首页 > 最新文献

Developmental Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Understanding the developmental transition between parallel and social play during preschool: A multiplex social network analysis. 了解学龄前平行游戏与社交游戏之间的发展过渡:多重社会网络分析。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001837
Jennifer Watling Neal, Zachary P Neal, Brian Brutzman, C Emily Durbin

Preschoolers are expected to transition from parallel play, where they engage in similar activities next to peers, to social play, where they engage in direct interactions with peers. We use longitudinal, multiplex social network analysis to examine the transition between observed parallel and social play over a school year in a 3-year-old classroom (N = 25, 45% girls, 48% White) and a 4-year-old classroom (N = 28, 42.86% girls, 60.71% White). In both classrooms, the existence of a parallel play relationship between two children predicted the formation of a social play relationship between the same two children over time but not vice versa. Findings provide support for a unidirectional, sequential transition from parallel to social play with the same peers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

学龄前儿童会从平行游戏过渡到社交游戏,在平行游戏中,他们会在同伴旁边进行类似的活动,在社交游戏中,他们会与同伴进行直接互动。我们采用纵向、多重社会网络分析方法,研究了一个 3 岁班级(人数=25,45% 为女孩,48% 为白人)和一个 4 岁班级(人数=28,42.86% 为女孩,60.71% 为白人)一学年中观察到的平行游戏和社交游戏之间的过渡情况。在这两个班级中,如果两个孩子之间存在平行游戏关系,则随着时间的推移,这两个孩子之间会形成社交游戏关系,反之亦然。研究结果支持从平行游戏到与同龄人社交游戏的单向、有序过渡。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Understanding the developmental transition between parallel and social play during preschool: A multiplex social network analysis.","authors":"Jennifer Watling Neal, Zachary P Neal, Brian Brutzman, C Emily Durbin","doi":"10.1037/dev0001837","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001837","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Preschoolers are expected to transition from parallel play, where they engage in similar activities next to peers, to social play, where they engage in direct interactions with peers. We use longitudinal, multiplex social network analysis to examine the transition between observed parallel and social play over a school year in a 3-year-old classroom (<i>N</i> = 25, 45% girls, 48% White) and a 4-year-old classroom (<i>N</i> = 28, 42.86% girls, 60.71% White). In both classrooms, the existence of a parallel play relationship between two children predicted the formation of a social play relationship between the same two children over time but not vice versa. Findings provide support for a unidirectional, sequential transition from parallel to social play with the same peers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298907","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The dynamic and relational nature of parent-child conflict from childhood into emerging adulthood. 从童年到成年,亲子冲突的动态性和关系性。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001847
S Alexandra Burt, Sarah Carroll, Elizabeth A Shewark, Kelly L Klump, Jenae M Neiderhiser, Luke W Hyde

Although the parent-child relationship is widely regarded as a foundational context for youth development, the developmental origins of this relationship remain unknown. The present study addressed these gaps, leveraging longitudinal and genetically informed methods to illuminate the developmental origins of mother-child conflict as it unfolds from middle childhood into emerging adulthood. Participants consisted of 2,060 twins in 1,030 twin families (51% male, 49% female; 82% White, 10% Black, 1% Asian, 1% Indigenous, 6% multiracial) from the Michigan State University Twin Registry. Families were assessed up to five times. We fitted a series of latent growth curve models (univariate and parallel process) to data from mothers and children, after which we estimated genetic and environmental sources of variance within and covariance among the intercepts and slopes. Parallel process analyses indicated that maternal reports of conflict at baseline shaped their own and their children's perceptions of change in conflict over time but that children's reports of conflict at baseline predicted only their own rate of change in conflict. Subsequent biometric analyses indicated substantial environmental contributions to the intercepts in childhood, as well as prominent environmental origins to the overlap between maternal and child intercepts. By contrast, we observed robust genetically influenced child effects on maternal rate of change and on the association between the maternal and child slopes. Such findings collectively illuminate the dynamic and relational nature of mother-child conflict from childhood into emerging adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管亲子关系被广泛认为是青少年发展的基础,但这种关系的发展起源仍不为人知。本研究针对这些空白,采用纵向和遗传学方法,揭示了母子冲突从童年中期发展到成年期的发展起源。参与者包括密歇根州立大学双胞胎登记处的 1,030 个双胞胎家庭中的 2,060 对双胞胎(51% 为男性,49% 为女性;82% 为白人,10% 为黑人,1% 为亚裔,1% 为土著,6% 为多种族)。这些家庭最多接受了五次评估。我们对母亲和孩子的数据拟合了一系列潜在的生长曲线模型(单变量和平行过程),然后估算了截距和斜率内部的遗传和环境方差来源以及它们之间的协方差。平行过程分析表明,母亲在基线时对冲突的报告影响了她们自己和子女对冲突随时间变化的看法,但子女在基线时对冲突的报告只预测了他们自己的冲突变化率。随后的生物计量分析表明,环境对童年时期的截距有很大影响,母婴截距的重叠也主要源于环境。相比之下,我们观察到受基因影响的儿童对母亲的变化率以及母亲和儿童斜率之间的关联产生了强有力的影响。这些发现共同揭示了从童年到成年期母子冲突的动态和关系性质。