Jennifer Watling Neal, Zachary P Neal, Brian Brutzman, C Emily Durbin
Preschoolers are expected to transition from parallel play, where they engage in similar activities next to peers, to social play, where they engage in direct interactions with peers. We use longitudinal, multiplex social network analysis to examine the transition between observed parallel and social play over a school year in a 3-year-old classroom (N = 25, 45% girls, 48% White) and a 4-year-old classroom (N = 28, 42.86% girls, 60.71% White). In both classrooms, the existence of a parallel play relationship between two children predicted the formation of a social play relationship between the same two children over time but not vice versa. Findings provide support for a unidirectional, sequential transition from parallel to social play with the same peers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"Understanding the developmental transition between parallel and social play during preschool: A multiplex social network analysis.","authors":"Jennifer Watling Neal, Zachary P Neal, Brian Brutzman, C Emily Durbin","doi":"10.1037/dev0001837","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001837","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Preschoolers are expected to transition from parallel play, where they engage in similar activities next to peers, to social play, where they engage in direct interactions with peers. We use longitudinal, multiplex social network analysis to examine the transition between observed parallel and social play over a school year in a 3-year-old classroom (<i>N</i> = 25, 45% girls, 48% White) and a 4-year-old classroom (<i>N</i> = 28, 42.86% girls, 60.71% White). In both classrooms, the existence of a parallel play relationship between two children predicted the formation of a social play relationship between the same two children over time but not vice versa. Findings provide support for a unidirectional, sequential transition from parallel to social play with the same peers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298907","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
S Alexandra Burt, Sarah Carroll, Elizabeth A Shewark, Kelly L Klump, Jenae M Neiderhiser, Luke W Hyde
Although the parent-child relationship is widely regarded as a foundational context for youth development, the developmental origins of this relationship remain unknown. The present study addressed these gaps, leveraging longitudinal and genetically informed methods to illuminate the developmental origins of mother-child conflict as it unfolds from middle childhood into emerging adulthood. Participants consisted of 2,060 twins in 1,030 twin families (51% male, 49% female; 82% White, 10% Black, 1% Asian, 1% Indigenous, 6% multiracial) from the Michigan State University Twin Registry. Families were assessed up to five times. We fitted a series of latent growth curve models (univariate and parallel process) to data from mothers and children, after which we estimated genetic and environmental sources of variance within and covariance among the intercepts and slopes. Parallel process analyses indicated that maternal reports of conflict at baseline shaped their own and their children's perceptions of change in conflict over time but that children's reports of conflict at baseline predicted only their own rate of change in conflict. Subsequent biometric analyses indicated substantial environmental contributions to the intercepts in childhood, as well as prominent environmental origins to the overlap between maternal and child intercepts. By contrast, we observed robust genetically influenced child effects on maternal rate of change and on the association between the maternal and child slopes. Such findings collectively illuminate the dynamic and relational nature of mother-child conflict from childhood into emerging adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"The dynamic and relational nature of parent-child conflict from childhood into emerging adulthood.","authors":"S Alexandra Burt, Sarah Carroll, Elizabeth A Shewark, Kelly L Klump, Jenae M Neiderhiser, Luke W Hyde","doi":"10.1037/dev0001847","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001847","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although the parent-child relationship is widely regarded as a foundational context for youth development, the developmental origins of this relationship remain unknown. The present study addressed these gaps, leveraging longitudinal and genetically informed methods to illuminate the developmental origins of mother-child conflict as it unfolds from middle childhood into emerging adulthood. Participants consisted of 2,060 twins in 1,030 twin families (51% male, 49% female; 82% White, 10% Black, 1% Asian, 1% Indigenous, 6% multiracial) from the Michigan State University Twin Registry. Families were assessed up to five