首页 > 最新文献

Developmental Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
The structure, development, and etiology of observed temperament during middle childhood. 中童年期观察到的气质的结构、发展和病因。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001818
Lior Abramson, Roni Pener-Tessler, Dvir Kleper, Kimberly J Saudino, Jeffrey R Gagne, Matityahu Angel, Ariel Knafo-Noam

Investigating the structure and etiology of temperament is key to understanding how children interact with the world (Kagan, 1994). Although these topics have yielded an abundance of research, fewer studies have employed observational data during middle childhood, when unique environmental challenges could influence temperament development. To address this gap, Israeli twin children were observed at Age 6.5 (N = 1,083, 564 families; 50.6% females) and again at Age 8-9 (N = 768, 388 families; 52.0% females; 611 children from 322 families had data from both ages). Temperament was assessed globally by trained coders and, at Age 8-9, also by the experimenter who interacted with the child. We examined whether Rothbart et al.'s (2000) three-factor model, according to which temperament includes the domains negative affect, positive affect/surgency, and effortful control, emerges from the data. In addition, we considered a bifactor model, where a fourth global factor accounts for all behaviors' commonality. Across the two ages and rating methods, confirmatory factor analyses supported the bifactor model. The global factor's loadings suggested that it reflects children's expressiveness. Adding this factor changed the associations between the other factors and enabled differentiation between surgency and positive affect. This suggests that in observational settings that capture temperament impressions holistically, children's expressiveness affects other traits' behavioral displays. Twin models revealed genetic influences for most traits. Importantly, twin models revealed shared-environmental influences for negative affect and expressiveness, which modestly contributed to temperament consistency across ages. These findings shed light on temperament traits' interrelatedness and stress the importance of the shared environment to temperament development during middle childhood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

调查气质的结构和病因是了解儿童如何与世界互动的关键(Kagan,1994)。尽管这些课题已经产生了大量的研究成果,但采用中童年期观察数据的研究较少,而中童年期独特的环境挑战可能会影响气质的发展。为了填补这一空白,研究人员对以色列双胞胎儿童在 6.5 岁时进行了观察(总数 = 1,083 人,564 个家庭;50.6% 为女性),并在 8-9 岁时再次进行观察(总数 = 768 人,388 个家庭;52.0% 为女性;来自 322 个家庭的 611 名儿童在两个年龄段都有数据)。脾气由训练有素的编码员进行全面评估,在 8-9 岁时,还由与儿童互动的实验员进行评估。我们研究了 Rothbart 等人(2000 年)的三因素模型,根据该模型,气质包括消极情绪、积极情绪/急躁情绪和努力控制。此外,我们还考虑了双因素模型,即由第四个总体因素来解释所有行为的共性。在两个年龄段和评级方法中,确认性因子分析支持双因子模型。全局因子的载荷表明,它反映了儿童的表现力。加入该因子后,其他因子之间的关联发生了变化,并能区分紧急情绪和积极情绪。这表明,在全面捕捉气质印象的观察环境中,儿童的表现力会影响其他特质的行为表现。双胞胎模型揭示了大多数特质的遗传影响。重要的是,双生子模型揭示了消极情绪和表现性的共同环境影响,这在一定程度上促进了不同年龄段儿童气质的一致性。这些发现揭示了气质特征的相互关联性,并强调了共同环境对儿童中期气质发展的重要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The structure, development, and etiology of observed temperament during middle childhood.","authors":"Lior Abramson, Roni Pener-Tessler, Dvir Kleper, Kimberly J Saudino, Jeffrey R Gagne, Matityahu Angel, Ariel Knafo-Noam","doi":"10.1037/dev0001818","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001818","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Investigating the structure and etiology of temperament is key to understanding how children interact with the world (Kagan, 1994). Although these topics have yielded an abundance of research, fewer studies have employed observational data during middle childhood, when unique environmental challenges could influence temperament development. To address this gap, Israeli twin children were observed at Age 6.5 (<i>N</i> = 1,083, 564 families; 50.6% females) and again at Age 8-9 (<i>N</i> = 768, 388 families; 52.0% females; 611 children from 322 families had data from both ages). Temperament was assessed globally by trained coders and, at Age 8-9, also by the experimenter who interacted with the child. We examined whether Rothbart et al.'s (2000) three-factor model, according to which temperament includes the domains negative affect, positive affect/surgency, and effortful control, emerges from the data. In addition, we considered a bifactor model, where a fourth global factor accounts for all behaviors' commonality. Across the two ages and rating methods, confirmatory factor analyses supported the bifactor model. The global factor's loadings suggested that it reflects children's expressiveness. Adding this factor changed the associations between the other factors and enabled differentiation between surgency and positive affect. This suggests that in observational settings that capture temperament impressions holistically, children's expressiveness affects other traits' behavioral displays. Twin models revealed genetic influences for most traits. Importantly, twin models revealed shared-environmental influences for negative affect and expressiveness, which modestly contributed to temperament consistency across ages. These findings shed light on temperament traits' interrelatedness and stress the importance of the shared environment to temperament development during middle childhood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337192","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
INSIGHT responsive parenting intervention effects on child self-regulation at ages 3 and 6 years. INSIGHT 应对型养育干预对 3 岁和 6 岁儿童自我调节能力的影响。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001839
Erika Hernandez Acton, Elizabeth Kubiniec, Sakshi Bhargava, Sara Tauriello, Ian M Paul, Jennifer S Savage, Stephanie Anzman-Frasca