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The dynamic and relational nature of parent-child conflict from childhood into emerging adulthood.","authors":"S Alexandra Burt, Sarah Carroll, Elizabeth A Shewark, Kelly L Klump, Jenae M Neiderhiser, Luke W Hyde","doi":"10.1037/dev0001847","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001847","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although the parent-child relationship is widely regarded as a foundational context for youth development, the developmental origins of this relationship remain unknown. The present study addressed these gaps, leveraging longitudinal and genetically informed methods to illuminate the developmental origins of mother-child conflict as it unfolds from middle childhood into emerging adulthood. Participants consisted of 2,060 twins in 1,030 twin families (51% male, 49% female; 82% White, 10% Black, 1% Asian, 1% Indigenous, 6% multiracial) from the Michigan State University Twin Registry. Families were assessed up to five times. We fitted a series of latent growth curve models (univariate and parallel process) to data from mothers and children, after which we estimated genetic and environmental sources of variance within and covariance among the intercepts and slopes. Parallel process analyses indicated that maternal reports of conflict at baseline shaped their own and their children's perceptions of change in conflict over time but that children's reports of conflict at baseline predicted only their own rate of change in conflict. Subsequent biometric analyses indicated substantial environmental contributions to the intercepts in childhood, as well as prominent environmental origins to the overlap between maternal and child intercepts. By contrast, we observed robust genetically influenced child effects on maternal rate of change and on the association between the maternal and child slopes. Such findings collectively illuminate the dynamic and relational nature of mother-child conflict from childhood into emerging adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Not all punishment is equal: The effect of punishment severity on children's social evaluations. 并非所有惩罚都是平等的:惩罚严重程度对儿童社会评价的影响。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001845
Young-Eun Lee, Larisa Heiphetz Solomon

Punishment is a key mechanism to regulate selfish behaviors and maintain cooperation in a society. However, children often show mixed evaluations about third-party punishment. The current work asked how punishment severity might shape children's social judgments. In two studies, 5- to 10-year-old children heard about a punisher who took different numbers of items from a transgressor and evaluated the punisher's behavior and moral character. In Study 1 (n = 68), when the transgression was relatively mild (i.e., unfair sharing), children across ages evaluated taking no items from the unfair sharer ("no punishment") most positively, while evaluating taking three items ("harshest punishment") most negatively. In Study 2 (n = 68), when the transgression was more serious (i.e., stealing), younger children evaluated taking two items ("equality-establishing punishment") more positively than older children, while evaluating taking none most negatively. However, children became more likely to evaluate equality-establishing punishment negatively with age. Overall, the current results show that punishment severity is a key factor underlying children's third-party punishment judgments. The current research extends work on moral development by showing how children conceptualize the severity of punishment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

惩罚是调节自私行为和维持社会合作的重要机制。然而,儿童对第三方惩罚的评价往往喜忧参半。目前的研究探讨了惩罚的严厉程度会如何影响儿童的社会判断。在两项研究中,5 到 10 岁的儿童听说惩罚者从违法者身上拿走了不同数量的物品,并对惩罚者的行为和道德品质进行了评价。在研究 1(n = 68)中,当违规行为相对较轻(即不公平分享)时,各年龄段儿童对不公平分享者不拿走任何物品("无惩罚")的评价最为积极,而对拿走三件物品("最严厉的惩罚")的评价最为消极。在研究 2(n = 68)中,当越轨行为更为严重(即偷窃)时,年龄较小的儿童比年龄较大的儿童对拿走两件物品("建立平等的惩罚")的评价更为积极,而对不拿走任何物品的评价则最为消极。然而,随着年龄的增长,儿童更有可能对 "建立平等惩罚 "作出负面评价。总之,目前的研究结果表明,惩罚的严重程度是儿童对第三方惩罚作出判断的一个关键因素。当前的研究通过展示儿童如何看待惩罚的严重程度,扩展了道德发展方面的研究。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Not all punishment is equal: The effect of punishment severity on children's social evaluations.","authors":"Young-Eun Lee, Larisa Heiphetz Solomon","doi":"10.1037/dev0001845","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001845","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Punishment is a key mechanism to regulate selfish behaviors and maintain cooperation in a society. However, children often show mixed evaluations about third-party punishment. The current work asked how punishment severity might shape children's social judgments. In two studies, 5- to 10-year-old children heard about a punisher who took different numbers of items from a transgressor and evaluated the punisher's behavior and moral character. In Study 1 (<i>n</i> = 68), when the transgression was relatively mild (i.e., unfair sharing), children across ages evaluated taking no items from the unfair sharer (\"no punishment\") most positively, while evaluating taking three items (\"harshest punishment\") most negatively. In Study 2 (<i>n</i> = 68), when the transgression was more serious (i.e., stealing), younger children evaluated taking two items (\"equality-establishing punishment\") more positively than older children, while evaluating taking none most negatively. However, children became more likely to evaluate equality-establishing punishment negatively with age. Overall, the current results show that punishment severity is a key factor underlying children's third-party punishment judgments. The current research extends work on moral development by showing how children conceptualize the severity of punishment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298903","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pubertal timing in adolescence and adulthood: Relations among contemporaneous and retrospective measures. 青春期和成年期的青春期时间:同期测量与回顾测量之间的关系。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-09 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001784
Natasha Chaku,Sheri A Berenbaum,Yiming Qian,Robin P Corley,Sally J Wadsworth,Chandra A Reynolds,Adriene M Beltz