times. We fitted a series of latent growth curve models (univariate and parallel process) to data from mothers and children, after which we estimated genetic and environmental sources of variance within and covariance among the intercepts and slopes. Parallel process analyses indicated that maternal reports of conflict at baseline shaped their own and their children's perceptions of change in conflict over time but that children's reports of conflict at baseline predicted only their own rate of change in conflict. Subsequent biometric analyses indicated substantial environmental contributions to the intercepts in childhood, as well as prominent environmental origins to the overlap between maternal and child intercepts. By contrast, we observed robust genetically influenced child effects on maternal rate of change and on the association between the maternal and child slopes. Such findings collectively illuminate the dynamic and relational nature of mother-child conflict from childhood into emerging adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Punishment is a key mechanism to regulate selfish behaviors and maintain cooperation in a society. However, children often show mixed evaluations about third-party punishment. The current work asked how punishment severity might shape children's social judgments. In two studies, 5- to 10-year-old children heard about a punisher who took different numbers of items from a transgressor and evaluated the punisher's behavior and moral character. In Study 1 (n = 68), when the transgression was relatively mild (i.e., unfair sharing), children across ages evaluated taking no items from the unfair sharer ("no punishment") most positively, while evaluating taking three items ("harshest punishment") most negatively. In Study 2 (n = 68), when the transgression was more serious (i.e., stealing), younger children evaluated taking two items ("equality-establishing punishment") more positively than older children, while evaluating taking none most negatively. However, children became more likely to evaluate equality-establishing punishment negatively with age. Overall, the current results show that punishment severity is a key factor underlying children's third-party punishment judgments. The current research extends work on moral development by showing how children conceptualize the severity of punishment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"Not all punishment is equal: The effect of punishment severity on children's social evaluations.","authors":"Young-Eun Lee, Larisa Heiphetz Solomon","doi":"10.1037/dev0001845","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001845","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Punishment is a key mechanism to regulate selfish behaviors and maintain cooperation in a society. However, children often show mixed evaluations about third-party punishment. The current work asked how punishment severity might shape children's social judgments. In two studies, 5- to 10-year-old children heard about a punisher who took different numbers of items from a transgressor and evaluated the punisher's behavior and moral character. In Study 1 (<i>n</i> = 68), when the transgression was relatively mild (i.e., unfair sharing), children across ages evaluated taking no items from the unfair sharer (\"no punishment\") most positively, while evaluating taking three items (\"harshest punishment\") most negatively. In Study 2 (<i>n</i> = 68), when the transgression was more serious (i.e., stealing), younger children evaluated taking two items (\"equality-establishing punishment\") more positively than older children, while evaluating taking none most negatively. However, children became more likely to evaluate equality-establishing punishment negatively with age. Overall, the current results show that punishment severity is a key factor underlying children's third-party punishment judgments. The current research extends work on moral development by showing how children conceptualize the severity of punishment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298903","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Natasha Chaku,Sheri A Berenbaum,Yiming Qian,Robin P Corley,Sally J Wadsworth,Chandra A Reynolds,Adriene M Beltz
