首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Motor Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Limb-Target Control Increases With Effective Index of Difficulty.
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-06 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2025.2461085
Jarrod Blinch, Maryam Nourollahimoghadam, Coby Trovinger, Maria Nida Roncesvalles, Melanie A Hart, Romeo Chua

Researchers have investigated the sensorimotor mechanisms that result in Fitts' law. One approach has been to analyse movement trajectories during Fitts' tasks to reveal the processes that occur during movement preparation and execution. We used trajectory analysis in the current study to investigate how limb-target control contributed to Fitts' law during the transition from ballistic movements to movements with online control. Twenty-five participants made discrete reaching movements in seven conditions with indexes of difficulty that ranged from one to seven. There were strong linear relationships between index of difficulty, effective index of difficulty and movement time. Trajectory analysis suggested that the easiest condition had movements that were mostly ballistic. There was enough time for visual-based online corrections, but the condition was probably too easy to require limb-target control. Trajectory analysis also suggested that there was an increased reliance on limb-target control as the index of difficulty increased. In conclusion, there was a strong linear relationship between effective index of difficulty and movement time even with conditions that ranged from mostly ballistic to a high degree of limb-target control. We suggest that there is a direct relationship between effective index of difficulty and degree of limb-target control.

{"title":"Limb-Target Control Increases With Effective Index of Difficulty.","authors":"Jarrod Blinch, Maryam Nourollahimoghadam, Coby Trovinger, Maria Nida Roncesvalles, Melanie A Hart, Romeo Chua","doi":"10.1080/00222895.2025.2461085","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2025.2461085","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Researchers have investigated the sensorimotor mechanisms that result in Fitts' law. One approach has been to analyse movement trajectories during Fitts' tasks to reveal the processes that occur during movement preparation and execution. We used trajectory analysis in the current study to investigate how limb-target control contributed to Fitts' law during the transition from ballistic movements to movements with online control. Twenty-five participants made discrete reaching movements in seven conditions with indexes of difficulty that ranged from one to seven. There were strong linear relationships between index of difficulty, effective index of difficulty and movement time. Trajectory analysis suggested that the easiest condition had movements that were mostly ballistic. There was enough time for visual-based online corrections, but the condition was probably too easy to require limb-target control. Trajectory analysis also suggested that there was an increased reliance on limb-target control as the index of difficulty increased. In conclusion, there was a strong linear relationship between effective index of difficulty and movement time even with conditions that ranged from mostly ballistic to a high degree of limb-target control. We suggest that there is a direct relationship between effective index of difficulty and degree of limb-target control.</p>","PeriodicalId":50125,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Motor Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"1-14"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143256896","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Control Mechanisms of Sensorimotor System on Manipulation of Proprioceptive Inputs During Balance Maintenance.
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-30 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2025.2458503
Nasim Shokri, Kohyar Yazdanpanah, Mohammed N Ashtiani

Proprioceptive inputs have crucial roles in control of the posture. The aim of the present study was to assess the effect of interfering with these signals on postural stability by ice-induced anaesthesia and local calf muscle fatigue. Seventeen healthy young individuals participated in this study to stand quietly and on an unstable platform under normal, anaesthesia, and fatigue conditions. A force platform calculated excursions of centre of pressure. Stabilogram-diffusion analysis was utilised to evaluate how body controls the posture with and without proprioceptive inputs. Time intervals of using the sensory feedback is significantly increased by anaesthesia in quiet standing (430 ms, p = 0.034) to note more delayed use of sensory information in a closed-loop. Additionally, fatigue significantly increased the time intervals of using sensory feedback during standing on the unstable platform (290 ms, p = 0.016). Interestingly, sensory interventions had no effect on the stability of the open-loop control of posture (short-term control), but they significantly influenced the closed-loop control (long-term control) (p < 0.004). Specifically, fatigue led to increased instabilities when the body used sensory inputs during both quiet standing (p = 0.021) and standing on the unstable platform (p = 0.041). These findings highlight the importance of proprioception in balance control for healthy individuals. Interfering with proprioceptive inputs, either through anaesthesia or fatigue, resulted in instabilities during balance maintenance. Our study provides new insights into the mechanisms underlying postural control, emphasising the significance of proprioceptive inputs. Understanding how proprioception affects balance maintenance may have implications for rehabilitation strategies, injury prevention, and the development of interventions to improve postural stability.