Pubertal development has short- and long-term effects on psychological adjustment. Many studies of long-term effects rely on retrospective measurement of pubertal timing, but such measures often reflect different aspects of puberty than those assessed in adolescence, raising questions about the utility and interpretation of retrospective reports. The present study leveraged longitudinal data collected in adolescence and established adulthood to determine: (1) the correspondence between pubertal timing indexed from logistic growth curves of self-reported physical development assessed contemporaneously across adolescence and pubertal timing indexed relative to peers assessed retrospectively in adulthood; (2) the associations between the two pubertal timing measures and psychological adjustment; and (3) potential recall biases. Participants were 748 individuals (50.1% female; 91.6% White) from two longitudinal studies who reported on their pubertal development annually from Grades 3-9, psychological adjustment (age at sexual initiation, substance use, depression) in late adolescence, and retrospective pubertal timing in established adulthood (Mage = 32.76; SD = 4.43). Results indicate moderate-to-high convergence between retrospective and contemporaneous indices. Most participants, especially women, had the same pubertal timing classification (i.e., early, on time, or late), but early-maturing adolescents often recalled on-time development as adults. Retrospective and contemporaneous indices were associated with psychological adjustment in similar ways, with some attenuation in the retrospective measure, especially for men. There was little evidence of recall bias due to age at retrospective assessment or time since puberty. Findings generally support the use of retrospective pubertal timing measures, with the recognition that some relations with adjustment may be weakened. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
青春期发育对心理适应有短期和长期的影响。许多关于长期影响的研究都依赖于对青春期时间的回顾性测量,但这些测量通常反映了青春期的不同方面,而不是青春期的评估,这就对回顾性报告的效用和解释提出了质疑。本研究利用在青春期和成年后收集的纵向数据来确定:(1)根据青春期同时评估的自我报告身体发育的逻辑生长曲线所得出的青春期时间指数与成年后回顾性评估的相对于同龄人的青春期时间指数之间的对应关系;(2)两种青春期时间测量与心理适应之间的关联;以及(3)潜在的回忆偏差。参与者为来自两项纵向研究的 748 人(50.1% 为女性;91.6% 为白人),他们报告了 3-9 年级每年的青春期发育情况、青春期后期的心理适应情况(开始性行为的年龄、药物使用、抑郁)以及成年后的青春期时间回顾(Mage = 32.76;SD = 4.43)。结果表明,回顾性指数和同期指数之间存在中度到高度的趋同性。大多数参与者,尤其是女性,都有相同的青春期时间分类(即早熟、准时或晚熟),但早熟的青少年往往回忆起成年时的准时发育。回顾性指数和同期指数与心理适应的相关性相似,但回顾性指数有所减弱,尤其是男性。几乎没有证据表明回顾性评估时的年龄或青春期后的时间会导致回忆偏差。研究结果总体上支持使用青春期时间回顾测量法,但也认识到与适应的某些关系可能会减弱。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Pubertal timing in adolescence and adulthood: Relations among contemporaneous and retrospective measures.","authors":"Natasha Chaku,Sheri A Berenbaum,Yiming Qian,Robin P Corley,Sally J Wadsworth,Chandra A Reynolds,Adriene M Beltz","doi":"10.1037/dev0001784","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001784","url":null,"abstract":"Pubertal development has short- and long-term effects on psychological adjustment. Many studies of long-term effects rely on retrospective measurement of pubertal timing, but such measures often reflect different aspects of puberty than those assessed in adolescence, raising questions about the utility and interpretation of retrospective reports. The present study leveraged longitudinal data collected in adolescence and established adulthood to determine: (1) the correspondence between pubertal timing indexed from logistic growth curves of self-reported physical development assessed contemporaneously across adolescence and pubertal timing indexed relative to peers assessed retrospectively in adulthood; (2) the associations between the two pubertal timing measures and psychological adjustment; and (3) potential recall biases. Participants were 748 individuals (50.1% female; 91.6% White) from two longitudinal studies who reported on their pubertal development annually from Grades 3-9, psychological adjustment (age at sexual initiation, substance use, depression) in late adolescence, and retrospective pubertal timing in established adulthood (Mage = 32.76; SD = 4.43). Results indicate moderate-to-high convergence between retrospective and contemporaneous indices. Most participants, especially women, had the same pubertal timing classification (i.e., early, on time, or late), but early-maturing adolescents often recalled on-time development as adults. Retrospective and contemporaneous indices were associated with psychological adjustment in similar ways, with some attenuation in the retrospective measure, especially for men. There was little evidence of recall bias due to age at retrospective assessment or time since puberty. Findings generally support the use of retrospective pubertal timing measures, with the recognition that some relations with adjustment may be weakened. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":"19 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142211900","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Math motivational belief development during high school by race/ethnicity: Teachers and parents as predictors of changes. 按种族/族裔划分的高中数学动机信念发展情况:教师和家长是变化的预测因素。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-09 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001823
Glona Lee-Poon,Jacquelynne S Eccles,Sandra D Simpkins
