首页 > 最新文献

Developmental Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Trajectories of emotional disclosure with parents during the college transition among Asian first-year students. 亚裔大一学生在大学过渡期间向父母披露情感的轨迹。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-08 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001781
GeckHong Yeo, Eddie M W Tong

The college transition is a time of great emotional lability, and sharing emotional experiences with parents can be beneficial for first-year students. Guided by the social sharing of emotions framework and the cultural theory on self-construal, this study investigated the developmental course of freshmen's emotional disclosure with parents during the first semester and the mediating role of independence-orientation in two Asian contexts-Beijing (China) and Singapore. Using experience sampling method (online diary), 205 Chinese freshmen (Mage = 19.43) and 291 Singapore freshmen (Mage = 19.44) reported on four facets of emotional disclosure with parents (frequency, intimacy, negativity, and positivity) across four timepoints and their independence-interdependence orientation (Time 1). Latent growth curve modeling comparing the two Asian samples revealed that frequency increased for Chinese students and decreased for Singapore students. Intimacy indicated upward trajectories for both Asian samples, whereas negativity and positivity showed downward trajectories but were less pronounced for Chinese students. Mediated latent growth curve modeling revealed that the cultures predicted independence-orientation, which in turn negatively predicted increase in frequency for the Singapore sample and positively predicted decrease for the Chinese sample. Independence-orientation negatively predicted increase in intimacy, positively predicted decline in negativity, and negatively predicted decline in positivity. In sum, we found complex and differentiated trajectories for the four facets of emotional disclosure in two Asian samples and the mediating role of independence-orientation in explaining cultural differences in the trajectories, which have implications for understanding emotional disclosure to parents during the developmental phase of the college transition in Asian contexts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

大学转型期是一个情绪非常不稳定的时期,与父母分享情绪体验对大一学生来说是有益的。本研究以情绪的社会分享框架和自我建构的文化理论为指导,在中国北京和新加坡两个亚洲背景下,调查了大一新生在第一学期与父母分享情绪的发展过程,以及独立取向的中介作用。通过经验取样法(在线日记),205 名中国新生(平均年龄 = 19.43)和 291 名新加坡新生(平均年龄 = 19.44)报告了他们在四个时间点上与父母情感披露的四个方面(频率、亲密程度、消极性和积极性)以及他们的独立-相互依赖取向(时间 1)。比较两个亚洲样本的潜在增长曲线模型显示,中国学生的频率增加,而新加坡学生的频率减少。亲密性在两个亚洲样本中都呈上升趋势,而消极性和积极性则呈下降趋势,但在中国学生中不太明显。中介潜成长曲线模型显示,文化预测了独立取向,而独立取向反过来又消极地预测了新加坡样本中频率的增加,积极地预测了中国样本中频率的减少。独立取向对亲密关系的增加有负面影响,对消极关系的减少有正面影响,对积极关系的减少有负面影响。总之,我们在两个亚洲样本中发现了情感披露四个方面复杂而不同的轨迹,以及独立取向在解释这些轨迹的文化差异中的中介作用,这对理解亚洲背景下大学过渡发展阶段向父母披露情感的情况具有重要意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Trajectories of emotional disclosure with parents during the college transition among Asian first-year students.","authors":"GeckHong Yeo, Eddie M W Tong","doi":"10.1037/dev0001781","DOIUrl":"10.1037/dev0001781","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The college transition is a time of great emotional lability, and sharing emotional experiences with parents can be beneficial for first-year students. Guided by the social sharing of emotions framework and the cultural theory on self-construal, this study investigated the developmental course of freshmen's emotional disclosure with parents during the first semester and the mediating role of independence-orientation in two Asian contexts-Beijing (China) and Singapore. Using experience sampling method (online diary), 205 Chinese freshmen (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 19.43) and 291 Singapore freshmen (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 19.44) reported on four facets of emotional disclosure with parents (frequency, intimacy, negativity, and positivity) across four timepoints and their independence-interdependence orientation (Time 1). Latent growth curve modeling comparing the two Asian samples revealed that frequency increased for Chinese students and decreased for Singapore students. Intimacy indicated upward trajectories for both Asian samples, whereas negativity and positivity showed downward trajectories but were less pronounced for Chinese students. Mediated latent growth curve modeling revealed that the cultures predicted independence-orientation, which in turn negatively predicted increase in frequency for the Singapore sample and positively predicted decrease for the Chinese sample. Independence-orientation negatively predicted increase in intimacy, positively predicted decline in negativity, and negatively predicted decline in positivity. In sum, we found complex and differentiated trajectories for the four facets of emotional disclosure in two Asian samples and the mediating role of independence-orientation in explaining cultural differences in the trajectories, which have implications for understanding emotional disclosure to parents during the developmental phase of the college transition in Asian contexts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1935-1947"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141560086","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Less is not always more: Rich and meaningful counting books lead to greater gains in number understanding than sparse counting books. 少并不总是多:与内容稀少的数数书相比,内容丰富、意义深远的数数书更能加深幼儿对数字的理解。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001826
Cristina Carrazza, Susan C Levine

