the ideal observer suggested this information was not crucial to performance, participants did not. In addition, both participants and the ideal observer showed a bias toward responding “same,” and often showed a stronger influence of category coherence on change trials. The consistencies between participant and ideal observer responses suggest participants often behaved as they did because these behaviors are optimal (or approximately so) for maximizing task performance. This may help explain conflicting results reported in the scientific literature.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 9","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13498","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142234960","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Kin Cognition and Communication: What Talking, Gesturing, and Drawing About Family Can Tell us About the Way We Think About This Core Social Structure 亲属认知与交流:关于家庭的谈话、手势和绘画能告诉我们关于这一核心社会结构的思考方式。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-04 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13484
Simon Devylder, Jennifer Hinnel, Joost van de Weier, Linea Brink Andersen, Lucie Laporte-Devylder, Heron Ken Tomaki Kulukul

When people talk about kinship systems, they often use co-speech gestures and other representations to elaborate. This paper investigates such polysemiotic (spoken, gestured, and drawn) descriptions of kinship relations, to see if they display recurring patterns of conventionalization that capture specific social structures. We present an exploratory hypothesis-generating study of descriptions produced by a lesser-known ethnolinguistic community to the cognitive sciences: the Paamese people of Vanuatu. Forty Paamese speakers were asked to talk about their family in semi-guided kinship interviews. Analyses of the speech, gesture, and drawings produced during these interviews revealed that lineality (i.e., mother's side vs. father's side) is lateralized in the speaker's gesture space. In other words, kinship members of the speaker's matriline are placed on the left side of the speaker's body and those of the patriline are placed on their right side, when they are mentioned in speech. Moreover, we find that the gesture produced by Paamese participants during verbal descriptions of marital relations are performed significantly more often on two diagonal directions of the sagittal axis. We show that these diagonals are also found in the few diagrams that participants drew on the ground to augment their verbo-gestural descriptions of marriage practices with drawing. We interpret this behavior as evidence of a spatial template, which Paamese speakers activate to think and communicate about family relations. We therefore argue that extending investigations of kinship structures beyond kinship terminologies alone can unveil additional key factors that shape kinship cognition and communication and hereby provide further insights into the diversity of social structures.

当人们谈论亲属关系系统时,他们通常会使用共同言语手势和其他表征来进行阐述。本文研究了这种对亲属关系的多语种(口语、手势和图画)描述,看看它们是否显示出捕捉特定社会结构的反复出现的常规化模式。我们对一个认知科学领域鲜为人知的民族语言群体--瓦努阿图的帕阿米人--进行了一项探索性假设研究。在半引导式亲属关系访谈中,40 位讲帕姆语的人被要求谈论他们的家庭。对这些访谈中产生的语言、手势和图画的分析表明,在说话者的手势空间中,线性(即母亲一方与父亲一方)是横向的。换句话说,当说话者的母系亲属被提及时,他们会被放在说话者身体的左侧,而父系亲属则会被放在说话者身体的右侧。此外,我们还发现,在对婚姻关系进行口头描述时,帕姆参与者在矢状轴的两个对角线方向上做出手势的频率明显更高。我们发现,这些对角线也出现在受试者在地上画的几幅图中,这些图是受试者用绘画来增强他们对婚姻习俗的口头描述。我们将这种行为解释为一种空间模板的证据,帕姆塞语使用者在思考和交流家庭关系时会激活这种模板。因此,我们认为,将对亲属关系结构的研究扩展到亲属关系术语之外,可以揭示形成亲属关系认知和交流的其他关键因素,从而为社会结构的多样性提供进一步的见解。
{"title":"Kin Cognition and Communication: What Talking, Gesturing, and Drawing About Family Can Tell us About the Way We Think About This Core Social Structure","authors":"Simon Devylder,&nbsp;Jennifer Hinnel,&nbsp;Joost van de Weier,&nbsp;Linea Brink Andersen,&nbsp;Lucie Laporte-Devylder,&nbsp;Heron Ken Tomaki Kulukul","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13484","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13484","url":null,"abstract":"<p>When people talk about kinship systems, they often use co-speech gestures and other representations to elaborate. This paper investigates such <i>polysemiotic</i> (spoken, gestured, and drawn) descriptions of kinship relations, to see if they display recurring patterns of conventionalization that capture specific social structures. We present an exploratory hypothesis-generating study of descriptions produced by a lesser-known ethnolinguistic community to the cognitive sciences: the Paamese people of Vanuatu. Forty Paamese speakers were asked to talk about their family in semi-guided kinship interviews. Analyses of the speech, gesture, and drawings produced during these interviews revealed that lineality (i.e., mother's side vs. father's side) is lateralized in the speaker's gesture space. In other words, kinship members of the speaker's matriline are placed on the left side of the speaker's body and those of the patriline are placed on their right side, when they are mentioned in speech. Moreover, we find that the gesture produced by Paamese participants during verbal descriptions of marital relations are performed significantly more often on two diagonal directions of the sagittal axis. We show that these diagonals are also found in the few diagrams that participants drew on the ground to augment their verbo-gestural descriptions of marriage practices with drawing. We interpret this behavior as evidence of a spatial template, which Paamese speakers activate to think and communicate about family relations. We therefore argue that extending investigations of kinship structures beyond kinship terminologies alone can unveil additional key factors that shape kinship cognition and communication and hereby provide further insights into the diversity of social structures.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 9","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13484","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142127040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Understanding the Role of Eye Movement Pattern and Consistency in Isolated English Word Reading Through Hidden Markov Modeling 通过隐马尔可夫模型了解眼动模式和一致性在孤立英语单词阅读中的作用。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13489
Weiyan Liao, Janet Hui-wen Hsiao