Pubertal development has short- and long-term effects on psychological adjustment. Many studies of long-term effects rely on retrospective measurement of pubertal timing, but such measures often reflect different aspects of puberty than those assessed in adolescence, raising questions about the utility and interpretation of retrospective reports. The present study leveraged longitudinal data collected in adolescence and established adulthood to determine: (1) the correspondence between pubertal timing indexed from logistic growth curves of self-reported physical development assessed contemporaneously across adolescence and pubertal timing indexed relative to peers assessed retrospectively in adulthood; (2) the associations between the two pubertal timing measures and psychological adjustment; and (3) potential recall biases. Participants were 748 individuals (50.1% female; 91.6% White) from two longitudinal studies who reported on their pubertal development annually from Grades 3-9, psychological adjustment (age at sexual initiation, substance use, depression) in late adolescence, and retrospective pubertal timing in established adulthood (Mage = 32.76; SD = 4.43). Results indicate moderate-to-high convergence between retrospective and contemporaneous indices. Most participants, especially women, had the same pubertal timing classification (i.e., early, on time, or late), but early-maturing adolescents often recalled on-time development as adults. Retrospective and contemporaneous indices were associated with psychological adjustment in similar ways, with some attenuation in the retrospective measure, especially for men. There was little evidence of recall bias due to age at retrospective assessment or time since puberty. Findings generally support the use of retrospective pubertal timing measures, with the recognition that some relations with adjustment may be weakened. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"Pubertal timing in adolescence and adulthood: Relations among contemporaneous and retrospective measures.","authors":"Natasha Chaku,Sheri A Berenbaum,Yiming Qian,Robin P Corley,Sally J Wadsworth,Chandra A Reynolds,Adriene M Beltz","doi":"10.1037/dev0001784","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001784","url":null,"abstract":"Pubertal development has short- and long-term effects on psychological adjustment. Many studies of long-term effects rely on retrospective measurement of pubertal timing, but such measures often reflect different aspects of puberty than those assessed in adolescence, raising questions about the utility and interpretation of retrospective reports. The present study leveraged longitudinal data collected in adolescence and established adulthood to determine: (1) the correspondence between pubertal timing indexed from logistic growth curves of self-reported physical development assessed contemporaneously across adolescence and pubertal timing indexed relative to peers assessed retrospectively in adulthood; (2) the associations between the two pubertal timing measures and psychological adjustment; and (3) potential recall biases. Participants were 748 individuals (50.1% female; 91.6% White) from two longitudinal studies who reported on their pubertal development annually from Grades 3-9, psychological adjustment (age at sexual initiation, substance use, depression) in late adolescence, and retrospective pubertal timing in established adulthood (Mage = 32.76; SD = 4.43). Results indicate moderate-to-high convergence between retrospective and contemporaneous indices. Most participants, especially women, had the same pubertal timing classification (i.e., early, on time, or late), but early-maturing adolescents often recalled on-time development as adults. Retrospective and contemporaneous indices were associated with psychological adjustment in similar ways, with some attenuation in the retrospective measure, especially for men. There was little evidence of recall bias due to age at retrospective assessment or time since puberty. Findings generally support the use of retrospective pubertal timing measures, with the recognition that some relations with adjustment may be weakened. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":"19 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142211900","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Glona Lee-Poon,Jacquelynne S Eccles,Sandra D Simpkins
The changes in adolescents' math motivational beliefs (i.e., expectancies for success, interest, and utility value) across Grades 9-11 and the associations between these changes and adolescents' experiences with socializers (i.e., perceived teacher unfairness and parent-adolescent discussions) were examined within each of the four largest racial/ethnic groups in the United States using the High School Longitudinal Study, a nationally representative data set (n = 19,010; 50% female; 9% Asian; 11% Black; 18% Latine; 62% White; Mage = 14.53 in Grade 9). Cross-tabulation analyses suggested that similar developmental trends emerged within each racial/ethnic group (which were tested separately). Many adolescents maintained their high or low expectancies, interest, and utility values across Grades 9-11. Some patterns varied by belief; for example, several adolescents switched from high to low interest by Grade 11, whereas several adolescents switched from low to high utility value. Parent-adolescent discussions predicted positive changes among Asian and Latine adolescents, whereas perceived teacher unfairness predicted negative changes among Black adolescents. The findings from the present study highlight the diverse developmental trends in adolescents' motivational beliefs and the potential role of socializers as sources of strength or challenge in their motivational belief development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