Children vary widely in their number knowledge by the time they enter kindergarten, and this variation is related to their future academic success. Although talk about number predicts children's early understanding of foundational number concepts, we know little about whether interventions can increase this talk nor about the types of number talk that are most beneficial to children's number understanding. The current project examines whether embedding number talk in goal-based stories leads to more robust number learning than providing the same numeric input outside of this context. Parent-child dyads (N = 71; child age = 3 years) were randomly assigned within their preintervention cardinal number understanding levels to one of three conditions: rich counting books (narrative and pictures involving a number goal), sparse counting books (text and pictures that provide opportunities to count and label the same cardinalities as in the rich condition but with no narrative), or nonnumerical control books that involve labeling colors of objects. Children's number knowledge was measured at pretest and at 2 and 4 weeks into the intervention. Findings showed that children randomized to rich counting book condition showed significantly greater cardinal number knowledge and counting skill by the final testing session compared to children in the sparse counting book condition and the color control book condition, which did not significantly differ from each other. Results have implications for the types of number talk that most effectively support children's number learning in the home environment and for the design of interventions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

儿童在进入幼儿园时,对数字的认识存在很大差异,而这种差异与他们未来的学业成就息息相关。虽然关于数字的谈话能预测儿童对基础数字概念的早期理解,但我们对干预措施是否能增加这种谈话以及对儿童数字理解最有益的数字谈话类型知之甚少。本项目研究了在基于目标的故事中嵌入数字对话是否会比在此背景之外提供相同的数字输入更能促进儿童的数字学习。亲子二人组(N = 71;儿童年龄 = 3 岁)在干预前的心算数字理解水平内被随机分配到三种条件之一:丰富的数数书(涉及数字目标的叙述和图片)、稀疏的数数书(提供数数和标注与丰富条件相同的心算数字机会的文字和图片,但没有叙述)或涉及标注物体颜色的非数字对照书。儿童的数字知识是在干预前、干预后 2 周和 4 周进行测量的。结果表明,在最后的测试中,被随机分配到内容丰富的数数书条件下的儿童,与内容稀少的数数书条件下的儿童和颜色对照书条件下的儿童相比,在心数知识和数数技能方面都有显著提高,但两者之间没有显著差异。研究结果对在家庭环境中最有效地帮助儿童学习数字的数字谈话类型以及干预措施的设计具有启示意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Less is not always more: Rich and meaningful counting books lead to greater gains in number understanding than sparse counting books.","authors":"Cristina Carrazza, Susan C Levine","doi":"10.1037/dev0001826","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001826","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Children vary widely in their number knowledge by the time they enter kindergarten, and this variation is related to their future academic success. Although talk about number predicts children's early understanding of foundational number concepts, we know little about whether interventions can increase this talk nor about the types of number talk that are most beneficial to children's number understanding. The current project examines whether embedding number talk in goal-based stories leads to more robust number learning than providing the same numeric input outside of this context. Parent-child dyads (<i>N</i> = 71; child age = 3 years) were randomly assigned within their preintervention cardinal number understanding levels to one of three conditions: rich counting books (narrative and pictures involving a number goal), sparse counting books (text and pictures that provide opportunities to count and label the same cardinalities as in the rich condition but with no narrative), or nonnumerical control books that involve labeling colors of objects. Children's number knowledge was measured at pretest and at 2 and 4 weeks into the intervention. Findings showed that children randomized to rich counting book condition showed significantly greater cardinal number knowledge and counting skill by the final testing session compared to children in the sparse counting book condition and the color control book condition, which did not significantly differ from each other. Results have implications for the types of number talk that most effectively support children's number learning in the home environment and for the design of interventions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337190","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ethical and epistemic costs of a lack of geographical and cultural diversity in developmental science. 发展科学缺乏地理和文化多样性的伦理和认识代价。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001841
Leher Singh, Dana Basnight-Brown, Bobby K Cheon, Rowena Garcia, Melanie Killen, Reiko Mazuka

Increasing geographical and cultural diversity in research participation has been a key priority for psychological researchers. In this article, we track changes in participant diversity in developmental science over the past decade. These analyses reveal surprisingly modest shifts in global diversity of research participants over time, calling into question the generalizability of our empirical foundation. We provide examples from the study of early child development of the significant epistemic and ethical costs of a lack of geographical and cultural diversity to demonstrate why greater diversification is essential to a generalizable science of human development. We also discuss strategies for diversification that could be implemented throughout the research ecosystem in the service of a culturally anchored, generalizable, and replicable science. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