In isolated English word reading, readers have the optimal performance when their initial eye fixation is directed to the area between the beginning and word center, that is, the optimal viewing position (OVP). Thus, how well readers voluntarily direct eye gaze to this OVP during isolated word reading may be associated with reading performance. Using Eye Movement analysis with Hidden Markov Models, we discovered two representative eye movement patterns during lexical decisions through clustering, which focused at the OVP and the word center, respectively. Higher eye movement similarity to the OVP-focusing pattern predicted faster lexical decision time in addition to cognitive abilities and lexical knowledge. However, the OVP-focusing pattern was associated with longer isolated single letter naming time, suggesting conflicting visual abilities required for identifying isolated letters and multi-letter words. In contrast, in both word and pseudoword naming, although clustering did not reveal an OVP-focused pattern, higher consistency of the first fixation as measured in entropy predicted faster naming time in addition to cognitive abilities and lexical knowledge. Thus, developing a consistent eye movement pattern focusing on the OVP is essential for word orthographic processing and reading fluency. This finding has important implications for interventions for reading difficulties.

在孤立的英语单词阅读中,当读者的初始视线固定在开头和单词中心之间的区域,即最佳注视位置(OVP)时,他们的阅读成绩最佳。因此,在孤立词阅读过程中,读者将眼球注视到这一最佳注视位置的情况可能与阅读成绩有关。利用隐马尔可夫模型进行眼动分析,我们通过聚类发现了词义判断过程中两种有代表性的眼动模式,它们分别集中在OVP和单词中心。除了认知能力和词汇知识外,与 OVP 聚焦模式相似度越高的眼动模式预示着更快的词汇决策时间。然而,OVP 聚焦模式与更长的孤立单个字母命名时间相关,这表明识别孤立字母和多字母单词所需的视觉能力相互冲突。相反,在单词和假词的命名中,虽然聚类没有显示出以 OVP 为中心的模式,但以熵衡量的首次定点的一致性越高,则除了认知能力和词汇知识外,还能预测更快的命名时间。因此,发展以 OVP 为重点的一致眼动模式对单词正字法处理和阅读流畅性至关重要。这一发现对阅读困难的干预具有重要意义。
{"title":"Understanding the Role of Eye Movement Pattern and Consistency in Isolated English Word Reading Through Hidden Markov Modeling","authors":"Weiyan Liao,&nbsp;Janet Hui-wen Hsiao","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13489","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13489","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In isolated English word reading, readers have the optimal performance when their initial eye fixation is directed to the area between the beginning and word center, that is, the optimal viewing position (OVP). Thus, how well readers voluntarily direct eye gaze to this OVP during isolated word reading may be associated with reading performance. Using Eye Movement analysis with Hidden Markov Models, we discovered two representative eye movement patterns during lexical decisions through clustering, which focused at the OVP and the word center, respectively. Higher eye movement similarity to the OVP-focusing pattern predicted faster lexical decision time in addition to cognitive abilities and lexical knowledge. However, the OVP-focusing pattern was associated with longer isolated single letter naming time, suggesting conflicting visual abilities required for identifying isolated letters and multi-letter words. In contrast, in both word and pseudoword naming, although clustering did not reveal an OVP-focused pattern, higher consistency of the first fixation as measured in entropy predicted faster naming time in addition to cognitive abilities and lexical knowledge. Thus, developing a consistent eye movement pattern focusing on the OVP is essential for word orthographic processing and reading fluency. This finding has important implications for interventions for reading difficulties.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 9","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13489","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142127042","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring Early Number Abilities With Multimodal Transformers 利用多模式转换器探索早期数字能力
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13492
Alice Hein, Klaus Diepold