The changes in adolescents' math motivational beliefs (i.e., expectancies for success, interest, and utility value) across Grades 9-11 and the associations between these changes and adolescents' experiences with socializers (i.e., perceived teacher unfairness and parent-adolescent discussions) were examined within each of the four largest racial/ethnic groups in the United States using the High School Longitudinal Study, a nationally representative data set (n = 19,010; 50% female; 9% Asian; 11% Black; 18% Latine; 62% White; Mage = 14.53 in Grade 9). Cross-tabulation analyses suggested that similar developmental trends emerged within each racial/ethnic group (which were tested separately). Many adolescents maintained their high or low expectancies, interest, and utility values across Grades 9-11. Some patterns varied by belief; for example, several adolescents switched from high to low interest by Grade 11, whereas several adolescents switched from low to high utility value. Parent-adolescent discussions predicted positive changes among Asian and Latine adolescents, whereas perceived teacher unfairness predicted negative changes among Black adolescents. The findings from the present study highlight the diverse developmental trends in adolescents' motivational beliefs and the potential role of socializers as sources of strength or challenge in their motivational belief development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
我们利用具有全国代表性的数据集《高中纵向研究》(High School Longitudinal Study)(n = 19010;50% 女性;9% 亚洲人;11% 黑人;18% 拉丁人;62% 白人;9 年级学生平均年龄 = 14.53),对美国四大种族/民族群体中的每一个群体进行了研究。交叉分析表明,每个种族/族裔群体(分别进行测试)都出现了类似的发展趋向。许多青少年在九年级至十一年级期间保持着或高或低的期望值、兴趣值和效用值。一些模式因信仰而异;例如,一些青少年在十一年级时从高兴趣转为低兴趣,而一些青少年则从低效用值转为高效用值。家长与青少年的讨论预示着亚裔和拉丁裔青少年的积极变化,而认为教师不公平则预示着黑人青少年的消极变化。本研究的结果凸显了青少年动机信念的不同发展趋向,以及社会化者在青少年动机信念发展过程中作为力量来源或挑战来源的潜在作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Math motivational belief development during high school by race/ethnicity: Teachers and parents as predictors of changes.","authors":"Glona Lee-Poon,Jacquelynne S Eccles,Sandra D Simpkins","doi":"10.1037/dev0001823","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001823","url":null,"abstract":"The changes in adolescents' math motivational beliefs (i.e., expectancies for success, interest, and utility value) across Grades 9-11 and the associations between these changes and adolescents' experiences with socializers (i.e., perceived teacher unfairness and parent-adolescent discussions) were examined within each of the four largest racial/ethnic groups in the United States using the High School Longitudinal Study, a nationally representative data set (n = 19,010; 50% female; 9% Asian; 11% Black; 18% Latine; 62% White; Mage = 14.53 in Grade 9). Cross-tabulation analyses suggested that similar developmental trends emerged within each racial/ethnic group (which were tested separately). Many adolescents maintained their high or low expectancies, interest, and utility values across Grades 9-11. Some patterns varied by belief; for example, several adolescents switched from high to low interest by Grade 11, whereas several adolescents switched from low to high utility value. Parent-adolescent discussions predicted positive changes among Asian and Latine adolescents, whereas perceived teacher unfairness predicted negative changes among Black adolescents. The findings from the present study highlight the diverse developmental trends in adolescents' motivational beliefs and the potential role of socializers as sources of strength or challenge in their motivational belief development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":"20 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142211906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Narrating lives across 16 years: Developmental trajectories of coherence and relations to well-being in a lifespan sample. 叙述跨越 16 年的生活:在一个生命周期样本中,连贯性的发展轨迹及其与幸福感的关系。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-09 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001775
Isabel Peters,Florian Schmiedek,Tilmann Habermas
The ability to narrate a life coherently develops first in adolescence, but little is known about its course across adulthood and into old age. Also, the potential association of life narrative coherence with well-being has never been studied. Our aim was to investigate lifespan development of coherence and associations with well-being using data from the longitudinal MainLife study. A total of 172 urban Germans (8-80 years; 87 females) narrated their lives up to six times across 16 years (N = 803 brief entire life narratives). Most were highly educated, and the proportion of a migration background was typical for the local population. Life narratives were rated for three types of global coherence (temporal, causal-motivational, and thematic) and coded for two types of autobiographical arguments (stability maintaining and change engendering). Multilevel models were used to investigate their lifespan development and relations to well-being. While most life narrative measures increased up to emerging adulthood and then remained stable, thematic coherence continued to increase into middle adulthood. Only change-engendering autobiographical arguments slowly decreased from midlife onward. Unexpectedly, neither autobiographical arguments nor global coherence correlated significantly with well-being. Data exploration suggested an association between thematic coherence and self-continuity. We conclude that life narrative coherence may only be related to well-being if samples include cases with more extreme noncoherence (e,g., clinical disorders). Our findings add to understanding the development of the life story across the lifespan, especially in older age, and suggest studying relations of coherence, self-continuity, and well-being specifically in life crises and in clinical samples. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