提高研究参与的地域和文化多样性一直是心理学研究人员的首要任务。在本文中,我们追踪了过去十年来发展科学领域参与者多样性的变化。这些分析表明,随着时间的推移,研究参与者的全球多样性发生了令人惊讶的微小变化,这让我们对我们的经验基础是否具有普遍性产生了疑问。我们从儿童早期发展研究中举例说明了缺乏地域和文化多样性所带来的重大认识论和伦理代价,从而说明为什么更大程度的多样性对于可推广的人类发展科学至关重要。我们还讨论了可在整个研究生态系统中实施的多样化战略,以服务于一门立足于文化、可普及和可复制的科学。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Ethical and epistemic costs of a lack of geographical and cultural diversity in developmental science.","authors":"Leher Singh, Dana Basnight-Brown, Bobby K Cheon, Rowena Garcia, Melanie Killen, Reiko Mazuka","doi":"10.1037/dev0001841","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001841","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Increasing geographical and cultural diversity in research participation has been a key priority for psychological researchers. In this article, we track changes in participant diversity in developmental science over the past decade. These analyses reveal surprisingly modest shifts in global diversity of research participants over time, calling into question the generalizability of our empirical foundation. We provide examples from the study of early child development of the significant epistemic and ethical costs of a lack of geographical and cultural diversity to demonstrate why greater diversification is essential to a generalizable science of human development. We also discuss strategies for diversification that could be implemented throughout the research ecosystem in the service of a culturally anchored, generalizable, and replicable science. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337187","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Computational modeling approaches to emotional development. 情绪发展的计算建模方法。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001830
Andrea G Stein, Seth D Pollak

Computational models of development have the potential to address a key challenge in emotional development research: investigating not only what changes across development but also how these changes come about. Drawing on connectionist and Bayesian methods, this review considers how computational modeling could augment the processes of theorizing and behavioral research to investigate causal processes underlying emotional development. As an illustrative example, we consider how different modeling approaches could help researchers evaluate different ideas about how children come to reason about others' emotions in increasingly sophisticated ways across development. This example is just a starting point; we propose that computational modeling could be an invaluable tool for exploring a variety of yet unresolved "how" questions in emotional development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

情绪发展的计算模型有可能解决情绪发展研究中的一个关键挑战:不仅要研究在整个发展过程中发生了哪些变化,还要研究这些变化是如何产生的。本综述借鉴联结主义和贝叶斯方法,探讨了计算建模如何增强理论化和行为研究过程,以调查情绪发展的因果过程。作为一个示例,我们考虑了不同的建模方法如何帮助研究人员评估不同的观点,即儿童如何在整个发展过程中以越来越复杂的方式推理他人的情绪。这个例子只是一个起点;我们建议,计算建模可以成为探索情绪发展中各种尚未解决的 "如何 "问题的宝贵工具。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Computational modeling approaches to emotional development.","authors":"Andrea G Stein, Seth D Pollak","doi":"10.1037/dev0001830","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001830","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Computational models of development have the potential to address a key challenge in emotional development research: investigating not only what changes across development but also how these changes come about. Drawing on connectionist and Bayesian methods, this review considers how computational modeling could augment the processes of theorizing and behavioral research to investigate causal processes underlying emotional development. As an illustrative example, we consider how different modeling approaches could help researchers evaluate different ideas about how children come to reason about others' emotions in increasingly sophisticated ways across development. This example is just a starting point; we propose that computational modeling could be an invaluable tool for exploring a variety of yet unresolved \"how\" questions in emotional development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337185","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Childhood maltreatment, social connectedness, and depression: A prospective analysis of trajectories over time. 童年虐待、社会联系和抑郁症:对随时间变化的轨迹的前瞻性分析。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001836
Hang Heather Do, Cathy Spatz Widom

Childhood maltreatment is associated with a range of negative social and psychological outcomes at different developmental stages. Using data from a prospective longitudinal study of the consequences of childhood maltreatment, we examine whether childhood maltreatment predicts lower levels of social connectedness and more depression symptoms over a 30-year time period and examine the directionality of the trajectories from childhood into middle adulthood. Children (ages 0-11 years) with documented histories of maltreatment and demographically matched controls were followed into adulthood across four waves: 1989-1995 (n = 1,196; Mage = 29.2 years, SD = 3.9), 2000-2002 (n = 896; Mage = 39.5 years, SD = 3.6), 2003-2005 (n = 808; Mage = 41.2 years, SD = 3.6), and 2009-2010 (n = 649; Mage = 47.0 years, SD = 3.5). Social connectedness and depressive symptoms were measured at all four time points. Random intercept cross-lagged panel models were used to estimate bidirectional relationships between social connectedness and depression symptoms and the extent to which the trajectories differed for the maltreated and control groups. Individuals with documented histories of childhood maltreatment generally had significantly lower levels of social connectedness and higher levels of depression compared to matched controls. Depression was associated with lower levels of social connectedness concurrently at ages 39, 41, and 47. Depression predicted lower social connectedness, and social connectedness predicted lower depression in middle adulthood, but not young adulthood. Better understanding of the long-term impact of childhood maltreatment on social connectedness and depression has implications for designing effective interventions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