Early number skills represent critical milestones in children's cognitive development and are shaped over years of interacting with quantities and numerals in various contexts. Several connectionist computational models have attempted to emulate how certain number concepts may be learned, represented, and processed in the brain. However, these models mainly used highly simplified inputs and focused on limited tasks. We expand on previous work in two directions: First, we train a model end-to-end on video demonstrations in a synthetic environment with multimodal visual and language inputs. Second, we use a more holistic dataset of 35 tasks, covering enumeration, set comparisons, symbolic digits, and seriation. The order in which the model acquires tasks reflects input length and variability, and the resulting trajectories mostly fit with findings from educational psychology. The trained model also displays symbolic and non-symbolic size and distance effects. Using techniques from interpretability research, we investigate how our attention-based model integrates cross-modal representations and binds them into context-specific associative networks to solve different tasks. We compare models trained with and without symbolic inputs and find that the purely non-symbolic model employs more processing-intensive strategies to determine set size.

早期的数字技能是儿童认知发展的重要里程碑,是多年来在各种情境中与数量和数字互动形成的。一些联结主义计算模型试图模拟某些数字概念是如何在大脑中学习、表示和处理的。然而,这些模型主要使用高度简化的输入,并且只关注有限的任务。我们从两个方向扩展了之前的工作:首先,我们在一个具有多模态视觉和语言输入的合成环境中,通过视频演示对模型进行端到端训练。其次,我们使用了一个包含 35 项任务的更全面的数据集,涵盖了枚举、集合比较、符号数字和序列化。模型获取任务的顺序反映了输入的长度和可变性,由此产生的轨迹大多符合教育心理学的研究结果。训练后的模型还显示出符号和非符号的大小和距离效应。利用可解释性研究的技术,我们研究了基于注意力的模型如何整合跨模态表征,并将它们绑定到特定语境的联想网络中,以解决不同的任务。我们比较了有符号输入和无符号输入的模型,发现纯粹的非符号模型在确定集合大小时采用了更密集的处理策略。
{"title":"Exploring Early Number Abilities With Multimodal Transformers","authors":"Alice Hein,&nbsp;Klaus Diepold","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13492","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13492","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Early number skills represent critical milestones in children's cognitive development and are shaped over years of interacting with quantities and numerals in various contexts. Several connectionist computational models have attempted to emulate how certain number concepts may be learned, represented, and processed in the brain. However, these models mainly used highly simplified inputs and focused on limited tasks. We expand on previous work in two directions: First, we train a model end-to-end on video demonstrations in a synthetic environment with multimodal visual and language inputs. Second, we use a more holistic dataset of 35 tasks, covering enumeration, set comparisons, symbolic digits, and seriation. The order in which the model acquires tasks reflects input length and variability, and the resulting trajectories mostly fit with findings from educational psychology. The trained model also displays symbolic and non-symbolic size and distance effects. Using techniques from interpretability research, we investigate how our attention-based model integrates cross-modal representations and binds them into context-specific associative networks to solve different tasks. We compare models trained with and without symbolic inputs and find that the purely non-symbolic model employs more processing-intensive strategies to determine set size.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 9","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13492","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142127039","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neural Dynamic Principles for an Intentional Embodied Agent 意念化代理的神经动态原理
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13491
Jan Tekülve, Gregor Schöner