连贯叙述人生的能力首先是在青少年时期形成的,但人们对这种能力在成年期和老年期的发展过程知之甚少。此外,生活叙事连贯性与幸福感之间的潜在联系也从未被研究过。我们的目的是利用 "主要生活 "纵向研究的数据,调查生活连贯性的寿命发展及其与幸福感的关系。共有 172 名城市德国人(8-80 岁;87 名女性)在 16 年中对自己的生活进行了多达六次的叙述(N = 803 个简短的完整生活叙述)。大多数人受过高等教育,移民背景的比例在当地人口中很典型。我们对人生叙事的三种总体连贯性(时间连贯性、因果-动机连贯性和主题连贯性)进行了评定,并对两种自传论点(维持稳定和促成变化)进行了编码。我们使用多层次模型来研究它们的生命周期发展以及与幸福感的关系。虽然大多数生活叙事测量指标在成年期之前都在增加,然后保持稳定,但主题一致性在成年中期继续增加。只有引发变化的自传论点从中年开始慢慢减少。出乎意料的是,自传性论点和整体一致性都与幸福感没有明显的相关性。数据探索表明,主题一致性与自我连续性之间存在关联。我们的结论是,只有当样本中包括更极端的不连贯性(如临床失调)时,生活叙事的连贯性才可能与幸福感相关。我们的研究结果有助于理解生命故事在整个生命周期中的发展,尤其是在老年阶段,并建议对生命危机和临床样本中的连贯性、自我连续性和幸福感之间的关系进行专门研究。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Narrating lives across 16 years: Developmental trajectories of coherence and relations to well-being in a lifespan sample.","authors":"Isabel Peters,Florian Schmiedek,Tilmann Habermas","doi":"10.1037/dev0001775","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001775","url":null,"abstract":"The ability to narrate a life coherently develops first in adolescence, but little is known about its course across adulthood and into old age. Also, the potential association of life narrative coherence with well-being has never been studied. Our aim was to investigate lifespan development of coherence and associations with well-being using data from the longitudinal MainLife study. A total of 172 urban Germans (8-80 years; 87 females) narrated their lives up to six times across 16 years (N = 803 brief entire life narratives). Most were highly educated, and the proportion of a migration background was typical for the local population. Life narratives were rated for three types of global coherence (temporal, causal-motivational, and thematic) and coded for two types of autobiographical arguments (stability maintaining and change engendering). Multilevel models were used to investigate their lifespan development and relations to well-being. While most life narrative measures increased up to emerging adulthood and then remained stable, thematic coherence continued to increase into middle adulthood. Only change-engendering autobiographical arguments slowly decreased from midlife onward. Unexpectedly, neither autobiographical arguments nor global coherence correlated significantly with well-being. Data exploration suggested an association between thematic coherence and self-continuity. We conclude that life narrative coherence may only be related to well-being if samples include cases with more extreme noncoherence (e,g., clinical disorders). Our findings add to understanding the development of the life story across the lifespan, especially in older age, and suggest studying relations of coherence, self-continuity, and well-being specifically in life crises and in clinical samples. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":"58 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142211907","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When ethnic-racial discrimination from math teachers spills over and predicts the math adjustment of nondiscriminated adolescents: The mediating role of math classroom climate perceptions. 数学教师的民族-种族歧视何时蔓延并预测未受歧视青少年的数学适应情况?数学课堂氛围感知的中介作用。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-09 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001833
Juan Del Toro,Kamilah Legette,N Keita Christophe,Michelle Pasco,Dana Miller-Cotto,Ming-Te Wang
Ethnic-racial discrimination is a pernicious experience that affects discriminated adolescents' healthy human development, but the spillover consequences of discrimination on the nondiscriminated adolescent population are less clear. Adolescents who vicariously witness their classmates experience ethnic-racial discrimination from educators may question their educators' authority and classroom rules, and educators who perpetuate discrimination may engage in other practices that disadvantage the entire classroom. Thus, we posed three research questions: Did classmates' ethnic-racial discrimination from teachers predict adolescents' classroom adjustment outcomes (e.g., class grades, test scores, and engagement), did classroom climate mediate the link between classmates' ethnic-racial discrimination and adolescents' classroom adjustment outcomes, and did the results differ between early versus middle adolescents? To answer these research questions, the present study leveraged longitudinal data among 1,539 adolescents (Mage = 13.81, SDage = 1.49; 60% Black, 30% White, 9% other, 1% Asian; 49% female, 51% male) nested in 104 math classrooms, as math is a subject domain with pervasive ethnic-racial stereotypes about students' abilities and opportunities to succeed in class. Results illustrated that direct and vicarious ethnic-racial discrimination from math educators in the fall semester predicted worse math course grades, state-administered standardized test scores, and classroom engagement across the fall and spring semesters. Math classroom climate perceptions mediated the longitudinal relations between ethnic-racial discrimination and their math adjustment outcomes, and the role of ethnic-racial discrimination varied across different developmental stages of adolescence. Implications for the measurement of ethnic-racial discrimination in the classroom context and the social contagion linked to ethnic-racial disadvantage are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
民族-种族歧视是一种有害的经历,会影响受歧视青少年的健康成长,但歧视对未受歧视青少年群体的溢出效应却不太清楚。青少年如果亲眼目睹自己的同学遭受教育者的民族-种族歧视,可能会质疑教育者的权威和课堂规则,而长期实施歧视的教育者可能会采取其他不利于整个课堂的做法。因此,我们提出了三个研究问题:同学受到教师的种族-民族歧视是否会预测青少年的课堂适应结果(如班级成绩、考试成绩和参与度),课堂氛围是否会调解同学的种族-民族歧视与青少年课堂适应结果之间的联系,以及早期青少年与中期青少年的结果是否有所不同?为了回答这些研究问题,本研究利用了嵌套在 104 个数学课堂中的 1,539 名青少年的纵向数据(年龄平均值 = 13.81,年龄最小值 = 1.49;60% 为黑人,30% 为白人,9% 为其他族裔,1% 为亚裔;49% 为女性,51% 为男性),因为数学是一个对学生能力和课堂成功机会普遍存在种族成见的学科领域。研究结果表明,秋季学期数学教育者直接或间接的种族-种族歧视会导致秋季和春季学期的数学课程成绩、州立标准化考试成绩和课堂参与度下降。数学课堂氛围感知介导了民族-种族歧视与数学适应结果之间的纵向关系,民族-种族歧视的作用在青少年的不同发展阶段各不相同。