{"title":"Control Mechanisms of Sensorimotor System on Manipulation of Proprioceptive Inputs During Balance Maintenance.","authors":"Nasim Shokri, Kohyar Yazdanpanah, Mohammed N Ashtiani","doi":"10.1080/00222895.2025.2458503","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2025.2458503","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Proprioceptive inputs have crucial roles in control of the posture. The aim of the present study was to assess the effect of interfering with these signals on postural stability by ice-induced anaesthesia and local calf muscle fatigue. Seventeen healthy young individuals participated in this study to stand quietly and on an unstable platform under normal, anaesthesia, and fatigue conditions. A force platform calculated excursions of centre of pressure. Stabilogram-diffusion analysis was utilised to evaluate how body controls the posture with and without proprioceptive inputs. Time intervals of using the sensory feedback is significantly increased by anaesthesia in quiet standing (430 ms, <i>p</i> = 0.034) to note more delayed use of sensory information in a closed-loop. Additionally, fatigue significantly increased the time intervals of using sensory feedback during standing on the unstable platform (290 ms, <i>p</i> = 0.016). Interestingly, sensory interventions had no effect on the stability of the open-loop control of posture (short-term control), but they significantly influenced the closed-loop control (long-term control) (<i>p</i> < 0.004). Specifically, fatigue led to increased instabilities when the body used sensory inputs during both quiet standing (<i>p</i> = 0.021) and standing on the unstable platform (<i>p</i> = 0.041). These findings highlight the importance of proprioception in balance control for healthy individuals. Interfering with proprioceptive inputs, either through anaesthesia or fatigue, resulted in instabilities during balance maintenance. Our study provides new insights into the mechanisms underlying postural control, emphasising the significance of proprioceptive inputs. Understanding how proprioception affects balance maintenance may have implications for rehabilitation strategies, injury prevention, and the development of interventions to improve postural stability.</p>","PeriodicalId":50125,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Motor Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"1-9"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143068661","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Guiding the Hand to an Invisible Target.
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-22 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2025.2455179
Marcin Furtak, Eli Brenner

Numerous devices are being developed to assist visually impaired and blind individuals in performing everyday tasks such as reaching out to grasp objects. Considering that the size, weight, and cost of assistive devices significantly impact their acceptance, it would be useful to know how effective various types of guiding information can be. As an initial exploration of this issue, we conducted four studies in which participants with normal vision were visually guided toward targets. They were guided by information about the direction to the target, and either about the distance to the target or about the time required to reach the target. We compared participants' performance when provided with different amounts of each of these kinds of information. We found that restricting information about the distance from the target or the time it would take to reach the target to only a few possible values does not affect performance substantially. Restricting information about the direction to the target to only a few possible values appears to be more detrimental, but the disadvantage of having few possible directions can be mitigated by combining values in multiple directions. These findings can help optimize haptic presentations in assistive technology.