How situated embodied agents may achieve goals using knowledge is the classical question of natural and artificial intelligence. How organisms achieve this with their nervous systems is a central challenge for a neural theory of embodied cognition. To structure this challenge, we borrow terms from Searle's analysis of intentionality in its two directions of fit and six psychological modes (perception, memory, belief, intention-in-action, prior intention, desire). We postulate that intentional states are instantiated by neural activation patterns that are stabilized by neural interaction. Dynamic instabilities provide the neural mechanism for initiating and terminating intentional states and are critical to organizing sequences of intentional states. Beliefs represented by networks of concept nodes are autonomously learned and activated in response to desired outcomes. The neural dynamic principles of an intentional agent are demonstrated in a toy scenario in which a robotic agent explores an environment and paints objects in desired colors based on learned color transformation rules.

情景具身代理如何利用知识实现目标是自然和人工智能的经典问题。生物如何利用其神经系统实现这一目标,是具身认知神经理论面临的核心挑战。为了构建这一挑战,我们借用了塞尔(Searle)对意向性的分析中的两个契合方向和六种心理模式(感知、记忆、信念、行动意向、先验意向、欲望)的术语。我们推测,意向状态是由神经激活模式实例化的,而神经激活模式则通过神经交互作用得以稳定。动态不稳定性提供了启动和终止意向状态的神经机制,对于组织意向状态序列至关重要。概念节点网络所代表的信念可以自主学习,并根据预期结果激活。意向代理的神经动态原理在一个玩具场景中得到了展示,在该场景中,机器人代理探索环境,并根据学习到的颜色转换规则将物体涂成所需的颜色。
{"title":"Neural Dynamic Principles for an Intentional Embodied Agent","authors":"Jan Tekülve,&nbsp;Gregor Schöner","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13491","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13491","url":null,"abstract":"<p>How situated embodied agents may achieve goals using knowledge is the classical question of natural and artificial intelligence. How organisms achieve this with their nervous systems is a central challenge for a neural theory of embodied cognition. To structure this challenge, we borrow terms from Searle's analysis of intentionality in its two directions of fit and six psychological modes (perception, memory, belief, intention-in-action, prior intention, desire). We postulate that intentional states are instantiated by neural activation patterns that are stabilized by neural interaction. Dynamic instabilities provide the neural mechanism for initiating and terminating intentional states and are critical to organizing sequences of intentional states. Beliefs represented by networks of concept nodes are autonomously learned and activated in response to desired outcomes. The neural dynamic principles of an intentional agent are demonstrated in a toy scenario in which a robotic agent explores an environment and paints objects in desired colors based on learned color transformation rules.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 9","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13491","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142127041","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reasoning From Quantified Modal Premises 从量化模态前提推理。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-19 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13485
Ana Cristina Quelhas, Célia Rasga, P. N. Johnson-Laird

Quantified modal inferences interest logicians, linguists, and computer scientists, but no previous psychological study of them appears to be in the literature. Here is an example of one:

People tend to conclude: Paulo is possibly a businessman (Experiment 1). It seems plausible, and it follows from an intuitive mental model in which Paulo is one of a set of artists who are businessmen. Further deliberation can yield a model of an alternative possibility in which Paulo is not one of the artists, which confirms that the conclusion is only a possibility. The snag is that standard modal logics, which deal with possibilities, cannot yield a particular conclusion to any premises: Infinitely many follow validly (from any premises) but they do not include the present conclusion. Yet, further experiments corroborated a new mental model theory's predictions for various inferences (Experiment 2), for the occurrence of factual conclusions drawn from premises about possibilities (Experiment 3) and for inferences from premises of modal syllogisms (Experiment 4). The theory is therefore plausible, but we explore the feasibility of a cognitive theory based on modifications to modal logic.