我们利用具有全国代表性的数据集《高中纵向研究》(High School Longitudinal Study)(n = 19010;50% 女性;9% 亚洲人;11% 黑人;18% 拉丁人;62% 白人;9 年级学生平均年龄 = 14.53),对美国四大种族/民族群体中的每一个群体进行了研究。交叉分析表明,每个种族/族裔群体(分别进行测试)都出现了类似的发展趋向。许多青少年在九年级至十一年级期间保持着或高或低的期望值、兴趣值和效用值。一些模式因信仰而异;例如,一些青少年在十一年级时从高兴趣转为低兴趣,而一些青少年则从低效用值转为高效用值。家长与青少年的讨论预示着亚裔和拉丁裔青少年的积极变化,而认为教师不公平则预示着黑人青少年的消极变化。本研究的结果凸显了青少年动机信念的不同发展趋向,以及社会化者在青少年动机信念发展过程中作为力量来源或挑战来源的潜在作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Math motivational belief development during high school by race/ethnicity: Teachers and parents as predictors of changes.","authors":"Glona Lee-Poon,Jacquelynne S Eccles,Sandra D Simpkins","doi":"10.1037/dev0001823","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001823","url":null,"abstract":"The changes in adolescents' math motivational beliefs (i.e., expectancies for success, interest, and utility value) across Grades 9-11 and the associations between these changes and adolescents' experiences with socializers (i.e., perceived teacher unfairness and parent-adolescent discussions) were examined within each of the four largest racial/ethnic groups in the United States using the High School Longitudinal Study, a nationally representative data set (n = 19,010; 50% female; 9% Asian; 11% Black; 18% Latine; 62% White; Mage = 14.53 in Grade 9). Cross-tabulation analyses suggested that similar developmental trends emerged within each racial/ethnic group (which were tested separately). Many adolescents maintained their high or low expectancies, interest, and utility values across Grades 9-11. Some patterns varied by belief; for example, several adolescents switched from high to low interest by Grade 11, whereas several adolescents switched from low to high utility value. Parent-adolescent discussions predicted positive changes among Asian and Latine adolescents, whereas perceived teacher unfairness predicted negative changes among Black adolescents. The findings from the present study highlight the diverse developmental trends in adolescents' motivational beliefs and the potential role of socializers as sources of strength or challenge in their motivational belief development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":"20 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142211906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
The ability to narrate a life coherently develops first in adolescence, but little is known about its course across adulthood and into old age. Also, the potential association of life narrative coherence with well-being has never been studied. Our aim was to investigate lifespan development of coherence and associations with well-being using data from the longitudinal MainLife study. A total of 172 urban Germans (8-80 years; 87 females) narrated their lives up to six times across 16 years (N = 803 brief entire life narratives). Most were highly educated, and the proportion of a migration background was typical for the local population. Life narratives were rated for three types of global coherence (temporal, causal-motivational, and thematic) and coded for two types of autobiographical arguments (stability maintaining and change engendering). Multilevel models were used to investigate their lifespan development and relations to well-being. While most life narrative measures increased up to emerging adulthood and then remained stable, thematic coherence continued to increase into middle adulthood. Only change-engendering autobiographical arguments slowly decreased from midlife onward. Unexpectedly, neither autobiographical arguments nor global coherence correlated significantly with well-being. Data exploration suggested an association between thematic coherence and self-continuity. We conclude that life narrative coherence may only be related to well-being if samples include cases with more extreme noncoherence (e,g., clinical disorders). Our findings add to understanding the development of the life story across the lifespan, especially in older age, and suggest studying relations of coherence, self-continuity, and well-being specifically in life crises and in clinical samples. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"Narrating lives across 16 years: Developmental trajectories of coherence and relations to well-being in a lifespan sample.","authors":"Isabel Peters,Florian Schmiedek,Tilmann Habermas","doi":"10.1037/dev0001775","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001775","url":null,"abstract":"The ability to narrate a life coherently develops first in adolescence, but little is known about its course across adulthood and into old age. Also, the potential association of life narrative coherence with well-being has never been studied. Our aim was to investigate lifespan development of coherence and associations with well-being using data from the longitudinal MainLife study. A total of 172 urban Germans (8-80 years; 87 females) narrated their lives up to six times across 16 years (N = 803 brief entire life narratives). Most were highly educated, and the proportion of a migration background was typical for the local population. Life narratives were rated for three types of global coherence (temporal, causal-motivational, and thematic) and coded for two types of autobiographical arguments (stability maintaining and change engendering). Multilevel models were used to investigate their lifespan development and relations to well-being. While most life narrative measures increased up to emerging adulthood and then remained stable, thematic coherence continued to increase into middle adulthood. Only change-engendering autobiographical arguments slowly decreased from midlife onward. Unexpectedly, neither autobiographical arguments nor global coherence correlated significantly with well-being. Data exploration suggested an association between thematic coherence and self-continuity. We conclude that life narrative coherence may only be related to well-being if samples include cases with more extreme noncoherence (e,g., clinical disorders). Our findings add to understanding the development of the life story across the lifespan, especially in older age, and suggest studying relations of coherence, self-continuity, and well-being specifically in life crises and in clinical samples. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":"58 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142211907","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Juan Del Toro,Kamilah Legette,N Keita Christophe,Michelle Pasco,Dana Miller-Cotto,Ming-Te Wang
Ethnic-racial discrimination is a pernicious experience that affects discriminated adolescents' healthy human development, but the spillover consequences of discrimination on the nondiscriminated adolescent population are less clear. Adolescents who vicariously witness their classmates experience ethnic-racial discrimination from educators may question their educators' authority and classroom rules, and educators who perpetuate discrimination may engage in other practices that disadvantage the entire classroom. Thus, we posed three research questions: Did classmates' ethnic-racial discrimination from teachers predict adolescents' classroom adjustment outcomes (e.g., class grades, test scores, and engagement), did classroom climate mediate the link between classmates' ethnic-racial discrimination and adolescents' classroom adjustment outcomes, and did the results differ between early versus middle adolescents? To answer these research questions, the present study leveraged longitudinal data among 1,539 adolescents (Mage = 13.81, SDage = 1.49; 60% Black, 30% White, 9% other, 1% Asian; 49% female, 51% male) nested in 104 math classrooms, as math is a subject domain with pervasive ethnic-racial stereotypes about students' abilities and opportunities to succeed in class. Results illustrated that direct and vicarious ethnic-racial discrimination from math educators in the fall semester predicted worse math course grades, state-administered standardized test scores, and classroom engagement across the fall and spring semesters. Math classroom climate perceptions mediated the longitudinal relations between ethnic-racial discrimination and their math adjustment outcomes, and the role of ethnic-racial discrimination varied across different developmental stages of adolescence. Implications for the measurement of ethnic-racial discrimination in the classroom context and the social contagion linked to ethnic-racial disadvantage are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
民族-种族歧视是一种有害的经历,会影响受歧视青少年的健康成长,但歧视对未受歧视青少年群体的溢出效应却不太清楚。青少年如果亲眼目睹自己的同学遭受教育者的民族-种族歧视,可能会质疑教育者的权威和课堂规则,而长期实施歧视的教育者可能会采取其他不利于整个课堂的做法。因此,我们提出了三个研究问题:同学受到教师的种族-民族歧视是否会预测青少年的课堂适应结果(如班级成绩、考试成绩和参与度),课堂氛围是否会调解同学的种族-民族歧视与青少年课堂适应结果之间的联系,以及早期青少年与中期青少年的结果是否有所不同?为了回答这些研究问题,本研究利用了嵌套在 104 个数学课堂中的 1,539 名青少年的纵向数据(年龄平均值 = 13.81,年龄最小值 = 1.49;60% 为黑人,30% 为白人,9% 为其他族裔,1% 为亚裔;49% 为女性,51% 为男性),因为数学是一个对学生能力和课堂成功机会普遍存在种族成见的学科领域。研究结果表明,秋季学期数学教育者直接或间接的种族-种族歧视会导致秋季和春季学期的数学课程成绩、州立标准化考试成绩和课堂参与度下降。数学课堂氛围感知介导了民族-种族歧视与数学适应结果之间的纵向关系,民族-种族歧视的作用在青少年的不同发展阶段各不相同。