童年虐待与不同成长阶段的一系列负面社会和心理结果有关。利用一项关于儿童虐待后果的前瞻性纵向研究的数据,我们研究了在长达 30 年的时间里,儿童虐待是否预示着较低的社会联系水平和较多的抑郁症状,并研究了从童年到成年中期的轨迹的方向性。我们对有虐待史记录的儿童(0-11 岁)和人口统计学上匹配的对照组进行了四次成年期跟踪调查:1989-1995年(n = 1,196;平均年龄 = 29.2岁,SD = 3.9)、2000-2002年(n = 896;平均年龄 = 39.5岁,SD = 3.6)、2003-2005年(n = 808;平均年龄 = 41.2岁,SD = 3.6)和2009-2010年(n = 649;平均年龄 = 47.0岁,SD = 3.5)。在所有四个时间点都测量了社会联系和抑郁症状。随机截距交叉滞后面板模型用于估计社会联系与抑郁症状之间的双向关系,以及受虐待组和对照组的轨迹差异程度。与匹配的对照组相比,有记录的童年虐待史者的社会联系水平普遍较低,抑郁水平较高。在 39、41 和 47 岁时,抑郁与较低的社会联系水平同时存在。抑郁症预示着较低的社会联系性,而社会联系性预示着中年期较低的抑郁水平,但并不预示着青年期的抑郁水平。更好地了解童年虐待对社会联系性和抑郁症的长期影响,对设计有效的干预措施具有重要意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Childhood maltreatment, social connectedness, and depression: A prospective analysis of trajectories over time.","authors":"Hang Heather Do, Cathy Spatz Widom","doi":"10.1037/dev0001836","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001836","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Childhood maltreatment is associated with a range of negative social and psychological outcomes at different developmental stages. Using data from a prospective longitudinal study of the consequences of childhood maltreatment, we examine whether childhood maltreatment predicts lower levels of social connectedness and more depression symptoms over a 30-year time period and examine the directionality of the trajectories from childhood into middle adulthood. Children (ages 0-11 years) with documented histories of maltreatment and demographically matched controls were followed into adulthood across four waves: 1989-1995 (<i>n</i> = 1,196; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 29.2 years, <i>SD</i> = 3.9), 2000-2002 (<i>n</i> = 896; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 39.5 years, <i>SD</i> = 3.6), 2003-2005 (<i>n</i> = 808; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 41.2 years, <i>SD</i> = 3.6), and 2009-2010 (n = 649; Mage = 47.0 years, SD = 3.5). Social connectedness and depressive symptoms were measured at all four time points. Random intercept cross-lagged panel models were used to estimate bidirectional relationships between social connectedness and depression symptoms and the extent to which the trajectories differed for the maltreated and control groups. Individuals with documented histories of childhood maltreatment generally had significantly lower levels of social connectedness and higher levels of depression compared to matched controls. Depression was associated with lower levels of social connectedness concurrently at ages 39, 41, and 47. Depression predicted lower social connectedness, and social connectedness predicted lower depression in middle adulthood, but not young adulthood. Better understanding of the long-term impact of childhood maltreatment on social connectedness and depression has implications for designing effective interventions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337184","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of changes in peer victimization from elementary to high school and autonomic reactivity on adolescent reactive aggression. 从小学到高中,同伴受害情况的变化以及自律性反应对青少年反应性攻击的影响。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001840
Christina C Moore, Julie A Hubbard, Fanny Mlawer, Zachary Meehan, Megan Bookhout