量化模态推理引起了逻辑学家、语言学家和计算机科学家的兴趣,但以前的文献中似乎还没有关于量化模态推理的心理学研究。下面是一个例子:所有这些艺术家都是商人。保罗可能是艺术家之一。然后呢?人们倾向于得出结论:保罗可能是商人(实验 1)。这似乎是可信的,而且它来自于一个直观的心智模型,在这个模型中,保罗是一群身为商人的艺术家之一。进一步斟酌可以得出另一种可能性的模型,即保罗不是艺术家之一,这就证实了结论只是一种可能性。问题在于,处理可能性的标准模态逻辑无法根据任何前提得出特定的结论:从任何前提都可以得出无限多的有效结论,但其中并不包括现在的结论。然而,进一步的实验证实了新的心智模式理论对各种推论(实验 2)、从关于可能性的前提得出的事实结论(实验 3)和从模态三段论的前提得出的推论(实验 4)的预测。因此,该理论是可信的,但我们探讨了基于模态逻辑修正的认知理论的可行性。
{"title":"Reasoning From Quantified Modal Premises","authors":"Ana Cristina Quelhas,&nbsp;Célia Rasga,&nbsp;P. N. Johnson-Laird","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13485","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13485","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Quantified modal inferences interest logicians, linguists, and computer scientists, but no previous psychological study of them appears to be in the literature. Here is an example of one:\u0000\u0000 </p><p>People tend to conclude: <i>Paulo is possibly a businessman</i> (Experiment 1). It seems plausible, and it follows from an intuitive mental model in which Paulo is one of a set of artists who are businessmen. Further deliberation can yield a model of an alternative possibility in which Paulo is not one of the artists, which confirms that the conclusion is only a possibility. The snag is that standard modal logics, which deal with possibilities, cannot yield a particular conclusion to any premises: Infinitely many follow validly (from any premises) but they do not include the present conclusion. Yet, further experiments corroborated a new mental model theory's predictions for various inferences (Experiment 2), for the occurrence of factual conclusions drawn from premises about possibilities (Experiment 3) and for inferences from premises of modal syllogisms (Experiment 4). The theory is therefore plausible, but we explore the feasibility of a cognitive theory based on modifications to modal logic.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 8","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142005591","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Partner-Specific Adaptation in Disfluency Processing 流畅性处理中的伙伴特定适应性
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-19 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13490
Si On Yoon, Sarah Brown-Schmidt

Speakers tend to produce disfluencies when naming unexpected or complex items; in turn, when perceiving disfluency, listeners tend to expect upcoming reference to items that are unexpected or complex to name. In two experiments, we examined if these disfluency-based expectations are routine, or instead, if they adapt to the way the speaker uses disfluency in the current context in a talker-specific manner. Participants listened to instructions to look at objects in contexts with several images, some of which lacked conventional names. We manipulated the co-occurrence of disfluency and reference to novel versus familiar objects in a single talker situation (Experiment 1) and in a multi-talker situation (Experiment 2). In the predictive condition, disfluent expressions referred to novel objects, and fluent expressions referred to familiar objects. In the nonpredictive condition, fluent and disfluent trials referred to either familiar or novel objects. Participants’ gaze revealed that listeners more readily predicted familiar images for fluent trials and novel images for disfluent trials in the predictive condition than in the nonpredictive condition. In sum, listeners adapted their expectations about upcoming words based on recent experience with disfluency. Disfluency is not invariably processed, but instead a cue that is flexibly interpreted depending on the local context even in a multi-talker setting.