本文讨论了在课堂环境中测量民族-种族歧视以及与民族-种族劣势相关的社会传染的意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"When ethnic-racial discrimination from math teachers spills over and predicts the math adjustment of nondiscriminated adolescents: The mediating role of math classroom climate perceptions.","authors":"Juan Del Toro,Kamilah Legette,N Keita Christophe,Michelle Pasco,Dana Miller-Cotto,Ming-Te Wang","doi":"10.1037/dev0001833","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001833","url":null,"abstract":"Ethnic-racial discrimination is a pernicious experience that affects discriminated adolescents' healthy human development, but the spillover consequences of discrimination on the nondiscriminated adolescent population are less clear. Adolescents who vicariously witness their classmates experience ethnic-racial discrimination from educators may question their educators' authority and classroom rules, and educators who perpetuate discrimination may engage in other practices that disadvantage the entire classroom. Thus, we posed three research questions: Did classmates' ethnic-racial discrimination from teachers predict adolescents' classroom adjustment outcomes (e.g., class grades, test scores, and engagement), did classroom climate mediate the link between classmates' ethnic-racial discrimination and adolescents' classroom adjustment outcomes, and did the results differ between early versus middle adolescents? To answer these research questions, the present study leveraged longitudinal data among 1,539 adolescents (Mage = 13.81, SDage = 1.49; 60% Black, 30% White, 9% other, 1% Asian; 49% female, 51% male) nested in 104 math classrooms, as math is a subject domain with pervasive ethnic-racial stereotypes about students' abilities and opportunities to succeed in class. Results illustrated that direct and vicarious ethnic-racial discrimination from math educators in the fall semester predicted worse math course grades, state-administered standardized test scores, and classroom engagement across the fall and spring semesters. Math classroom climate perceptions mediated the longitudinal relations between ethnic-racial discrimination and their math adjustment outcomes, and the role of ethnic-racial discrimination varied across different developmental stages of adolescence. Implications for the measurement of ethnic-racial discrimination in the classroom context and the social contagion linked to ethnic-racial disadvantage are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":"263 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142211902","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Martín Antúnez, Marco McSweeney, Selin Zeytinoglu, Enda Tan, Charles H Zeanah, Charles A Nelson, Nathan A Fox
Aperiodic activity is a background arrhythmic component of electroencephalogram (EEG) that is present in the power spectrum and characterized by an aperiodic offset and an aperiodic exponent. These components have been proposed as a marker of brain maturation, reflecting alterations in excitatory-inhibitory (E:I) balance and exhibiting developmental changes over time. Currently, there is limited understanding regarding how aperiodic activity changes over the course of an individual's life, particularly from early childhood to adolescence, a period when the brain undergoes significant structural and functional transformation. More importantly, considering that brain development is affected by early experience, there is no evidence on how early adversity might affect these parameters. Here, we examined the developmental trajectories of aperiodic activity from EEG data collected in the Bucharest Early Intervention Project, from early childhood to mid adolescence (from 42 months of age up to 16 years old). We examined the effects of a history of early adversity and the impact of early intervention on background aperiodic EEG activity. Surprisingly, we found little influence of a history of adversity or early intervention on these characteristics of the signal. Rather, we found nonlinear age-related trajectories in both aperiodic offset and aperiodic exponent and sex differences in the trajectory for aperiodic offset (but not exponent). These findings provide information on the maturational patterns and trajectories of brain development from early childhood to mid adolescence and how background aperiodic activity describes one aspect of EEG development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"Exploring background aperiodic electroencephalography (EEG) activity in the Bucharest Early Intervention Project.","authors":"Martín Antúnez, Marco McSweeney, Selin Zeytinoglu, Enda Tan, Charles H Zeanah, Charles A Nelson, Nathan A Fox","doi":"10.1037/dev0001804","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001804","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Aperiodic activity is a background arrhythmic component of electroencephalogram (EEG) that is present in the power spectrum and characterized by an aperiodic offset and an aperiodic exponent. These components have been proposed as a marker of brain maturation, reflecting alterations in excitatory-inhibitory (E:I) balance and exhibiting developmental changes over time. Currently, there is limited understanding regarding how aperiodic activity changes over the course of an individual's