Self-regulation encompasses the ability to modulate behavior, cognition, and emotions. Parents can promote child self-regulation with responsive parenting (RP). RP shapes various components of self-regulation and is associated with numerous developmental outcomes. Here, we examine long-term effects of an early-life RP intervention designed for obesity prevention on later child self-regulation and temperament. Participants were from a randomized clinical trial comparing the RP intervention against a safety control (n = 279). RP intervention content in the domains of feeding, sleep, emotion regulation, and interactive play was delivered to primiparous mothers and infants at four home visits during the first year after birth, followed by clinical research center visits at ages 1 and 2 years and phone calls at 1.5 and 2.5 years. Child self-regulation and temperament were assessed with behavioral tasks and the Children's Behavior Questionnaire at child ages 3 and 6 years. A path model tested whether the RP intervention affected child self-regulation in comparison to the control group. At 6 years, children in the RP group had lower parent-reported negative affect (b = -0.34, SE = 0.15, p = .023) and better observed emotion regulation (b = 0.45, SE = 0.16, p = .007). Findings indicate that an RP intervention designed for early obesity prevention promoted emotional aspects of self-regulation in middle childhood, highlighting RP as a strategy for promoting healthy behavior across multiple domains. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

自我调节包括调节行为、认知和情绪的能力。父母可以通过反应型养育(RP)来促进儿童的自我调节能力。RP 塑造了自我调节的各种要素,并与许多发展结果相关。在此,我们研究了为预防肥胖而设计的早期反应性养育干预对儿童日后自我调节和气质的长期影响。参与者来自一项随机临床试验,该试验将 RP 干预与安全对照进行了比较(n = 279)。在初产妇和婴儿出生后第一年的四次家访中,向其提供了喂养、睡眠、情绪调节和互动游戏等方面的 RP 干预内容,随后在 1 岁和 2 岁时进行了临床研究中心访问,并在 1.5 岁和 2.5 岁时进行了电话访问。在儿童 3 岁和 6 岁时,通过行为任务和儿童行为问卷对儿童的自我调节能力和性情进行了评估。与对照组相比,路径模型检验了 RP 干预是否会影响儿童的自我调节能力。6 岁时,RP 组儿童的父母报告的负面情绪较低(b = -0.34,SE = 0.15,p = .023),观察到的情绪调节较好(b = 0.45,SE = 0.16,p = .007)。研究结果表明,为预防早期肥胖症而设计的 RP 干预措施促进了儿童中期情绪方面的自我调节,突出表明 RP 是一种促进多领域健康行为的策略。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"INSIGHT responsive parenting intervention effects on child self-regulation at ages 3 and 6 years.","authors":"Erika Hernandez Acton, Elizabeth Kubiniec, Sakshi Bhargava, Sara Tauriello, Ian M Paul, Jennifer S Savage, Stephanie Anzman-Frasca","doi":"10.1037/dev0001839","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001839","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Self-regulation encompasses the ability to modulate behavior, cognition, and emotions. Parents can promote child self-regulation with responsive parenting (RP). RP shapes various components of self-regulation and is associated with numerous developmental outcomes. Here, we examine long-term effects of an early-life RP intervention designed for obesity prevention on later child self-regulation and temperament. Participants were from a randomized clinical trial comparing the RP intervention against a safety control (<i>n</i> = 279). RP intervention content in the domains of feeding, sleep, emotion regulation, and interactive play was delivered to primiparous mothers and infants at four home visits during the first year after birth, followed by clinical research center visits at ages 1 and 2 years and phone calls at 1.5 and 2.5 years. Child self-regulation and temperament were assessed with behavioral tasks and the Children's Behavior Questionnaire at child ages 3 and 6 years. A path model tested whether the RP intervention affected child self-regulation in comparison to the control group. At 6 years, children in the RP group had lower parent-reported negative affect (<i>b</i> = -0.34, <i>SE</i> = 0.15, <i>p</i> = .023) and better observed emotion regulation (<i>b</i> = 0.45, <i>SE</i> = 0.16, <i>p</i> = .007). Findings indicate that an RP intervention designed for early obesity prevention promoted emotional aspects of self-regulation in middle childhood, highlighting RP as a strategy for promoting healthy behavior across multiple domains. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337189","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Infants' home auditory environment: Background sounds shape language interactions. 婴儿的家庭听觉环境:背景声音影响语言互动
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001762
Catalina Suarez-Rivera, Katelyn K Fletcher, Catherine S Tamis-LeMonda