在命名意外或复杂的项目时,说话者往往会产生不连贯现象;反过来,在感知到不连贯现象时,听话者往往会预期即将提到意外或复杂的项目。在两个实验中,我们研究了这些基于不连贯的预期是否是常规的,或者它们是否以说话者特有的方式适应说话者在当前语境中使用不连贯的方式。受试者聆听指令,在有多个图像的语境中观察物体,其中一些图像没有常规名称。我们分别在单个谈话者情境(实验 1)和多个谈话者情境(实验 2)中操纵了不连贯和对新物体与熟悉物体的提及的共同出现。在预测条件下,不流利的表达指的是新事物,而流利的表达指的是熟悉的事物。在非预测条件下,流利和不流利的试验均指熟悉或新奇的对象。受试者的注视表明,在预测条件下,听者比在非预测条件下更容易预测到流畅试验中的熟悉图像和不流畅试验中的新奇图像。总之,听者会根据最近的不流畅经验调整他们对即将出现的词语的预期。不连贯并不是一成不变的,而是一种线索,即使在多人交谈的情况下,也会根据当地的语境灵活解释。
{"title":"Partner-Specific Adaptation in Disfluency Processing","authors":"Si On Yoon,&nbsp;Sarah Brown-Schmidt","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13490","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13490","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Speakers tend to produce disfluencies when naming unexpected or complex items; in turn, when perceiving disfluency, listeners tend to expect upcoming reference to items that are unexpected or complex to name. In two experiments, we examined if these disfluency-based expectations are routine, or instead, if they adapt to the way the speaker uses disfluency in the current context in a talker-specific manner. Participants listened to instructions to look at objects in contexts with several images, some of which lacked conventional names. We manipulated the co-occurrence of disfluency and reference to novel versus familiar objects in a single talker situation (Experiment 1) and in a multi-talker situation (Experiment 2). In the predictive condition, disfluent expressions referred to novel objects, and fluent expressions referred to familiar objects. In the nonpredictive condition, fluent and disfluent trials referred to either familiar or novel objects. Participants’ gaze revealed that listeners more readily predicted familiar images for fluent trials and novel images for disfluent trials in the predictive condition than in the nonpredictive condition. In sum, listeners adapted their expectations about upcoming words based on recent experience with disfluency. Disfluency is not invariably processed, but instead a cue that is flexibly interpreted depending on the local context even in a multi-talker setting.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 8","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142005590","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
High-Pitched Sound is Open and Low-Pitched Sound is Closed: Representing the Spatial Meaning of Pitch Height 高音是开放的,低音是封闭的:表现音高的空间意义。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13486
Lari Vainio, Ida-Lotta Myllylä, Alexandra Wikström, Martti Vainio

Research shows that high- and low-pitch sounds can be associated with various meanings. For example, high-pitch sounds are associated with small concepts, whereas low-pitch sounds are associated with large concepts. This study presents three experiments revealing that high-pitch sounds are also associated with open concepts and opening hand actions, while low-pitch sounds are associated with closed concepts and closing hand actions. In Experiment 1, this sound-meaning correspondence effect was shown using the two-alternative forced-choice task, while Experiments 2 and 3 used reaction time tasks to show this interaction. In Experiment 2, high-pitch vocalizations were found to facilitate opening hand gestures, and low-pitch vocalizations were found to facilitate closing hand gestures, when performed simultaneously. In Experiment 3, high-pitched vocalizations were produced particularly rapidly when the visual target stimulus presented an open object, and low-pitched vocalizations were produced particularly rapidly when the target presented a closed object. These findings are discussed concerning the meaning of intonational cues. They are suggested to be based on cross-modally representing conceptual spatial knowledge in sensory, motor, and affective systems. Additionally, this pitch-opening effect might share cognitive processes with other pitch-meaning effects.

研究表明,高音调和低音调的声音可以与不同的含义相关联。例如,高音调的声音与小概念相关,而低音调的声音与大概念相关。本研究通过三个实验揭示了高音调声音也与开放概念和张手动作相关,而低音调声音则与封闭概念和收手动作相关。在实验 1 中,这种音义对应效应是通过双选项强迫选择任务来体现的,而实验 2 和实验 3 则是通过反应时间任务来体现这种交互作用。在实验 2 中,发现同时进行高音调发声和低音调发声时,高音调发声有利于张开手势,而低音调发声有利于收拢手势。在实验 3 中,当视觉目标刺激呈现开放物体时,高音调发声尤其迅速,而当目标呈现封闭物体时,低音调发声尤其迅速。这些发现与语调线索的意义有关。这些发现被认为是基于在感觉、运动和情感系统中跨模态表现概念空间知识。此外,这种音调开阔效应可能与其他音调意义效应共享认知过程。
{"title":"High-Pitched Sound is Open and Low-Pitched Sound is Closed: Representing the Spatial Meaning of Pitch Height","authors":"Lari Vainio,&nbsp;Ida-Lotta Myllylä,&nbsp;Alexandra Wikström,&nbsp;Martti Vainio","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13486","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13486","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Research shows that high- and low-pitch sounds can be associated with various meanings. For example, high-pitch sounds are associated with small concepts, whereas low-pitch sounds are associated with large concepts. This study presents three experiments revealing that high-pitch sounds are also associated with open concepts and opening hand actions, while low-pitch sounds are associated with closed concepts and closing hand actions. In Experiment 1, this sound-meaning correspondence effect was shown using the two-alternative forced-choice task, while Experiments 2 and 3 used reaction time tasks to show this interaction. In Experiment 2, high-pitch vocalizations were found to facilitate opening hand gestures, and low-pitch vocalizations were found to facilitate closing hand gestures, when performed simultaneously. In Experiment 3, high-pitched vocalizations were produced particularly rapidly when the visual target stimulus presented an open object, and low-pitched vocalizations were produced particularly rapidly when the target presented a closed object. These findings are discussed concerning the meaning of intonational cues. They are suggested to be based on cross-modally representing conceptual spatial knowledge in sensory, motor, and affective systems. Additionally, this pitch-opening effect might share cognitive processes with other pitch-meaning effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 8","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13486","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142001072","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring How Generating Metaphor Via Insight Versus Analysis Affects Metaphor Quality and Learning Outcomes 探索通过洞察和分析产生隐喻如何影响隐喻质量和学习效果。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13488
Yuhua Yu, Lindsay Krebs, Mark Beeman, Vicky T. Lai