life, particularly from early childhood to adolescence, a period when the brain undergoes significant structural and functional transformation. More importantly, considering that brain development is affected by early experience, there is no evidence on how early adversity might affect these parameters. Here, we examined the developmental trajectories of aperiodic activity from EEG data collected in the Bucharest Early Intervention Project, from early childhood to mid adolescence (from 42 months of age up to 16 years old). We examined the effects of a history of early adversity and the impact of early intervention on background aperiodic EEG activity. Surprisingly, we found little influence of a history of adversity or early intervention on these characteristics of the signal. Rather, we found nonlinear age-related trajectories in both aperiodic offset and aperiodic exponent and sex differences in the trajectory for aperiodic offset (but not exponent). These findings provide information on the maturational patterns and trajectories of brain development from early childhood to mid adolescence and how background aperiodic activity describes one aspect of EEG development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141377","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Mathilde H Prenevost, Ida B R Nilsen, Evalill Bølstad, Francisco Pons, Paul L Harris, Rolf Reber
An insight is a moment of sudden understanding followed by characteristic feelings of suddenness, positive affect, certainty, and ease, commonly known as an aha experience. Despite evidence from studies with adults that aha experiences benefit learning, little systematic research on children's aha experiences exists. The present study asks how children understand and experience insight. We presented a community sample of 160 children (age: 4-8 years, 47% girls, 51% boys, 2% nonbinary) with an illustrated clues task inspired by the Remote Associate Test, a task commonly used to study insight in adults. In this task, children saw three clues and were asked to find a solution word that was associated with the three clues. Self-reported and observed aha experiences were recorded, along with children's solution accuracy and confidence. Children also answered a set of questions to assess their understanding of aha experiences. We found that although the number of aha experiences remained stable across age, there was a clear developmental increase in the understanding of aha experiences. Children's ability to recognize their own aha experiences as well as their general understanding of the aha concept increased with age. This suggests a lag between the occurrence of children's aha experiences and their understanding of such experiences; children first have aha experiences and later develop an understanding of those experiences. Aha experiences were associated with higher accuracy, but not with higher confidence ratings. Observed aha experiences, but not self-reported aha experiences, predicted increased motivation. Our findings are in line with the literature on metacognitive development and the distinction between the experience and the understanding of emotion. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"Young children's understanding and experience of insight.","authors":"Mathilde H Prenevost, Ida B R Nilsen, Evalill Bølstad, Francisco Pons, Paul L Harris, Rolf Reber","doi":"10.1037/dev0001807","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001807","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An insight is a moment of sudden understanding followed by characteristic feelings of suddenness, positive affect, certainty, and ease, commonly known as an aha experience. Despite evidence from studies with adults that aha experiences benefit learning, little systematic research on children's aha experiences exists. The present study asks how children understand and experience insight. We presented a community sample of 160 children (age: 4-8 years, 47% girls, 51% boys, 2% nonbinary) with an illustrated clues task inspired by the Remote Associate Test, a task commonly used to study insight in adults. In this task, children saw three clues and were asked to find a solution word that was associated with the three clues. Self-reported and observed aha experiences were recorded, along with children's solution accuracy and confidence. Children also answered a set of questions to assess their understanding of aha experiences. We found that although the number of aha experiences remained stable across age, there was a clear developmental increase in the understanding of aha experiences. Children's ability to recognize their own aha experiences as well as their general understanding of the aha concept increased with age. This suggests a lag between the occurrence of children's aha experiences and their understanding of such experiences; children first have aha experiences and later develop an understanding of those experiences. Aha experiences were associated with higher accuracy, but not with higher confidence ratings. Observed aha experiences, but not self-reported aha experiences, predicted increased motivation. Our findings are in line with the literature on metacognitive development and the distinction between the experience and the understanding of emotion. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141388","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Patrizia M Maier, Iryna Schommartz, Deetje Iggena, Carsten Finke, Christoph J Ploner, Yee Lee Shing