本文讨论了在课堂环境中测量民族-种族歧视以及与民族-种族劣势相关的社会传染的意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"When ethnic-racial discrimination from math teachers spills over and predicts the math adjustment of nondiscriminated adolescents: The mediating role of math classroom climate perceptions.","authors":"Juan Del Toro,Kamilah Legette,N Keita Christophe,Michelle Pasco,Dana Miller-Cotto,Ming-Te Wang","doi":"10.1037/dev0001833","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001833","url":null,"abstract":"Ethnic-racial discrimination is a pernicious experience that affects discriminated adolescents' healthy human development, but the spillover consequences of discrimination on the nondiscriminated adolescent population are less clear. Adolescents who vicariously witness their classmates experience ethnic-racial discrimination from educators may question their educators' authority and classroom rules, and educators who perpetuate discrimination may engage in other practices that disadvantage the entire classroom. Thus, we posed three research questions: Did classmates' ethnic-racial discrimination from teachers predict adolescents' classroom adjustment outcomes (e.g., class grades, test scores, and engagement), did classroom climate mediate the link between classmates' ethnic-racial discrimination and adolescents' classroom adjustment outcomes, and did the results differ between early versus middle adolescents? To answer these research questions, the present study leveraged longitudinal data among 1,539 adolescents (Mage = 13.81, SDage = 1.49; 60% Black, 30% White, 9% other, 1% Asian; 49% female, 51% male) nested in 104 math classrooms, as math is a subject domain with pervasive ethnic-racial stereotypes about students' abilities and opportunities to succeed in class. Results illustrated that direct and vicarious ethnic-racial discrimination from math educators in the fall semester predicted worse math course grades, state-administered standardized test scores, and classroom engagement across the fall and spring semesters. Math classroom climate perceptions mediated the longitudinal relations between ethnic-racial discrimination and their math adjustment outcomes, and the role of ethnic-racial discrimination varied across different developmental stages of adolescence. Implications for the measurement of ethnic-racial discrimination in the classroom context and the social contagion linked to ethnic-racial disadvantage are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":"263 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142211902","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring background aperiodic electroencephalography (EEG) activity in the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. 探索布加勒斯特早期干预项目中的背景非周期性脑电图(EEG)活动。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-05 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001804
Martín Antúnez, Marco McSweeney, Selin Zeytinoglu, Enda Tan, Charles H Zeanah, Charles A Nelson, Nathan A Fox

Aperiodic activity is a background arrhythmic component of electroencephalogram (EEG) that is present in the power spectrum and characterized by an aperiodic offset and an aperiodic exponent. These components have been proposed as a marker of brain maturation, reflecting alterations in excitatory-inhibitory (E:I) balance and exhibiting developmental changes over time. Currently, there is limited understanding regarding how aperiodic activity changes over the course of an individual's life, particularly from early childhood to adolescence, a period when the brain undergoes significant structural and functional transformation. More importantly, considering that brain development is affected by early experience, there is no evidence on how early adversity might affect these parameters. Here, we examined the developmental trajectories of aperiodic activity from EEG data collected in the Bucharest Early Intervention Project, from early childhood to mid adolescence (from 42 months of age up to 16 years old). We examined the effects of a history of early adversity and the impact of early intervention on background aperiodic EEG activity. Surprisingly, we found little influence of a history of adversity or early intervention on these characteristics of the signal. Rather, we found nonlinear age-related trajectories in both aperiodic offset and aperiodic exponent and sex differences in the trajectory for aperiodic offset (but not exponent). These findings provide information on the maturational patterns and trajectories of brain development from early childhood to mid adolescence and how background aperiodic activity describes one aspect of EEG development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

非周期性活动是脑电图(EEG)中的一种背景心律失常成分,存在于功率谱中,以非周期性偏移和非周期性指数为特征。这些成分被认为是大脑成熟的标志,反映了兴奋-抑制(E:I)平衡的改变,并随着时间的推移表现出发育变化。目前,人们对非周期性活动在人的一生中,特别是从幼儿期到青春期的变化了解有限,而这一时期正是大脑结构和功能发生重大转变的时期。更重要的是,考虑到大脑发育受早期经历的影响,目前还没有证据表明早期逆境会如何影响这些参数。在此,我们从布加勒斯特早期干预项目收集的脑电图数据中,研究了从幼儿期到青春期中期(从 42 个月大到 16 岁)非周期性活动的发展轨迹。我们研究了早期逆境史的影响以及早期干预对背景非周期性脑电活动的影响。令人惊讶的是,我们发现逆境史或早期干预对这些信号特征的影响很小。相反,我们发现非周期性偏移和非周期性指数都存在与年龄相关的非线性轨迹,而且非周期性偏移(而非指数)的轨迹存在性别差异。这些发现提供了从幼儿期到青春期中期大脑发育成熟模式和轨迹的信息,以及背景非周期性活动如何描述脑电图发育的一个方面。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Exploring background aperiodic electroencephalography (EEG) activity in the Bucharest Early Intervention Project.","authors":"Martín Antúnez, Marco McSweeney, Selin Zeytinoglu, Enda Tan, Charles H Zeanah, Charles A Nelson, Nathan A Fox","doi":"10.1037/dev0001804","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001804","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Aperiodic activity is a background arrhythmic component of electroencephalogram (EEG) that is present in the power spectrum and characterized by an aperiodic offset and an aperiodic exponent. These components have been proposed as a marker of brain maturation, reflecting alterations in excitatory-inhibitory (E:I) balance and exhibiting developmental changes over time. Currently, there is limited understanding regarding how aperiodic activity changes over the course of an individual's life, particularly from early childhood to adolescence, a period when the brain undergoes significant