The goals of the present study were to investigate links between changes in peer victimization from elementary to high school and adolescent reactive aggression (Goal 1), whether heightened autonomic nervous system (ANS) reactivity to social and nonsocial stress increases risk for adolescent reactive aggression (Goal 2), and whether increased ANS reactivity strengthens the association between changes in victimization and adolescent reactive aggression (Goal 3). Participants included 145 adolescents (Mage = 16; 54% female; 76% European American, 13% African American, 11% Latino American, 7% Asian American, 5% of mixed race or ethnicity; 60% with family incomes of $100,000 or greater). We collected self-report data in elementary (Time 1 [T1]); (Time 2 [T2]); middle (Time 3 [T3]); and high school (Time 4 [T4]) to assess victimization. At T4, we measured self-reported reactive and proactive aggression, and ANS reactivity (preejection period [PEP], respiratory sinus arrhythmia [RSA]) to peer rejection and nonsocial frustration. More positive victimization slope over time (meaning both less decreasing slopes and increasing slopes) predicted greater adolescent reactive, but not proactive aggression (Goal 1). Greater RSA augmentation to peer rejection and more PEP reactivity to nonsocial frustration predicted more reactive, but not proactive aggression (Goal 2). The link between victimization slope and reactive aggression emerged only for adolescents exhibiting RSA augmentation to peer rejection (marginal; Goal 3). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究的目标是调查从小学到高中同伴受害情况的变化与青少年反应性攻击之间的联系(目标 1),自律神经系统(ANS)对社会和非社会压力的反应性是否会增加青少年反应性攻击的风险(目标 2),自律神经系统反应性的增加是否会加强受害情况的变化与青少年反应性攻击之间的联系(目标 3)。参与者包括 145 名青少年(16 岁;54% 为女性;76% 为欧洲裔美国人,13% 为非洲裔美国人,11% 为拉丁美洲裔美国人,7% 为亚裔美国人,5% 为混血或少数民族;60% 的家庭收入在 10 万美元或以上)。我们收集了小学(时间 1 [T1])、初中(时间 2 [T2])、高中(时间 3 [T3])和高中(时间 4 [T4])的自我报告数据,以评估受害情况。在时间 4,我们测量了自我报告的反应性和主动攻击性,以及自律神经系统对同伴排斥和非社交挫折的反应性(前射期[PEP]、呼吸窦性心律失常[RSA])。随着时间的推移,受害斜率越正(即斜率递减和斜率递增都较小),青少年的反应性攻击行为就越大,而主动性攻击行为则不大(目标 1)。RSA对同伴排斥的增强作用越大,PEP对非社会挫折的反应性越强,则预示着青少年的反应性攻击行为越强,而非主动性攻击行为越强(目标2)。只有对同伴排斥表现出 RSA 增强的青少年(边缘;目标 3)才会出现受害斜率与反应性攻击之间的联系。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The role of changes in peer victimization from elementary to high school and autonomic reactivity on adolescent reactive aggression.","authors":"Christina C Moore, Julie A Hubbard, Fanny Mlawer, Zachary Meehan, Megan Bookhout","doi":"10.1037/dev0001840","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001840","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The goals of the present study were to investigate links between changes in peer victimization from elementary to high school and adolescent reactive aggression (Goal 1), whether heightened autonomic nervous system (ANS) reactivity to social and nonsocial stress increases risk for adolescent reactive aggression (Goal 2), and whether increased ANS reactivity strengthens the association between changes in victimization and adolescent reactive aggression (Goal 3). Participants included 145 adolescents (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 16; 54% female; 76% European American, 13% African American, 11% Latino American, 7% Asian American, 5% of mixed race or ethnicity; 60% with family incomes of $100,000 or greater). We collected self-report data in elementary (Time 1 [T1]); (Time 2 [T2]); middle (Time 3 [T3]); and high school (Time 4 [T4]) to assess victimization. At T4, we measured self-reported reactive and proactive aggression, and ANS reactivity (preejection period [PEP], respiratory sinus arrhythmia [RSA]) to peer rejection and nonsocial frustration. More positive victimization slope over time (meaning both less decreasing slopes and increasing slopes) predicted greater adolescent reactive, but not proactive aggression (Goal 1). Greater RSA augmentation to peer rejection and more PEP reactivity to nonsocial frustration predicted more reactive, but not proactive aggression (Goal 2). The link between victimization slope and reactive aggression emerged only for adolescents exhibiting RSA augmentation to peer rejection (marginal; Goal 3). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337191","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Developmental cascades from maternal depressive symptoms in childhood to adolescents' friendship quality: A 13-year longitudinal study. 从母亲童年时期的抑郁症状到青少年友谊质量的发展级联:一项为期 13 年的纵向研究。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001834
Laura Bechtiger, Annekatrin Steinhoff, Jessica Dollar, Susan Keane, Susan Calkins, Lilly Shanahan

Adolescent friendships of positive quality promote well-being for decades to come. But what impedes the development of positive friendship quality? The present study examined whether maternal depressive symptoms during early childhood predict children's friendship quality into adolescence, and whether observed negative parenting behavior and children's earlier friendship quality, social skills, and their own depressive symptoms in middle childhood mediate these associations. We used six waves of data from a prospective longitudinal community sample (N = 396). The study followed children and their mothers across 13 years from child ages 2-15 years (52% female, 67% White, 26% Black), collecting multi-informant data (from mothers, children, teachers, and behavioral observations). The significance of indirect effects was estimated with structural equation modeling. Exposure to high levels of maternal depressive symptoms in early childhood (child ages 2-5 years) was negatively correlated with children's later adolescent friendship quality (at age 15). Structural equation models revealed that this association was mediated by children's poorer social skills (age 7) and friendship quality (age 10). Negative parenting behavior and children's depressive symptoms did not mediate this association. Maternal depressive symptoms have downstream associations with children's friendship quality into adolescence, including via children's social skills. Promoting the social skills of children exposed to maternal depressive symptoms could have long-term positive effects. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