Background sounds at home-namely those from television, communication devices, music, appliances, transportation, and construction-can support or impede infant language interactions and learning. Yet real-time connections at home between background sound and infant-caregiver language interactions remain unexamined. We quantified background sounds in the home environment, from 1- to 2-hr video recordings of infant-mother everyday activities (infants aged 8-26 months, 36 female) in two samples: European-American, English-speaking, middle-socioeconomic status (SES) families (N = 36) and Latine, Spanish-speaking, low-SES families (N = 40). From videos, we identified and coded five types of background sound: television/screens, communication devices, music, appliances, and transportation/construction. Exposure to background sounds varied enormously among homes and was stable across a week, with television/screens and music being the most dominant type of background sounds. Infants' vocalizations and mothers' speech to infants were reduced in the presence of background sound (although effect sizes were small), highlighting real-time processes that affect everyday language exchanges. Over the course of a day, infants in homes with high amounts of background sounds may hear and produce less language than infants in homes with less background sounds, highlighting potential cascading influences from environmental features to everyday interactions to language learning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

家中的背景声音,即来自电视、通讯设备、音乐、电器、交通和建筑的声音,可以支持或阻碍婴儿的语言互动和学习。然而,家庭背景声音与婴儿-照顾者语言互动之间的实时联系仍未得到研究。我们通过对两个样本(8-26 个月大的婴儿,36 名女性)进行 1 到 2 小时的婴儿-母亲日常活动视频记录,对家庭环境中的背景声音进行了量化:两个样本:讲英语、社会经济地位中等的欧美家庭(36 人)和讲西班牙语、社会经济地位低的拉丁家庭(40 人)。通过视频,我们确定了五种背景声音并进行了编码:电视/屏幕、通讯设备、音乐、电器和交通/建筑。不同家庭所接触到的背景声音差异很大,但在一周内保持稳定,电视/屏幕和音乐是最主要的背景声音类型。在有背景声音的情况下,婴儿的发声和母亲对婴儿的讲话都会减少(尽管影响大小很小),这突出了影响日常语言交流的实时过程。在一天的时间里,背景声音较多的家庭中的婴儿可能比背景声音较少的家庭中的婴儿听到和产生的语言更少,这凸显了从环境特征到日常互动再到语言学习的潜在连带影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Infants' home auditory environment: Background sounds shape language interactions.","authors":"Catalina Suarez-Rivera, Katelyn K Fletcher, Catherine S Tamis-LeMonda","doi":"10.1037/dev0001762","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001762","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Background sounds at home-namely those from television, communication devices, music, appliances, transportation, and construction-can support or impede infant language interactions and learning. Yet real-time connections at home between background sound and infant-caregiver language interactions remain unexamined. We quantified background sounds in the home environment, from 1- to 2-hr video recordings of infant-mother everyday activities (infants aged 8-26 months, 36 female) in two samples: European-American, English-speaking, middle-socioeconomic status (SES) families (<i>N</i> = 36) and Latine, Spanish-speaking, low-SES families (<i>N</i> = 40). From videos, we identified and coded five types of background sound: television/screens, communication devices, music, appliances, and transportation/construction. Exposure to background sounds varied enormously among homes and was stable across a week, with television/screens and music being the most dominant type of background sounds. Infants' vocalizations and mothers' speech to infants were reduced in the presence of background sound (although effect sizes were small), highlighting real-time processes that affect everyday language exchanges. Over the course of a day, infants in homes with high amounts of background sounds may hear and produce less language than infants in homes with less background sounds, highlighting potential cascading influences from environmental features to everyday interactions to language learning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337188","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reciprocal relationships between a child's engagement with faces and mother-child interaction at 8, 30, and 60 months. 8 个月、30 个月和 60 个月时儿童与面孔的互动与母子互动之间的相互关系。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001831
Aino Luotola, Riikka Korja, Jukka Leppänen, Akie Yada, Eeva Eskola, Tuomo Häikiö, Hetti Lahtela, Eeva Holmberg, Elisabeth Nordenswan, Saara Nolvi, Hasse Karlsson, Linnea Karlsson, Eeva-Leena Kataja

Prioritized attention to faces can be viewed as an early-developing marker of social engagement. This behavior is closely linked with early interactions, but there has been little research examining the longitudinal associations between social engagement and parent-child interaction. We examined the reciprocal relations between mother-child interaction and child engagement with faces from infancy to preschool age. Participants of this study were 738 mother-child dyads from the FinnBrain Birth Cohort. We used Emotional Availability Scales to examine mothers' emotional availability in interaction and eye tracking to examine attention dwell time for pictured faces and nonface patterns under distraction at 8, 30, and 60 months. Using a random intercept cross-lagged panel model, which differentiates between-dyad variance from within-dyad variance (deviations from the individual's latent average), we found that higher maternal emotional availability was associated with shorter dwell time for faces at the between-dyad level. At the within-dyad level, stability (smaller deviations from the individual's latent average) in a mother's emotional availability at 30 months was associated with stability in the child's face engagement in the subsequent assessment at 60 months. Similar associations were not found in analyses of dwell times for nonfaces. Together, our findings show an interconnection between mother-child interaction and the child's engagement with faces and raise the possibility that shifts in the quality of these interactions within specific developmental stage may lead to changes in how children engage with social cues. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