structural and functional transformation. More importantly, considering that brain development is affected by early experience, there is no evidence on how early adversity might affect these parameters. Here, we examined the developmental trajectories of aperiodic activity from EEG data collected in the Bucharest Early Intervention Project, from early childhood to mid adolescence (from 42 months of age up to 16 years old). We examined the effects of a history of early adversity and the impact of early intervention on background aperiodic EEG activity. Surprisingly, we found little influence of a history of adversity or early intervention on these characteristics of the signal. Rather, we found nonlinear age-related trajectories in both aperiodic offset and aperiodic exponent and sex differences in the trajectory for aperiodic offset (but not exponent). These findings provide information on the maturational patterns and trajectories of brain development from early childhood to mid adolescence and how background aperiodic activity describes one aspect of EEG development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141377","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Young children's understanding and experience of insight. 幼儿对洞察力的理解和体验。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-05 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001807
Mathilde H Prenevost, Ida B R Nilsen, Evalill Bølstad, Francisco Pons, Paul L Harris, Rolf Reber

An insight is a moment of sudden understanding followed by characteristic feelings of suddenness, positive affect, certainty, and ease, commonly known as an aha experience. Despite evidence from studies with adults that aha experiences benefit learning, little systematic research on children's aha experiences exists. The present study asks how children understand and experience insight. We presented a community sample of 160 children (age: 4-8 years, 47% girls, 51% boys, 2% nonbinary) with an illustrated clues task inspired by the Remote Associate Test, a task commonly used to study insight in adults. In this task, children saw three clues and were asked to find a solution word that was associated with the three clues. Self-reported and observed aha experiences were recorded, along with children's solution accuracy and confidence. Children also answered a set of questions to assess their understanding of aha experiences. We found that although the number of aha experiences remained stable across age, there was a clear developmental increase in the understanding of aha experiences. Children's ability to recognize their own aha experiences as well as their general understanding of the aha concept increased with age. This suggests a lag between the occurrence of children's aha experiences and their understanding of such experiences; children first have aha experiences and later develop an understanding of those experiences. Aha experiences were associated with higher accuracy, but not with higher confidence ratings. Observed aha experiences, but not self-reported aha experiences, predicted increased motivation. Our findings are in line with the literature on metacognitive development and the distinction between the experience and the understanding of emotion. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

所谓 "洞察力",是指突然明白事理的一瞬间,随之而来的是突如其来的、积极的、确定的和轻松的感觉,通常被称为 "啊哈体验"。尽管对成人的研究表明,"啊哈 "体验有益于学习,但对儿童 "啊哈 "体验的系统研究却很少。本研究询问儿童如何理解和体验洞察力。我们在一个由 160 名儿童(年龄:4-8 岁,47% 为女孩,51% 为男孩,2% 为非二元)组成的社区样本中开展了一项插图线索任务,该任务的灵感来源于 "远程联想测试"(Remote Associate Test),这是一项常用于研究成人洞察力的任务。在这项任务中,孩子们看到三条线索,并被要求找出与这三条线索相关联的解词。在记录儿童自我报告和观察到的 "啊哈 "体验的同时,还记录了他们解决问题的准确性和自信心。孩子们还回答了一组问题,以评估他们对 "啊哈 "体验的理解。我们发现,虽然 "啊哈 "体验的数量在不同年龄段保持稳定,但对 "啊哈 "体验的理解却明显随着年龄的增长而增加。随着年龄的增长,儿童识别自己 "啊哈 "体验的能力以及对 "啊哈 "概念的总体理解能力也在提高。这表明,儿童的 "啊哈 "体验的出现与他们对这种体验的理解之间存在着一定的滞后性;儿童先有 "啊哈 "体验,然后才会对这些体验产生理解。"啊哈 "体验与较高的准确度有关,但与较高的信心评级无关。观察到的 "啊哈 "体验(而非自我报告的 "啊哈 "体验)可预测动机的增强。我们的研究结果与元认知发展方面的文献以及情感体验和理解之间的区别是一致的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Young children's understanding and experience of insight.","authors":"Mathilde H Prenevost, Ida B R Nilsen, Evalill Bølstad, Francisco Pons, Paul L Harris, Rolf Reber","doi":"10.1037/dev0001807","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001807","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An insight is a moment of sudden understanding followed by characteristic feelings of suddenness, positive affect, certainty, and ease, commonly known as an aha experience. Despite evidence from studies with adults that aha experiences benefit learning, little systematic research on children's aha experiences exists. The present study asks how children understand and experience insight. We presented a community sample of 160 children (age: 4-8 years, 47% girls, 51% boys, 2% nonbinary) with an illustrated clues task inspired by the Remote Associate Test, a task commonly used to study insight in adults. In this task, children saw three clues and were asked to find a solution word that was associated with the three clues. Self-reported and observed aha experiences were recorded, along with children's solution accuracy and confidence. Children also answered a set of questions to assess their understanding of aha experiences. We found that although the number of aha experiences remained stable across age, there was a clear developmental increase in the understanding of aha experiences. Children's ability to recognize their own aha experiences as well as their general understanding of the aha concept increased with age. This suggests a lag between the occurrence of children's aha experiences and their understanding of such experiences; children first have aha experiences and later develop an understanding of those experiences. Aha experiences were associated with higher accuracy, but not with higher confidence ratings. Observed aha experiences, but not self-reported aha experiences, predicted increased motivation. Our findings are in line with the literature on metacognitive development and the distinction between the experience and the understanding of emotion. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141388","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Development of spatial memory consolidation: A comparison between children and adults. 空间记忆巩固的发展:儿童与成人的比较