{"title":"Guiding the Hand to an Invisible Target.","authors":"Marcin Furtak, Eli Brenner","doi":"10.1080/00222895.2025.2455179","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2025.2455179","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Numerous devices are being developed to assist visually impaired and blind individuals in performing everyday tasks such as reaching out to grasp objects. Considering that the size, weight, and cost of assistive devices significantly impact their acceptance, it would be useful to know how effective various types of guiding information can be. As an initial exploration of this issue, we conducted four studies in which participants with normal vision were visually guided toward targets. They were guided by information about the direction to the target, and either about the distance to the target or about the time required to reach the target. We compared participants' performance when provided with different amounts of each of these kinds of information. We found that restricting information about the distance from the target or the time it would take to reach the target to only a few possible values does not affect performance substantially. Restricting information about the direction to the target to only a few possible values appears to be more detrimental, but the disadvantage of having few possible directions can be mitigated by combining values in multiple directions. These findings can help optimize haptic presentations in assistive technology.</p>","PeriodicalId":50125,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Motor Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"1-9"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143025469","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Auditory Cues and Feedback in the Serial Reaction Time Task: Evidence for Sequence Acquisition and Sensory Transfer. 序列反应时间任务中的听觉线索和反馈:序列获取和感觉转移的证据。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-19 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2024.2448130
Elena M Broeckelmann, Toby Martin, Cheryl M Glazebrook

The present experiment used the Serial Reaction Time Task(SRTT) to investigate if auditory cueing or feedback in the form of spatially compatible tones benefited sequence learning similarly. Fifty-three neurotypical adults (18-35 years; 32 cis-females; 21 cis-males) were randomly assigned to three groups in which they practiced a visual SRTT: Group AC was supplemented with auditory cues; group AF received auditory feedback; group NS performed without sound. Retention and transfer tests (i.e., in the other two sensory conditions), and an explicit awareness test were conducted 48 h after practice. Changes in Total Sequence Time (TST), Total Error (TE), and acquired knowledge of the 10-item sequence order quantified sequence learning and were assessed using a two-way mixed ANOVA with repeated measures (p ≤ 0.05). A significant group-by-time interaction indicated only the AC group maintained their performance improvements when the sequence was perturbed. Overall, improvements in TST on day 1 and day 2 were consistent with all groups acquiring task-general and sequence-specific knowledge. TE outcomes suggested no speed-accuracy tradeoff. On Day 2, all groups performed best in the no-sound condition, indicating performance was maintained when sound cues or feedback were removed. All groups acquired equivalent implicit motor sequence knowledge regardless of sound condition.

本实验采用序列反应时间任务(SRTT)来研究听觉提示或空间相容音调形式的反馈是否同样有利于序列学习。53名典型神经成年人(18-35岁;32 cis-females;21名顺式男性被随机分为三组,每组进行视觉SRTT练习:AC组辅以听觉提示;AF组接受听觉反馈;NS组无声音表演。练习后48小时进行保留和转移测试(即在其他两种感觉条件下)和外显意识测试。总序列时间(TST)、总误差(TE)和获得的10项序列顺序知识的变化量化了序列学习,并采用重复测量的双向混合方差分析(p≤0.05)进行评估。显著的组-时间交互作用表明,当序列受到干扰时,只有AC组保持了他们的表现改善。总体而言,第1天和第2天的TST改善与所有组获得任务一般性和序列特异性知识一致。TE结果显示没有速度和准确性的权衡。在第2天,所有组在无声音条件下表现最好,这表明当声音提示或反馈被移除时,表现仍保持不变。无论健康状况如何,所有组都获得了相同的隐性动作序列知识。
{"title":"Auditory Cues and Feedback in the Serial Reaction Time Task: Evidence for Sequence Acquisition and Sensory Transfer.","authors":"Elena M Broeckelmann, Toby Martin, Cheryl M Glazebrook","doi":"10.1080/00222895.2024.2448130","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2024.2448130","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present experiment used the Serial Reaction Time Task(SRTT) to investigate if auditory cueing or feedback in the form of spatially compatible tones benefited sequence learning similarly. Fifty-three neurotypical adults (18-35 years; 32 cis-females; 21 cis-males) were randomly assigned to three groups in which they practiced a visual SRTT: Group AC was supplemented with auditory cues; group AF received auditory feedback; group NS performed without sound. Retention and transfer tests (i.e., in the other two sensory conditions), and an explicit awareness test were conducted 48 h after practice. Changes in Total Sequence Time (TST), Total Error (TE), and acquired knowledge of the 10-item sequence order quantified sequence learning and were assessed using a two-way mixed ANOVA with repeated measures (<i>p</i> ≤ 0.05). A significant group-by-time interaction indicated only the AC group maintained their performance improvements when the sequence was perturbed. Overall, improvements in TST on day 1 and day 2 were consistent with all groups acquiring task-general and sequence-specific knowledge. TE outcomes suggested no speed-accuracy tradeoff. On Day 2, all groups performed best in the no-sound condition, indicating performance was maintained when sound cues or feedback were removed. All groups acquired equivalent implicit motor sequence knowledge regardless of sound condition.</p>","PeriodicalId":50125,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Motor Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"1-16"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143015332","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of Constraining Postural Sway During Upper-Limb Precision Aiming Task Practice in Individuals with Stroke. 在脑卒中患者进行上肢精确瞄准任务练习时限制姿势摇摆的影响
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-18 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2024.2416232
Sarah M Schwab-Farrell, Riley Mayr, Tehran J Davis, Michael A Riley, Paula L Silva