具有积极品质的青少年友谊能促进未来几十年的幸福。但是,是什么阻碍了积极友谊品质的发展呢?本研究探讨了幼儿期的母亲抑郁症状是否会影响孩子进入青春期后的友谊质量,以及观察到的负面养育行为和孩子早期的友谊质量、社交技能以及他们自己在中年期的抑郁症状是否会调节这些关联。我们使用了前瞻性纵向社区样本(N = 396)的六波数据。该研究对 2-15 岁儿童(52% 为女性,67% 为白人,26% 为黑人)及其母亲进行了长达 13 年的跟踪调查,收集了多种信息数据(来自母亲、儿童、教师和行为观察)。通过结构方程模型估计了间接效应的重要性。儿童早期(2-5 岁儿童)接触高水平的母亲抑郁症状与儿童青少年时期(15 岁)的友谊质量呈负相关。结构方程模型显示,这种关联通过儿童较差的社交技能(7 岁)和友谊质量(10 岁)来调节。父母的负面教养行为和儿童的抑郁症状并没有起到中介作用。母亲的抑郁症状与儿童进入青春期后的友谊质量有下游关联,包括通过儿童的社交技能。提高受母亲抑郁症状影响的儿童的社交技能可能会产生长期的积极影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Developmental cascades from maternal depressive symptoms in childhood to adolescents' friendship quality: A 13-year longitudinal study.","authors":"Laura Bechtiger, Annekatrin Steinhoff, Jessica Dollar, Susan Keane, Susan Calkins, Lilly Shanahan","doi":"10.1037/dev0001834","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001834","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Adolescent friendships of positive quality promote well-being for decades to come. But what impedes the development of positive friendship quality? The present study examined whether maternal depressive symptoms during early childhood predict children's friendship quality into adolescence, and whether observed negative parenting behavior and children's earlier friendship quality, social skills, and their own depressive symptoms in middle childhood mediate these associations. We used six waves of data from a prospective longitudinal community sample (<i>N</i> = 396). The study followed children and their mothers across 13 years from child ages 2-15 years (52% female, 67% White, 26% Black), collecting multi-informant data (from mothers, children, teachers, and behavioral observations). The significance of indirect effects was estimated with structural equation modeling. Exposure to high levels of maternal depressive symptoms in early childhood (child ages 2-5 years) was negatively correlated with children's later adolescent friendship quality (at age 15). Structural equation models revealed that this association was mediated by children's poorer social skills (age 7) and friendship quality (age 10). Negative parenting behavior and children's depressive symptoms did not mediate this association. Maternal depressive symptoms have downstream associations with children's friendship quality into adolescence, including via children's social skills. Promoting the social skills of children exposed to maternal depressive symptoms could have long-term positive effects. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337186","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
INSIGHT responsive parenting intervention effects on child self-regulation at ages 3 and 6 years. INSIGHT 应对型养育干预对 3 岁和 6 岁儿童自我调节能力的影响。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001839
Erika Hernandez Acton, Elizabeth Kubiniec, Sakshi Bhargava, Sara Tauriello, Ian M Paul, Jennifer S Savage, Stephanie Anzman-Frasca

Self-regulation encompasses the ability to modulate behavior, cognition, and emotions. Parents can promote child self-regulation with responsive parenting (RP). RP shapes various components of self-regulation and is associated with numerous developmental outcomes. Here, we examine long-term effects of an early-life RP intervention designed for obesity prevention on later child self-regulation and temperament. Participants were from a randomized clinical trial comparing the RP intervention against a safety control (n = 279). RP intervention content in the domains of feeding, sleep, emotion regulation, and interactive play was delivered to primiparous mothers and infants at four home visits during the first year after birth, followed by clinical research center visits at ages 1 and 2 years and phone calls at 1.5 and 2.5 years. Child self-regulation and temperament were assessed with behavioral tasks and the Children's Behavior Questionnaire at child ages 3 and 6 years. A path model tested whether the RP intervention affected child self-regulation in comparison to the control group. At 6 years, children in the RP group had lower parent-reported negative affect (b = -0.34, SE = 0.15, p = .023) and better observed emotion regulation (b = 0.45, SE = 0.16, p = .007). Findings indicate that an RP intervention designed for early obesity prevention promoted emotional aspects of self-regulation in middle childhood, highlighting RP as a strategy for promoting healthy behavior across multiple domains. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