对人脸的优先关注可被视为社会参与的早期发展标志。这种行为与早期互动密切相关,但很少有研究探讨社会参与与亲子互动之间的纵向联系。我们研究了从婴儿期到学龄前期母子互动与儿童参与面孔之间的相互关系。本研究的参与者是芬兰脑出生队列中的 738 个母子二人组。我们使用情感可得性量表来考察母亲在互动中的情感可得性,并使用眼动跟踪来考察 8 个月、30 个月和 60 个月时,在注意力分散的情况下,孩子对图画人脸和非人脸图案的注意力停留时间。使用随机截距交叉滞后面板模型(该模型区分了日间方差和日内方差(偏离个体潜在平均值的偏差)),我们发现,在日间水平上,较高的母亲情绪可用性与较短的面孔停留时间相关。在阶段内水平上,母亲在 30 个月时的情绪可得性的稳定性(与个体潜平均值的偏差较小)与儿童在 60 个月时的后续评估中的面孔参与稳定性相关。在对非脸部的停留时间进行分析时,没有发现类似的关联。总之,我们的研究结果表明,母子互动与儿童对人脸的参与之间存在着相互联系,并提出了一种可能性,即在特定的发展阶段,这些互动质量的变化可能会导致儿童参与社交线索的方式发生变化。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Reciprocal relationships between a child's engagement with faces and mother-child interaction at 8, 30, and 60 months.","authors":"Aino Luotola, Riikka Korja, Jukka Leppänen, Akie Yada, Eeva Eskola, Tuomo Häikiö, Hetti Lahtela, Eeva Holmberg, Elisabeth Nordenswan, Saara Nolvi, Hasse Karlsson, Linnea Karlsson, Eeva-Leena Kataja","doi":"10.1037/dev0001831","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001831","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prioritized attention to faces can be viewed as an early-developing marker of social engagement. This behavior is closely linked with early interactions, but there has been little research examining the longitudinal associations between social engagement and parent-child interaction. We examined the reciprocal relations between mother-child interaction and child engagement with faces from infancy to preschool age. Participants of this study were 738 mother-child dyads from the FinnBrain Birth Cohort. We used Emotional Availability Scales to examine mothers' emotional availability in interaction and eye tracking to examine attention dwell time for pictured faces and nonface patterns under distraction at 8, 30, and 60 months. Using a random intercept cross-lagged panel model, which differentiates between-dyad variance from within-dyad variance (deviations from the individual's latent average), we found that higher maternal emotional availability was associated with shorter dwell time for faces at the between-dyad level. At the within-dyad level, stability (smaller deviations from the individual's latent average) in a mother's emotional availability at 30 months was associated with stability in the child's face engagement in the subsequent assessment at 60 months. Similar associations were not found in analyses of dwell times for nonfaces. Together, our findings show an interconnection between mother-child interaction and the child's engagement with faces and raise the possibility that shifts in the quality of these interactions within specific developmental stage may lead to changes in how children engage with social cues. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298905","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Understanding the developmental transition between parallel and social play during preschool: A multiplex social network analysis. 了解学龄前平行游戏与社交游戏之间的发展过渡:多重社会网络分析。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001837
Jennifer Watling Neal, Zachary P Neal, Brian Brutzman, C Emily Durbin

Preschoolers are expected to transition from parallel play, where they engage in similar activities next to peers, to social play, where they engage in direct interactions with peers. We use longitudinal, multiplex social network analysis to examine the transition between observed parallel and social play over a school year in a 3-year-old classroom (N = 25, 45% girls, 48% White) and a 4-year-old classroom (N = 28, 42.86% girls, 60.71% White). In both classrooms, the existence of a parallel play relationship between two children predicted the formation of a social play relationship between the same two children over time but not vice versa. Findings provide support for a unidirectional, sequential transition from parallel to social play with the same peers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

学龄前儿童会从平行游戏过渡到社交游戏,在平行游戏中,他们会在同伴旁边进行类似的活动,在社交游戏中,他们会与同伴进行直接互动。我们采用纵向、多重社会网络分析方法,研究了一个 3 岁班级(人数=25,45% 为女孩,48% 为白人)和一个 4 岁班级(人数=28,42.86% 为女孩,60.71% 为白人)一学年中观察到的平行游戏和社交游戏之间的过渡情况。在这两个班级中,如果两个孩子之间存在平行游戏关系,则随着时间的推移,这两个孩子之间会形成社交游戏关系,反之亦然。研究结果支持从平行游戏到与同龄人社交游戏的单向、有序过渡。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Understanding the developmental transition between parallel and social play during preschool: A multiplex social network analysis.","authors":"Jennifer Watling Neal, Zachary P Neal, Brian Brutzman, C Emily Durbin","doi":"10.1037/dev0001837","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001837","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Preschoolers are expected to transition from parallel play, where they engage in similar activities next to peers, to social play, where they engage in direct interactions with peers. We use longitudinal, multiplex social network analysis to examine the transition between observed parallel and social play over a school year in a 3-year-old classroom (<i>N</i> = 25, 45% girls, 48% White) and a 4-year-old classroom (<i>N</i> = 28, 42.86% girls, 60.71% White). In both classrooms, the existence of a parallel play relationship between two children predicted the formation of a social play relationship between the same two children over time but not vice versa. Findings provide support for a unidirectional, sequential transition from parallel to social play with the same peers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298907","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parental guidance fosters hands-on learning by infants in culturally different ways. 父母的指导以不同的文化方式促进婴儿动手学习。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001842
Su-Hua Wang