Individuals post-stroke commonly demonstrate alterations in motor behavior with regard to both task performance and the motor strategies used in pursuit of task goals. We evaluated whether constraining postural sway (motor strategy) during practice would affect upper-limb precision aiming performance (task performance) and postural control adaptations. Adults with stroke stood on a force plate while immersed in a virtual scene displaying an anterior target. Participants aimed to position a virtual laser pointer (via handheld device) in the target. Participants then completed practice trials involving aiming at a lateral target. For this practice session, participants were randomized to either (a) a "constraint" group wherein they received physical constraint to limit postural sway, or (b) a "no-constraint" group. Task performance and postural control were assessed before and after practice, and transfer to another upper-limb task was evaluated. After practice, both groups improved paretic upper-limb performance. For the target task, the no-constraint group showed task-sensitive changes in postural control. The constraint group showed no changes in postural control. At transfer, the constraint group increased postural sway. Constraining postural sway after stroke should be carefully considered with the recognition that postural sway arises from exploratory movements involved in the discovery of adaptable motor solutions.

中风后患者的运动行为通常会在任务表现和追求任务目标时所使用的运动策略两方面发生改变。我们评估了在练习过程中限制姿势摇摆(运动策略)是否会影响上肢精确瞄准性能(任务性能)和姿势控制适应性。患有中风的成年人站在受力板上,同时沉浸在显示前方目标的虚拟场景中。参与者瞄准目标定位虚拟激光笔(通过手持设备)。然后,参与者完成瞄准侧面目标的练习试验。在这一练习环节中,参与者被随机分配到(a)"约束 "组或(b)"无约束 "组,前者接受物理约束以限制姿势摇摆。在练习前后对任务表现和姿势控制进行了评估,并对转移到另一项上肢任务的情况进行了评估。练习后,两组瘫痪者的上肢表现都有所改善。在目标任务中,无限制组的姿势控制出现了任务敏感性变化。约束组的姿势控制没有变化。在转移时,约束组增加了姿势摇摆。中风后姿势摇摆的约束应谨慎考虑,因为姿势摇摆源于探索性运动,涉及发现适应性运动解决方案。
{"title":"Effects of Constraining Postural Sway During Upper-Limb Precision Aiming Task Practice in Individuals with Stroke.","authors":"Sarah M Schwab-Farrell, Riley Mayr, Tehran J Davis, Michael A Riley, Paula L Silva","doi":"10.1080/00222895.2024.2416232","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00222895.2024.2416232","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Individuals post-stroke commonly demonstrate alterations in motor behavior with regard to both task performance and the motor strategies used in pursuit of task goals. We evaluated whether constraining postural sway (motor strategy) during practice would affect upper-limb precision aiming performance (task performance) and postural control adaptations. Adults with stroke stood on a force plate while immersed in a virtual scene displaying an anterior target. Participants aimed to position a virtual laser pointer (via handheld device) in the target. Participants then completed practice trials involving aiming at a lateral target. For this practice session, participants were randomized to either (a) a \"constraint\" group wherein they received physical constraint to limit postural sway, or (b) a \"no-constraint\" group. Task performance and postural control were assessed before and after practice, and transfer to another upper-limb task was evaluated. After practice, both groups improved paretic upper-limb performance. For the target task, the no-constraint group showed task-sensitive changes in postural control. The constraint group showed no changes in postural control. At transfer, the constraint group increased postural sway. Constraining postural sway after stroke should be carefully considered with the recognition that postural sway arises from exploratory movements involved in the discovery of adaptable motor solutions.</p>","PeriodicalId":50125,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Motor Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"61-76"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142479310","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of External Pacing Type on the Cross-Education of Motor Skill. 外部起搏类型对运动技能交叉教育的影响
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-19 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2024.2390032
Justin W Andrushko, Dakota T Zirk, Aryan R Kurniawan, Doug W Renshaw, Jonathan P Farthing