自我调节包括调节行为、认知和情绪的能力。父母可以通过反应型养育(RP)来促进儿童的自我调节能力。RP 塑造了自我调节的各种要素,并与许多发展结果相关。在此,我们研究了为预防肥胖而设计的早期反应性养育干预对儿童日后自我调节和气质的长期影响。参与者来自一项随机临床试验,该试验将 RP 干预与安全对照进行了比较(n = 279)。在初产妇和婴儿出生后第一年的四次家访中,向其提供了喂养、睡眠、情绪调节和互动游戏等方面的 RP 干预内容,随后在 1 岁和 2 岁时进行了临床研究中心访问,并在 1.5 岁和 2.5 岁时进行了电话访问。在儿童 3 岁和 6 岁时,通过行为任务和儿童行为问卷对儿童的自我调节能力和性情进行了评估。与对照组相比,路径模型检验了 RP 干预是否会影响儿童的自我调节能力。6 岁时,RP 组儿童的父母报告的负面情绪较低(b = -0.34,SE = 0.15,p = .023),观察到的情绪调节较好(b = 0.45,SE = 0.16,p = .007)。研究结果表明,为预防早期肥胖症而设计的 RP 干预措施促进了儿童中期情绪方面的自我调节,突出表明 RP 是一种促进多领域健康行为的策略。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"INSIGHT responsive parenting intervention effects on child self-regulation at ages 3 and 6 years.","authors":"Erika Hernandez Acton, Elizabeth Kubiniec, Sakshi Bhargava, Sara Tauriello, Ian M Paul, Jennifer S Savage, Stephanie Anzman-Frasca","doi":"10.1037/dev0001839","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001839","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Self-regulation encompasses the ability to modulate behavior, cognition, and emotions. Parents can promote child self-regulation with responsive parenting (RP). RP shapes various components of self-regulation and is associated with numerous developmental outcomes. Here, we examine long-term effects of an early-life RP intervention designed for obesity prevention on later child self-regulation and temperament. Participants were from a randomized clinical trial comparing the RP intervention against a safety control (<i>n</i> = 279). RP intervention content in the domains of feeding, sleep, emotion regulation, and interactive play was delivered to primiparous mothers and infants at four home visits during the first year after birth, followed by clinical research center visits at ages 1 and 2 years and phone calls at 1.5 and 2.5 years. Child self-regulation and temperament were assessed with behavioral tasks and the Children's Behavior Questionnaire at child ages 3 and 6 years. A path model tested whether the RP intervention affected child self-regulation in comparison to the control group. At 6 years, children in the RP group had lower parent-reported negative affect (<i>b</i> = -0.34, <i>SE</i> = 0.15, <i>p</i> = .023) and better observed emotion regulation (<i>b</i> = 0.45, <i>SE</i> = 0.16, <i>p</i> = .007). Findings indicate that an RP intervention designed for early obesity prevention promoted emotional aspects of self-regulation in middle childhood, highlighting RP as a strategy for promoting healthy behavior across multiple domains. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337189","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reciprocal relationships between a child's engagement with faces and mother-child interaction at 8, 30, and 60 months. 8 个月、30 个月和 60 个月时儿童与面孔的互动与母子互动之间的相互关系。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001831
Aino Luotola, Riikka Korja, Jukka Leppänen, Akie Yada, Eeva Eskola, Tuomo Häikiö, Hetti Lahtela, Eeva Holmberg, Elisabeth Nordenswan, Saara Nolvi, Hasse Karlsson, Linnea Karlsson, Eeva-Leena Kataja

Prioritized attention to faces can be viewed as an early-developing marker of social engagement. This behavior is closely linked with early interactions, but there has been little research examining the longitudinal associations between social engagement and parent-child interaction. We examined the reciprocal relations between mother-child interaction and child engagement with faces from infancy to preschool age. Participants of this study were 738 mother-child dyads from the FinnBrain Birth Cohort. We used Emotional Availability Scales to examine mothers' emotional availability in interaction and eye tracking to examine attention dwell time for pictured faces and nonface patterns under distraction at 8, 30, and 60 months. Using a random intercept cross-lagged panel model, which differentiates between-dyad variance from within-dyad variance (deviations from the individual's latent average), we found that higher maternal emotional availability was associated with shorter dwell time for faces at the between-dyad level. At the within-dyad level, stability (smaller deviations from the individual's latent average) in a mother's emotional availability at 30 months was associated with stability in the child's face engagement in the subsequent assessment at 60 months. Similar associations were not found in analyses of dwell times for nonfaces. Together, our findings show an interconnection between mother-child interaction and the child's engagement with faces and raise the possibility that shifts in the quality of these interactions within specific developmental stage may lead to changes in how children engage with social cues. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