The present research examined cultural patterns of parental guidance for infants as they learned about a new physical rule with hands-on experience. Nine-month-olds participated in two sites: Taipei, Taiwan and Santa Cruz, California, United States (N = 96; 48 males, 48 females). They watched a single exemplar of covering events that demonstrated the to-be-learned rule, which was insufficient visual experience to learn the rule. As infants explored the objects while observing, their mothers provided culturally distinct guidance. The dyads in Taipei co-enacted directive guidance through parents' hand-holding infants, whereas the dyads in Santa Cruz focused on infants' free exploration. Despite different emphases of learning, both groups of infants benefited from hands-on experience and learned the rule with the single exemplar. The finding points to diverse pathways to support the early development of physical concepts for infants from different cultural backgrounds. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究考察了在婴儿通过亲身体验了解新的物理规则时,父母对婴儿进行指导的文化模式。九个月大的婴儿参加了两个地点的研究:台湾台北和美国加利福尼亚州圣克鲁斯(N = 96;男 48 人,女 48 人)。他们只观看了一个覆盖事件的范例,该范例展示了要学习的规则,但这并不足以让他们获得学习规则的视觉经验。当婴儿在观察时探索物体时,他们的母亲会提供不同文化背景的指导。台北的二人组通过父母手拉手的方式共同进行指令性指导,而圣克鲁斯的二人组则侧重于婴儿的自由探索。尽管学习的侧重点不同,但两组婴儿都从亲身体验中获益,并通过单一范例学会了规则。这一发现为来自不同文化背景的婴儿提供了支持其早期物理概念发展的不同途径。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Parental guidance fosters hands-on learning by infants in culturally different ways.","authors":"Su-Hua Wang","doi":"10.1037/dev0001842","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001842","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present research examined cultural patterns of parental guidance for infants as they learned about a new physical rule with hands-on experience. Nine-month-olds participated in two sites: Taipei, Taiwan and Santa Cruz, California, United States (<i>N</i> = 96; 48 males, 48 females). They watched a single exemplar of covering events that demonstrated the to-be-learned rule, which was insufficient visual experience to learn the rule. As infants explored the objects while observing, their mothers provided culturally distinct guidance. The dyads in Taipei co-enacted directive guidance through parents' hand-holding infants, whereas the dyads in Santa Cruz focused on infants' free exploration. Despite different emphases of learning, both groups of infants benefited from hands-on experience and learned the rule with the single exemplar. The finding points to diverse pathways to support the early development of physical concepts for infants from different cultural backgrounds. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298904","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The dynamic and relational nature of parent-child conflict from childhood into emerging adulthood. 从童年到成年,亲子冲突的动态性和关系性。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001847
S Alexandra Burt, Sarah Carroll, Elizabeth A Shewark, Kelly L Klump, Jenae M Neiderhiser, Luke W Hyde