Cross-education (CE) is a phenomenon whereby motor training of one limb leads to improved performance in the opposite untrained limb. External pacing of a motor task can enhance CE; however, the influence of different pacing methods is poorly understood. This study explored how motor training with auditory (AP) and visual pacing (VP) impacts CE with a visuomotor force target task. Sixty-one participants performed a unimanual motor task. Participants were randomized into a visual (n = 31) or auditory (n = 30) pacing stimuli condition. The primary outcome was cumulative error scores for each hand, before and after visuomotor training. Pacing type did not yield different magnitudes of CE. However, after adjusting for baseline differences, a significant hand (trained vs. untrained) × practice side (dominant or non-dominant) interaction (p = .013, ηp2 = .106) and a group main effect (p = .036, ηp2 = .165) were observed. Visual pacing resulted in greater improvements in task performance compared to auditory pacing regardless of hand or practice side, while training the dominant limb resulting in a greater interlimb asymmetry regardless of pacing stimulus. These findings have implications for applying pacing strategies during rehabilitation from unilateral injury or neurological impairment.

交叉教育(Cross-education,CE)是指对一个肢体进行运动训练后,未接受训练的对侧肢体的运动能力得到提高的现象。运动任务的外部起搏可以增强交叉教育;然而,人们对不同起搏方法的影响知之甚少。本研究探讨了听觉起搏(AP)和视觉起搏(VP)的运动训练如何影响视觉运动力目标任务的CE。61 名参与者完成了一项单指运动任务。参与者被随机分配到视觉(31 人)或听觉(30 人)起搏刺激条件下。主要结果是视觉运动训练前后每只手的累积错误分数。起搏类型不会产生不同的 CE 值。然而,在调整基线差异后,观察到了显著的手(训练过与未训练过)×练习侧(优势侧或非优势侧)交互作用(p = .013,ηp2 = .106)和组主效应(p = .036,ηp2 = .165)。与听觉起搏相比,无论手或练习侧如何,视觉起搏都能更大程度地提高任务表现,而无论起搏刺激如何,训练优势肢体都会导致更大的肢体间不对称。这些发现对在单侧损伤或神经损伤的康复过程中应用起搏策略具有重要意义。
{"title":"Effects of External Pacing Type on the Cross-Education of Motor Skill.","authors":"Justin W Andrushko, Dakota T Zirk, Aryan R Kurniawan, Doug W Renshaw, Jonathan P Farthing","doi":"10.1080/00222895.2024.2390032","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00222895.2024.2390032","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Cross-education (CE) is a phenomenon whereby motor training of one limb leads to improved performance in the opposite untrained limb. External pacing of a motor task can enhance CE; however, the influence of different pacing methods is poorly understood. This study explored how motor training with auditory (AP) and visual pacing (VP) impacts CE with a visuomotor force target task. Sixty-one participants performed a unimanual motor task. Participants were randomized into a visual (<i>n</i> = 31) or auditory (<i>n</i> = 30) pacing stimuli condition. The primary outcome was cumulative error scores for each hand, before and after visuomotor training. Pacing type did not yield different magnitudes of CE. However, after adjusting for baseline differences, a significant hand (trained vs. untrained) × practice side (dominant or non-dominant) interaction (<i>p</i> = .013, <i>η</i><sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> = .106) and a group main effect (<i>p</i> = .036, <i>η</i><sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> = .165) were observed. Visual pacing resulted in greater improvements in task performance compared to auditory pacing regardless of hand or practice side, while training the dominant limb resulting in a greater interlimb asymmetry regardless of pacing stimulus. These findings have implications for applying pacing strategies during rehabilitation from unilateral injury or neurological impairment.</p>","PeriodicalId":50125,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Motor Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"1-11"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142001172","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining the Equivalence Between Imagery and Execution-Does Imagery Comprise the Intended Spatial Trajectory? 检验图像与执行之间的等效性--图像是否包含预期的空间轨迹?
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-27 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2024.2406925
James W Roberts, Caroline J Wakefield, John P de Grosbois