Successful navigation to spatial locations relies on lasting memories from previous experiences. Spatial navigation undergoes profound maturational changes during childhood. It is unclear how well children can consolidate navigation-based spatial memories and if age-related variations in navigation during training predict spatial memory. The present study examined the immediate and long-delay (after a 2-week period) consolidation of navigation-based spatial memories in 6- to 8-year-old children (n = 33, 18 female/15 male, Mage = 7.61, SDage = 0.71), 9- to 11-year-old children (n = 32, 13 female/19 male, Mage = 9.90, SDage = 0.59), and 20- to 30-year-old adults (n = 31, 15 female/16 male, Mage = 23.71, SDage = 2.87). Our results showed that, with age, participants navigated more efficiently during training and formed better immediate spatial memories. Long-delay spatial memory retention after 2 weeks was comparable between children and adults, indicating robust consolidation even in children. Interestingly, while children successfully distinguished between perceptually detailed landmarks after 2 weeks, their abstract knowledge of spatial boundaries and cognitive map of landmark relations was poor. Developmental trajectories were similar for egocentric and allocentric spatial memory. Age-related variations in initial navigation were predictive of spatial memory, that is, children with a more mature initial navigation were more likely to find and remember spatial locations immediately and after a 2-week delay. Taken together, our results show an overall robust spatial memory consolidation in mid and late childhood that can be predicted by initial navigation behavior, coupled with nuanced age differences in the recall of spatial boundaries and cognitive maps. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"Development of spatial memory consolidation: A comparison between children and adults.","authors":"Patrizia M Maier, Iryna Schommartz, Deetje Iggena, Carsten Finke, Christoph J Ploner, Yee Lee Shing","doi":"10.1037/dev0001799","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001799","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Successful navigation to spatial locations relies on lasting memories from previous experiences. Spatial navigation undergoes profound maturational changes during childhood. It is unclear how well children can consolidate navigation-based spatial memories and if age-related variations in navigation during training predict spatial memory. The present study examined the immediate and long-delay (after a 2-week period) consolidation of navigation-based spatial memories in 6- to 8-year-old children (n = 33, 18 female/15 male, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 7.61, <i>SD</i><sub>age</sub> = 0.71), 9- to 11-year-old children (<i>n</i> = 32, 13 female/19 male, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 9.90, <i>SD</i><sub>age</sub> = 0.59), and 20- to 30-year-old adults (<i>n</i> = 31, 15 female/16 male, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 23.71, <i>SD</i><sub>age</sub> = 2.87). Our results showed that, with age, participants navigated more efficiently during training and formed better immediate spatial memories. Long-delay spatial memory retention after 2 weeks was comparable between children and adults, indicating robust consolidation even in children. Interestingly, while children successfully distinguished between perceptually detailed landmarks after 2 weeks, their abstract knowledge of spatial boundaries and cognitive map of landmark relations was poor. Developmental trajectories were similar for egocentric and allocentric spatial memory. Age-related variations in initial navigation were predictive of spatial memory, that is, children with a more mature initial navigation were more likely to find and remember spatial locations immediately and after a 2-week delay. Taken together, our results show an overall robust spatial memory consolidation in mid and late childhood that can be predicted by initial navigation behavior, coupled with nuanced age differences in the recall of spatial boundaries and cognitive maps. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141376","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}