Although the parent-child relationship is widely regarded as a foundational context for youth development, the developmental origins of this relationship remain unknown. The present study addressed these gaps, leveraging longitudinal and genetically informed methods to illuminate the developmental origins of mother-child conflict as it unfolds from middle childhood into emerging adulthood. Participants consisted of 2,060 twins in 1,030 twin families (51% male, 49% female; 82% White, 10% Black, 1% Asian, 1% Indigenous, 6% multiracial) from the Michigan State University Twin Registry. Families were assessed up to five times. We fitted a series of latent growth curve models (univariate and parallel process) to data from mothers and children, after which we estimated genetic and environmental sources of variance within and covariance among the intercepts and slopes. Parallel process analyses indicated that maternal reports of conflict at baseline shaped their own and their children's perceptions of change in conflict over time but that children's reports of conflict at baseline predicted only their own rate of change in conflict. Subsequent biometric analyses indicated substantial environmental contributions to the intercepts in childhood, as well as prominent environmental origins to the overlap between maternal and child intercepts. By contrast, we observed robust genetically influenced child effects on maternal rate of change and on the association between the maternal and child slopes. Such findings collectively illuminate the dynamic and relational nature of mother-child conflict from childhood into emerging adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管亲子关系被广泛认为是青少年发展的基础,但这种关系的发展起源仍不为人知。本研究针对这些空白,采用纵向和遗传学方法,揭示了母子冲突从童年中期发展到成年期的发展起源。参与者包括密歇根州立大学双胞胎登记处的 1,030 个双胞胎家庭中的 2,060 对双胞胎(51% 为男性,49% 为女性;82% 为白人,10% 为黑人,1% 为亚裔,1% 为土著,6% 为多种族)。这些家庭最多接受了五次评估。我们对母亲和孩子的数据拟合了一系列潜在的生长曲线模型(单变量和平行过程),然后估算了截距和斜率内部的遗传和环境方差来源以及它们之间的协方差。平行过程分析表明,母亲在基线时对冲突的报告影响了她们自己和子女对冲突随时间变化的看法,但子女在基线时对冲突的报告只预测了他们自己的冲突变化率。随后的生物计量分析表明,环境对童年时期的截距有很大影响,母婴截距的重叠也主要源于环境。相比之下,我们观察到受基因影响的儿童对母亲的变化率以及母亲和儿童斜率之间的关联产生了强有力的影响。这些发现共同揭示了从童年到成年期母子冲突的动态和关系性质。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The dynamic and relational nature of parent-child conflict from childhood into emerging adulthood.","authors":"S Alexandra Burt, Sarah Carroll, Elizabeth A Shewark, Kelly L Klump, Jenae M Neiderhiser, Luke W Hyde","doi":"10.1037/dev0001847","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001847","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although the parent-child relationship is widely regarded as a foundational context for youth development, the developmental origins of this relationship remain unknown. The present study addressed these gaps, leveraging longitudinal and genetically informed methods to illuminate the developmental origins of mother-child conflict as it unfolds from middle childhood into emerging adulthood. Participants consisted of 2,060 twins in 1,030 twin families (51% male, 49% female; 82% White, 10% Black, 1% Asian, 1% Indigenous, 6% multiracial) from the Michigan State University Twin Registry. Families were assessed up to five times. We fitted a series of latent growth curve models (univariate and parallel process) to data from mothers and children, after which we estimated genetic and environmental sources of variance within and covariance among the intercepts and slopes. Parallel process analyses indicated that maternal reports of conflict at baseline shaped their own and their children's perceptions of change in conflict over time but that children's reports of conflict at baseline predicted only their own rate of change in conflict. Subsequent biometric analyses indicated substantial environmental contributions to the intercepts in childhood, as well as prominent environmental origins to the overlap between maternal and child intercepts. By contrast, we observed robust genetically influenced child effects on maternal rate of change and on the association between the maternal and child slopes. Such findings collectively illuminate the dynamic and relational nature of mother-child conflict from childhood into emerging adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Not all punishment is equal: The effect of punishment severity on children's social evaluations. 并非所有惩罚都是平等的:惩罚严重程度对儿童社会评价的影响。
IF 3.1 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001845
Young-Eun Lee, Larisa Heiphetz Solomon

Punishment is a key mechanism to regulate selfish behaviors and maintain cooperation in a society. However, children often show mixed evaluations about third-party punishment. The current work asked how punishment severity might shape children's social judgments. In two studies, 5- to 10-year-old children heard about a punisher who took different numbers of items from a transgressor and evaluated the punisher's behavior and moral character. In Study 1 (n = 68), when the transgression was relatively mild (i.e., unfair sharing), children across ages evaluated taking no items from the unfair sharer ("no punishment") most positively, while evaluating taking three items ("harshest punishment") most negatively. In Study 2 (n = 68), when the transgression was more serious (i.e., stealing), younger children evaluated taking two items ("equality-establishing punishment") more positively than older children, while evaluating taking none most negatively. However, children became more likely to evaluate equality-establishing punishment negatively with age. Overall, the current results show that punishment severity is a key factor underlying children's third-party punishment judgments. The current research extends work on moral development by showing how children conceptualize the severity of punishment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