The functional equivalence model suggests a common internal representation initiates both imagery and execution. This suggestion is supported by the mental chronometry effect, where there is a positive relation between task difficulty (as defined by the Index of Difficulty; ID) and imagined movement time. The present study extends this logic by examining whether imagery captures the spatial trajectory. Participants were initially tasked with the imagery and execution of a rapid aiming movement under different IDs. These initial attempts were adapted to configure auditory tones at early (25%) and late (75%) intervals for a separate set of imagery trials. If a tone had sounded, participants had to estimate post-trial where their imagined limb would have been located. The findings revealed increases in ID that coincided with increases in imagined and executed movement times. However, participant mean and standard deviation of estimated locations revealed limited differences between the early and late tones. Further inspection revealed some evidence for these estimated locations shifting further along in space following more rapid imagined movements. While equivalence is clearly evident within the temporal domain, there is comparatively little to suggest that this logic extends to the resolution required for simulating the spatial characteristics of movement.

功能等效模型表明,一个共同的内部表征同时启动了想象和执行。心理计时效应支持了这一观点,即任务难度(由难度指数定义)与想象运动时间之间存在正相关。本研究对这一逻辑进行了扩展,考察了想象是否能捕捉空间轨迹。最初,受试者需要在不同的难度指数下想象并执行快速瞄准动作。这些初步尝试经过调整后,在一组单独的想象试验中,分别在早期(25%)和晚期(75%)间隔配置了听觉音调。如果音调响起,参与者必须在试验后估计其想象中的肢体的位置。研究结果表明,ID 的增加与想象和执行动作时间的增加相吻合。然而,参与者估计位置的平均值和标准偏差显示,早期和晚期音调之间的差异有限。进一步检查发现,有证据表明,在更快的想象运动之后,这些估计位置在空间中会进一步移动。虽然在时间领域中,等效性是显而易见的,但这种逻辑延伸到模拟运动空间特征所需的分辨率的证据却相对较少。
{"title":"Examining the Equivalence Between Imagery and Execution-Does Imagery Comprise the Intended Spatial Trajectory?","authors":"James W Roberts, Caroline J Wakefield, John P de Grosbois","doi":"10.1080/00222895.2024.2406925","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00222895.2024.2406925","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The functional equivalence model suggests a common internal representation initiates both imagery and execution. This suggestion is supported by the mental chronometry effect, where there is a positive relation between task difficulty (as defined by the Index of Difficulty; ID) and imagined movement time. The present study extends this logic by examining whether imagery captures the spatial trajectory. Participants were initially tasked with the imagery and execution of a rapid aiming movement under different IDs. These initial attempts were adapted to configure auditory tones at early (25%) and late (75%) intervals for a separate set of imagery trials. If a tone had sounded, participants had to estimate post-trial where their imagined limb would have been located. The findings revealed increases in ID that coincided with increases in imagined and executed movement times. However, participant mean and standard deviation of estimated locations revealed limited differences between the early and late tones. Further inspection revealed some evidence for these estimated locations shifting further along in space following more rapid imagined movements. While equivalence is clearly evident within the temporal domain, there is comparatively little to suggest that this logic extends to the resolution required for simulating the spatial characteristics of movement.</p>","PeriodicalId":50125,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Motor Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"31-42"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142331455","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Increasing Movement Amplitude in Speeded Hitting Enhances Contact Velocity Without Affecting Directional Accuracy or Movement Variability. 在加速击球中增加运动幅度可提高接触速度,而不会影响方向准确性或运动变异性。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-14 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2024.2411995
Victor Hugo Alves Okazaki, Luis Augusto Teixeira