对人脸的优先关注可被视为社会参与的早期发展标志。这种行为与早期互动密切相关,但很少有研究探讨社会参与与亲子互动之间的纵向联系。我们研究了从婴儿期到学龄前期母子互动与儿童参与面孔之间的相互关系。本研究的参与者是芬兰脑出生队列中的 738 个母子二人组。我们使用情感可得性量表来考察母亲在互动中的情感可得性,并使用眼动跟踪来考察 8 个月、30 个月和 60 个月时,在注意力分散的情况下,孩子对图画人脸和非人脸图案的注意力停留时间。使用随机截距交叉滞后面板模型(该模型区分了日间方差和日内方差(偏离个体潜在平均值的偏差)),我们发现,在日间水平上,较高的母亲情绪可用性与较短的面孔停留时间相关。在阶段内水平上,母亲在 30 个月时的情绪可得性的稳定性(与个体潜平均值的偏差较小)与儿童在 60 个月时的后续评估中的面孔参与稳定性相关。在对非脸部的停留时间进行分析时,没有发现类似的关联。总之,我们的研究结果表明,母子互动与儿童对人脸的参与之间存在着相互联系,并提出了一种可能性,即在特定的发展阶段,这些互动质量的变化可能会导致儿童参与社交线索的方式发生变化。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Reciprocal relationships between a child's engagement with faces and mother-child interaction at 8, 30, and 60 months.","authors":"Aino Luotola, Riikka Korja, Jukka Leppänen, Akie Yada, Eeva Eskola, Tuomo Häikiö, Hetti Lahtela, Eeva Holmberg, Elisabeth Nordenswan, Saara Nolvi, Hasse Karlsson, Linnea Karlsson, Eeva-Leena Kataja","doi":"10.1037/dev0001831","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001831","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prioritized attention to faces can be viewed as an early-developing marker of social engagement. This behavior is closely linked with early interactions, but there has been little research examining the longitudinal associations between social engagement and parent-child interaction. We examined the reciprocal relations between mother-child interaction and child engagement with faces from infancy to preschool age. Participants of this study were 738 mother-child dyads from the FinnBrain Birth Cohort. We used Emotional Availability Scales to examine mothers' emotional availability in interaction and eye tracking to examine attention dwell time for pictured faces and nonface patterns under distraction at 8, 30, and 60 months. Using a random intercept cross-lagged panel model, which differentiates between-dyad variance from within-dyad variance (deviations from the individual's latent average), we found that higher maternal emotional availability was associated with shorter dwell time for faces at the between-dyad level. At the within-dyad level, stability (smaller deviations from the individual's latent average) in a mother's emotional availability at 30 months was associated with stability in the child's face engagement in the subsequent assessment at 60 months. Similar associations were not found in analyses of dwell times for nonfaces. Together, our findings show an interconnection between mother-child interaction and the child's engagement with faces and raise the possibility that shifts in the quality of these interactions within specific developmental stage may lead to changes in how children engage with social cues. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298905","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parental guidance fosters hands-on learning by infants in culturally different ways. 父母的指导以不同的文化方式促进婴儿动手学习。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001842
Su-Hua Wang

The present research examined cultural patterns of parental guidance for infants as they learned about a new physical rule with hands-on experience. Nine-month-olds participated in two sites: Taipei, Taiwan and Santa Cruz, California, United States (N = 96; 48 males, 48 females). They watched a single exemplar of covering events that demonstrated the to-be-learned rule, which was insufficient visual experience to learn the rule. As infants explored the objects while observing, their mothers provided culturally distinct guidance. The dyads in Taipei co-enacted directive guidance through parents' hand-holding infants, whereas the dyads in Santa Cruz focused on infants' free exploration. Despite different emphases of learning, both groups of infants benefited from hands-on experience and learned the rule with the single exemplar. The finding points to diverse pathways to support the early development of physical concepts for infants from different cultural backgrounds. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究考察了在婴儿通过亲身体验了解新的物理规则时,父母对婴儿进行指导的文化模式。九个月大的婴儿参加了两个地点的研究:台湾台北和美国加利福尼亚州圣克鲁斯(N = 96;男 48 人,女 48 人)。他们只观看了一个覆盖事件的范例,该范例展示了要学习的规则,但这并不足以让他们获得学习规则的视觉经验。当婴儿在观察时探索物体时,他们的母亲会提供不同文化背景的指导。台北的二人组通过父母手拉手的方式共同进行指令性指导,而圣克鲁斯的二人组则侧重于婴儿的自由探索。尽管学习的侧重点不同,但两组婴儿都从亲身体验中获益,并通过单一范例学会了规则。这一发现为来自不同文化背景的婴儿提供了支持其早期物理概念发展的不同途径。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Parental guidance fosters hands-on learning by infants in culturally different ways.","authors":"Su-Hua Wang","doi":"10.1037/dev0001842","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001842","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present research examined cultural patterns of parental guidance for infants as they learned about a new physical rule with hands-on experience. Nine-month-olds participated in two sites: Taipei, Taiwan and Santa Cruz, California, United States (<i>N</i> = 96; 48 males, 48 females). They watched a single exemplar of covering events that demonstrated the to-be-learned rule, which was insufficient visual experience to learn the rule. As infants explored the objects while observing, their mothers provided culturally distinct guidance. The dyads in Taipei co-enacted directive guidance through parents' hand-holding infants, whereas the dyads in Santa Cruz focused on infants' free exploration. Despite different emphases of learning, both groups of infants benefited from hands-on experience and learned the rule with the single exemplar. The finding points to diverse pathways to support the early development of physical concepts for infants from different cultural backgrounds. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298904","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Developmental Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1