惩罚是调节自私行为和维持社会合作的重要机制。然而,儿童对第三方惩罚的评价往往喜忧参半。目前的研究探讨了惩罚的严厉程度会如何影响儿童的社会判断。在两项研究中,5 到 10 岁的儿童听说惩罚者从违法者身上拿走了不同数量的物品,并对惩罚者的行为和道德品质进行了评价。在研究 1(n = 68)中,当违规行为相对较轻(即不公平分享)时,各年龄段儿童对不公平分享者不拿走任何物品("无惩罚")的评价最为积极,而对拿走三件物品("最严厉的惩罚")的评价最为消极。在研究 2(n = 68)中,当越轨行为更为严重(即偷窃)时,年龄较小的儿童比年龄较大的儿童对拿走两件物品("建立平等的惩罚")的评价更为积极,而对不拿走任何物品的评价则最为消极。然而,随着年龄的增长,儿童更有可能对 "建立平等惩罚 "作出负面评价。总之,目前的研究结果表明,惩罚的严重程度是儿童对第三方惩罚作出判断的一个关键因素。当前的研究通过展示儿童如何看待惩罚的严重程度,扩展了道德发展方面的研究。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Not all punishment is equal: The effect of punishment severity on children's social evaluations.","authors":"Young-Eun Lee, Larisa Heiphetz Solomon","doi":"10.1037/dev0001845","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001845","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Punishment is a key mechanism to regulate selfish behaviors and maintain cooperation in a society. However, children often show mixed evaluations about third-party punishment. The current work asked how punishment severity might shape children's social judgments. In two studies, 5- to 10-year-old children heard about a punisher who took different numbers of items from a transgressor and evaluated the punisher's behavior and moral character. In Study 1 (<i>n</i> = 68), when the transgression was relatively mild (i.e., unfair sharing), children across ages evaluated taking no items from the unfair sharer (\"no punishment\") most positively, while evaluating taking three items (\"harshest punishment\") most negatively. In Study 2 (<i>n</i> = 68), when the transgression was more serious (i.e., stealing), younger children evaluated taking two items (\"equality-establishing punishment\") more positively than older children, while evaluating taking none most negatively. However, children became more likely to evaluate equality-establishing punishment negatively with age. Overall, the current results show that punishment severity is a key factor underlying children's third-party punishment judgments. The current research extends work on moral development by showing how children conceptualize the severity of punishment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298903","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pubertal timing in adolescence and adulthood: Relations among contemporaneous and retrospective measures. 青春期和成年期的青春期时间:同期测量与回顾测量之间的关系。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-09 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001784
Natasha Chaku,Sheri A Berenbaum,Yiming Qian,Robin P Corley,Sally J Wadsworth,Chandra A Reynolds,Adriene M Beltz
Pubertal development has short- and long-term effects on psychological adjustment. Many studies of long-term effects rely on retrospective measurement of pubertal timing, but such measures often reflect different aspects of puberty than those assessed in adolescence, raising questions about the utility and interpretation of retrospective reports. The present study leveraged longitudinal data collected in adolescence and established adulthood to determine: (1) the correspondence between pubertal timing indexed from logistic growth curves of self-reported physical development assessed contemporaneously across adolescence and pubertal timing indexed relative to peers assessed retrospectively in adulthood; (2) the associations between the two pubertal timing measures and psychological adjustment; and (3) potential recall biases. Participants were 748 individuals (50.1% female; 91.6% White) from two longitudinal studies who reported on their pubertal development annually from Grades 3-9, psychological adjustment (age at sexual initiation, substance use, depression) in late adolescence, and retrospective pubertal timing in established adulthood (Mage = 32.76; SD = 4.43). Results indicate moderate-to-high convergence between retrospective and contemporaneous indices. Most participants, especially women, had the same pubertal timing classification (i.e., early, on time, or late), but early-maturing adolescents often recalled on-time development as adults. Retrospective and contemporaneous indices were associated with psychological adjustment in similar ways, with some attenuation in the retrospective measure, especially for men. There was little evidence of recall bias due to age at retrospective assessment or time since puberty. Findings generally support the use of retrospective pubertal timing measures, with the recognition that some relations with adjustment may be weakened. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
青春期发育对心理适应有短期和长期的影响。许多关于长期影响的研究都依赖于对青春期时间的回顾性测量,但这些测量通常反映了青春期的不同方面,而不是青春期的评估,这就对回顾性报告的效用和解释提出了质疑。本研究利用在青春期和成年后收集的纵向数据来确定:(1)根据青春期同时评估的自我报告身体发育的逻辑生长曲线所得出的青春期时间指数与成年后回顾性评估的相对于同龄人的青春期时间指数之间的对应关系;(2)两种青春期时间测量与心理适应之间的关联;以及(3)潜在的回忆偏差。参与者为来自两项纵向研究的 748 人(50.1% 为女性;91.6% 为白人),他们报告了 3-9 年级每年的青春期发育情况、青春期后期的心理适应情况(开始性行为的年龄、药物使用、抑郁)以及成年后的青春期时间回顾(Mage = 32.76;SD = 4.43)。结果表明,回顾性指数和同期指数之间存在中度到高度的趋同性。大多数参与者,尤其是女性,都有相同的青春期时间分类(即早熟、准时或晚熟),但早熟的青少年往往回忆起成年时的准时发育。回顾性指数和同期指数与心理适应的相关性相似,但回顾性指数有所减弱,尤其是男性。几乎没有证据表明回顾性评估时的年龄或青春期后的时间会导致回忆偏差。研究结果总体上支持使用青春期时间回顾测量法,但也认识到与适应的某些关系可能会减弱。