Metaphor generation is both a creative act and a means of learning. When learning a new concept, people often create a metaphor to connect the new concept to existing knowledge. Does the manner in which people generate a metaphor, via sudden insight (Aha! moment) or deliberate analysis, influence the quality of generation and subsequent learning outcomes? According to some research, deliberate processing enhances knowledge retention; hence, generation via analysis likely leads to better concept learning. However, other research has shown that solutions generated via insight are better remembered. In the current study, participants were presented with science concepts and descriptions, then generated metaphors for the concepts. They also indicated how they generated each metaphor and rated their metaphor for novelty and aptness. We assessed participants’ learning outcomes with a memory test and evaluated the creative quality of the metaphors based on self- and crowd-sourced ratings. Consistent with the deliberate processing benefit, participants became more familiar with the target science concept if they previously generated a metaphor for the concept via analysis compared to via insight. We also found that metaphors generated via analysis did not differ from metaphors generated via insight in quality (aptness or novelty) nor in how well they were remembered. However, participants’ self-evaluations of metaphors generated via insight showed more agreement with independent raters, suggesting the role of insight in modulating the creative ideation process. These preliminary findings have implications for understanding the nature of insight during idea generation and its impact on learning.

隐喻的产生既是一种创造行为,也是一种学习手段。在学习一个新概念时,人们通常会创造一个隐喻,将新概念与现有知识联系起来。人们产生隐喻的方式,是通过突然的洞察力("啊哈!"时刻)还是经过深思熟虑的分析,是否会影响隐喻产生的质量和随后的学习效果?一些研究表明,深思熟虑的处理方式会增强知识的保留;因此,通过分析产生隐喻可能会带来更好的概念学习。然而,其他研究表明,通过洞察力生成的解决方案记忆效果更好。在当前的研究中,研究人员向参与者展示了科学概念和描述,然后为概念生成隐喻。他们还说明了自己是如何产生每个隐喻的,并对隐喻的新颖性和恰当性进行了评分。我们通过记忆测试评估了参与者的学习成果,并根据自我评分和群众评分评估了隐喻的创意质量。与刻意加工的益处相一致的是,与洞察力相比,如果参与者之前通过分析为目标科学概念生成了隐喻,那么他们对该概念就会更加熟悉。我们还发现,通过分析产生的隐喻与通过洞察产生的隐喻在质量(恰当性或新颖性)和记忆程度上并无不同。然而,参与者对通过洞察力产生的隐喻的自我评价与独立评分者的一致程度更高,这表明洞察力在调节创造性构思过程中的作用。这些初步研究结果对于理解创意产生过程中洞察力的性质及其对学习的影响具有重要意义。
{"title":"Exploring How Generating Metaphor Via Insight Versus Analysis Affects Metaphor Quality and Learning Outcomes","authors":"Yuhua Yu,&nbsp;Lindsay Krebs,&nbsp;Mark Beeman,&nbsp;Vicky T. Lai","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13488","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13488","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Metaphor generation is both a creative act and a means of learning. When learning a new concept, people often create a metaphor to connect the new concept to existing knowledge. Does the manner in which people generate a metaphor, via sudden insight (Aha! moment) or deliberate analysis, influence the quality of generation and subsequent learning outcomes? According to some research, deliberate processing enhances knowledge retention; hence, generation via analysis likely leads to better concept learning. However, other research has shown that solutions generated via insight are better remembered. In the current study, participants were presented with science concepts and descriptions, then generated metaphors for the concepts. They also indicated how they generated each metaphor and rated their metaphor for novelty and aptness. We assessed participants’ learning outcomes with a memory test and evaluated the creative quality of the metaphors based on self- and crowd-sourced ratings. Consistent with the deliberate processing benefit, participants became more familiar with the target science concept if they previously generated a metaphor for the concept via analysis compared to via insight. We also found that metaphors generated via analysis did not differ from metaphors generated via insight in quality (aptness or novelty) nor in how well they were remembered. However, participants’ self-evaluations of metaphors generated via insight showed more agreement with independent raters, suggesting the role of insight in modulating the creative ideation process. These preliminary findings have implications for understanding the nature of insight during idea generation and its impact on learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 8","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13488","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142001071","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can Large Language Models Counter the Recent Decline in Literacy Levels? An Important Role for Cognitive Science 大型语言模型能否应对近期识字水平的下降?认知科学的重要作用。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13487
Falk Huettig, Morten H. Christiansen