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-05 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001799
Patrizia M Maier, Iryna Schommartz, Deetje Iggena, Carsten Finke, Christoph J Ploner, Yee Lee Shing

Successful navigation to spatial locations relies on lasting memories from previous experiences. Spatial navigation undergoes profound maturational changes during childhood. It is unclear how well children can consolidate navigation-based spatial memories and if age-related variations in navigation during training predict spatial memory. The present study examined the immediate and long-delay (after a 2-week period) consolidation of navigation-based spatial memories in 6- to 8-year-old children (n = 33, 18 female/15 male, Mage = 7.61, SDage = 0.71), 9- to 11-year-old children (n = 32, 13 female/19 male, Mage = 9.90, SDage = 0.59), and 20- to 30-year-old adults (n = 31, 15 female/16 male, Mage = 23.71, SDage = 2.87). Our results showed that, with age, participants navigated more efficiently during training and formed better immediate spatial memories. Long-delay spatial memory retention after 2 weeks was comparable between children and adults, indicating robust consolidation even in children. Interestingly, while children successfully distinguished between perceptually detailed landmarks after 2 weeks, their abstract knowledge of spatial boundaries and cognitive map of landmark relations was poor. Developmental trajectories were similar for egocentric and allocentric spatial memory. Age-related variations in initial navigation were predictive of spatial memory, that is, children with a more mature initial navigation were more likely to find and remember spatial locations immediately and after a 2-week delay. Taken together, our results show an overall robust spatial memory consolidation in mid and late childhood that can be predicted by initial navigation behavior, coupled with nuanced age differences in the recall of spatial boundaries and cognitive maps. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

对空间位置的成功导航依赖于对以往经验的持久记忆。空间导航在儿童时期经历了深刻的成熟变化。目前还不清楚儿童能在多大程度上巩固以导航为基础的空间记忆,也不清楚在训练过程中与年龄相关的导航变化是否能预测空间记忆。本研究考察了 6 至 8 岁儿童(n = 33,18 名女性/15 名男性,Mage = 7.61,SDage = 0.71)、9 至 11 岁儿童(n = 32,13 名女性/19 名男性,Mage = 9.90,SDage = 0.59)和 20 至 30 岁成年人(n = 31,15 名女性/16 名男性,Mage = 23.71,SDage = 2.87)基于导航的空间记忆的即时巩固和长延时(2 周后)巩固情况。我们的结果表明,随着年龄的增长,参与者在训练中的导航效率更高,形成的即时空间记忆也更好。两周后,儿童和成人的长延时空间记忆保持率相当,这表明即使是儿童也能很好地巩固空间记忆。有趣的是,虽然儿童在两周后能成功区分感知上详细的地标,但他们对空间边界的抽象知识和地标关系的认知图谱却很差。以自我为中心的空间记忆和以分配为中心的空间记忆的发展轨迹相似。与年龄相关的初始导航变化对空间记忆有预测作用,即初始导航更成熟的儿童更有可能立即找到并记住空间位置,延迟两周后也是如此。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,儿童中期和晚期的整体空间记忆巩固能力很强,这可以通过初始导航行为以及在回忆空间边界和认知地图方面的细微年龄差异来预测。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Development of spatial memory consolidation: A comparison between children and adults.","authors":"Patrizia M Maier, Iryna Schommartz, Deetje Iggena, Carsten Finke, Christoph J Ploner, Yee Lee Shing","doi":"10.1037/dev0001799","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001799","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Successful navigation to spatial locations relies on lasting memories from previous experiences. Spatial navigation undergoes profound maturational changes during childhood. It is unclear how well children can consolidate navigation-based spatial memories and if age-related variations in navigation during training predict spatial memory. The present study examined the immediate and long-delay (after a 2-week period) consolidation of navigation-based spatial memories in 6- to 8-year-old children (n = 33, 18 female/15 male, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 7.61, <i>SD</i><sub>age</sub> = 0.71), 9- to 11-year-old children (<i>n</i> = 32, 13 female/19 male, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 9.90, <i>SD</i><sub>age</sub> = 0.59), and 20- to 30-year-old adults (<i>n</i> = 31, 15 female/16 male, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 23.71, <i>SD</i><sub>age</sub> = 2.87). Our results showed that, with age, participants navigated more efficiently during training and formed better immediate spatial memories. Long-delay spatial memory retention after 2 weeks was comparable between children and adults, indicating robust consolidation even in children. Interestingly, while children successfully distinguished between perceptually detailed landmarks after 2 weeks, their abstract knowledge of spatial boundaries and cognitive map of landmark relations was poor. Developmental trajectories were similar for egocentric and allocentric spatial memory. Age-related variations in initial navigation were predictive of spatial memory, that is, children with a more mature initial navigation were more likely to find and remember spatial locations immediately and after a 2-week delay. Taken together, our results show an overall robust spatial memory consolidation in mid and late childhood that can be predicted by initial navigation behavior, coupled with nuanced age differences in the recall of spatial boundaries and cognitive maps. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141376","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Developmental Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1