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Pubertal timing in adolescence and adulthood: Relations among contemporaneous and retrospective measures.","authors":"Natasha Chaku,Sheri A Berenbaum,Yiming Qian,Robin P Corley,Sally J Wadsworth,Chandra A Reynolds,Adriene M Beltz","doi":"10.1037/dev0001784","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001784","url":null,"abstract":"Pubertal development has short- and long-term effects on psychological adjustment. Many studies of long-term effects rely on retrospective measurement of pubertal timing, but such measures often reflect different aspects of puberty than those assessed in adolescence, raising questions about the utility and interpretation of retrospective reports. The present study leveraged longitudinal data collected in adolescence and established adulthood to determine: (1) the correspondence between pubertal timing indexed from logistic growth curves of self-reported physical development assessed contemporaneously across adolescence and pubertal timing indexed relative to peers assessed retrospectively in adulthood; (2) the associations between the two pubertal timing measures and psychological adjustment; and (3) potential recall biases. Participants were 748 individuals (50.1% female; 91.6% White) from two longitudinal studies who reported on their pubertal development annually from Grades 3-9, psychological adjustment (age at sexual initiation, substance use, depression) in late adolescence, and retrospective pubertal timing in established adulthood (Mage = 32.76; SD = 4.43). Results indicate moderate-to-high convergence between retrospective and contemporaneous indices. Most participants, especially women, had the same pubertal timing classification (i.e., early, on time, or late), but early-maturing adolescents often recalled on-time development as adults. Retrospective and contemporaneous indices were associated with psychological adjustment in similar ways, with some attenuation in the retrospective measure, especially for men. There was little evidence of recall bias due to age at retrospective assessment or time since puberty. Findings generally support the use of retrospective pubertal timing measures, with the recognition that some relations with adjustment may be weakened. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142211900","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Math motivational belief development during high school by race/ethnicity: Teachers and parents as predictors of changes. 按种族/族裔划分的高中数学动机信念发展情况:教师和家长是变化的预测因素。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-09 DOI: 10.1037/dev0001823
Glona Lee-Poon,Jacquelynne S Eccles,Sandra D Simpkins
The changes in adolescents' math motivational beliefs (i.e., expectancies for success, interest, and utility value) across Grades 9-11 and the associations between these changes and adolescents' experiences with socializers (i.e., perceived teacher unfairness and parent-adolescent discussions) were examined within each of the four largest racial/ethnic groups in the United States using the High School Longitudinal Study, a nationally representative data set (n = 19,010; 50% female; 9% Asian; 11% Black; 18% Latine; 62% White; Mage = 14.53 in Grade 9). Cross-tabulation analyses suggested that similar developmental trends emerged within each racial/ethnic group (which were tested separately). Many adolescents maintained their high or low expectancies, interest, and utility values across Grades 9-11. Some patterns varied by belief; for example, several adolescents switched from high to low interest by Grade 11, whereas several adolescents switched from low to high utility value. Parent-adolescent discussions predicted positive changes among Asian and Latine adolescents, whereas perceived teacher unfairness predicted negative changes among Black adolescents. The findings from the present study highlight the diverse developmental trends in adolescents' motivational beliefs and the potential role of socializers as sources of strength or challenge in their motivational belief development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
我们利用具有全国代表性的数据集《高中纵向研究》(High School Longitudinal Study)(n = 19010;50% 女性;9% 亚洲人;11% 黑人;18% 拉丁人;62% 白人;9 年级学生平均年龄 = 14.53),对美国四大种族/民族群体中的每一个群体进行了研究。交叉分析表明,每个种族/族裔群体(分别进行测试)都出现了类似的发展趋向。许多青少年在九年级至十一年级期间保持着或高或低的期望值、兴趣值和效用值。一些模式因信仰而异;例如,一些青少年在十一年级时从高兴趣转为低兴趣,而一些青少年则从低效用值转为高效用值。家长与青少年的讨论预示着亚裔和拉丁裔青少年的积极变化,而认为教师不公平则预示着黑人青少年的消极变化。本研究的结果凸显了青少年动机信念的不同发展趋向,以及社会化者在青少年动机信念发展过程中作为力量来源或挑战来源的潜在作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Math motivational belief development during high school by race/ethnicity: Teachers and parents as predictors of changes.","authors":"Glona Lee-Poon,Jacquelynne S Eccles,Sandra D Simpkins","doi":"10.1037/dev0001823","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001823","url":null,"abstract":"The changes in adolescents' math motivational beliefs (i.e., expectancies for success, interest, and utility value) across Grades 9-11 and the associations between these changes and adolescents' experiences with socializers (i.e., perceived teacher unfairness and parent-adolescent discussions) were examined within each of the four largest racial/ethnic groups in the United States using the High School Longitudinal Study, a nationally representative data set (n = 19,010; 50% female; 9% Asian; 11% Black; 18% Latine; 62% White; Mage = 14.53 in Grade 9). Cross-tabulation analyses suggested that similar developmental trends emerged within each racial/ethnic group (which were tested separately). Many adolescents maintained their high or low expectancies, interest, and utility values across Grades 9-11. Some patterns varied by belief; for example, several adolescents switched from high to low interest by Grade 11, whereas several adolescents switched from low to high utility value. Parent-adolescent discussions predicted positive changes among Asian and Latine adolescents, whereas perceived teacher unfairness predicted negative changes among Black adolescents. The findings from the present study highlight the diverse developmental trends in adolescents' motivational beliefs and the potential role of socializers as sources of strength or challenge in their motivational belief development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48464,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142211906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Developmental Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1