Literacy is in decline in many parts of the world, accompanied by drops in associated cognitive skills (including IQ) and an increasing susceptibility to fake news. It is possible that the recent explosive growth and widespread deployment of Large Language Models (LLMs) might exacerbate this trend, but there is also a chance that LLMs can help turn things around. We argue that cognitive science is ideally suited to help steer future literacy development in the right direction by challenging and informing current educational practices and policy. Cognitive scientists have the right interdisciplinary skills to study, analyze, evaluate, and change LLMs to facilitate their critical use, to encourage turn-taking that promotes rather than hinders literacy, to support literacy acquisition in diverse and equitable ways, and to scaffold potential future changes in what it means to be literate. We urge cognitive scientists to take up this mantle—the future impact of LLMs on human literacy skills is too important to be left to the large, predominately U.S.-based tech companies.

在世界许多地方,识字率正在下降,伴随而来的是相关认知技能(包括智商)的下降和对假新闻的日益敏感。最近大语言模型(LLM)的爆炸式增长和广泛应用有可能加剧这一趋势,但也有可能帮助扭转局面。我们认为,认知科学非常适合通过挑战和指导当前的教育实践和政策,帮助引导未来的扫盲朝着正确的方向发展。认知科学家拥有适当的跨学科技能来研究、分析、评估和改变识字方法,以促进其批判性使用,鼓励促进而不是阻碍识字的轮流学习,以多样化和公平的方式支持识字学习,并为未来识字方法的潜在变革提供支架。我们敦促认知科学家承担起这一重任--未来扫盲对人类读写能力的影响太重要了,不能留给以美国为主的大型科技公司。
{"title":"Can Large Language Models Counter the Recent Decline in Literacy Levels? An Important Role for Cognitive Science","authors":"Falk Huettig,&nbsp;Morten H. Christiansen","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13487","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13487","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Literacy is in decline in many parts of the world, accompanied by drops in associated cognitive skills (including IQ) and an increasing susceptibility to fake news. It is possible that the recent explosive growth and widespread deployment of Large Language Models (LLMs) might exacerbate this trend, but there is also a chance that LLMs can help turn things around. We argue that cognitive science is ideally suited to help steer future literacy development in the right direction by challenging and informing current educational practices and policy. Cognitive scientists have the right interdisciplinary skills to study, analyze, evaluate, and change LLMs to facilitate their critical use, to encourage turn-taking that promotes rather than hinders literacy, to support literacy acquisition in diverse and equitable ways, and to scaffold potential future changes in what it means to be literate. We urge cognitive scientists to take up this mantle—the future impact of LLMs on human literacy skills is too important to be left to the large, predominately U.S.-based tech companies.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":"48 8","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142001070","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1