Performance of sport-related ballistic motor skills, like ball hitting in golf and baseball, requires wide movements to produce highly fast and spatially accurate movements. In this study, we assessed the effect of movement amplitude on directional accuracy in a ballistic hitting task. Participants performed the task of moving a manual handle over a flat surface to hit with high speed a moveable disc, aiming to propel it towards a frontal target. Five movement amplitudes were compared, ranging from 11.5 cm to 27.5 cm in steps of 4 cm. Kinematic analysis evaluated motions of the handle, disc, and arm joints. Results showed that greater movement amplitudes led to longer acceleration phases, with delayed peak velocities at the handle, shoulder and elbow, leading to higher contact and peak linear velocities of the handle, and higher angular velocities at the shoulder and elbow. Manipulation of movement amplitude led to no evidence for effects on either disc directional accuracy or variability. Results also revealed no evidence for differences in variability of contact velocity, peak velocity and time of peak velocity across movement amplitudes in the shoulder, elbow, and wrist. Our results indicated that greater movement amplitudes in hitting a spatial target lead to increased contact velocity while not affecting directional accuracy or movement variability.

与运动相关的弹道运动技能,如高尔夫球和棒球中的击球,需要大范围的运动来产生高度快速和空间精确的动作。在本研究中,我们评估了运动幅度对弹道击球任务中方向准确性的影响。受试者在一个平面上移动手动手柄,高速击打一个可移动的圆盘,目的是将其推向一个正面目标。比较了五种运动幅度,从 11.5 厘米到 27.5 厘米不等,步幅为 4 厘米。运动学分析评估了手柄、圆盘和手臂关节的运动。结果显示,运动幅度越大,加速阶段越长,手柄、肩部和肘部的峰值速度越晚,导致手柄的接触速度和峰值线速度越高,肩部和肘部的角速度越高。对运动幅度的控制没有证据表明会对圆盘方向的准确性或可变性产生影响。结果还显示,没有证据表明肩部、肘部和腕部不同运动幅度的接触速度、峰值速度和峰值速度时间的可变性存在差异。我们的结果表明,在击打空间目标时,较大的运动幅度会导致接触速度增加,但不会影响方向准确性或运动变异性。
{"title":"Increasing Movement Amplitude in Speeded Hitting Enhances Contact Velocity Without Affecting Directional Accuracy or Movement Variability.","authors":"Victor Hugo Alves Okazaki, Luis Augusto Teixeira","doi":"10.1080/00222895.2024.2411995","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00222895.2024.2411995","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Performance of sport-related ballistic motor skills, like ball hitting in golf and baseball, requires wide movements to produce highly fast and spatially accurate movements. In this study, we assessed the effect of movement amplitude on directional accuracy in a ballistic hitting task. Participants performed the task of moving a manual handle over a flat surface to hit with high speed a moveable disc, aiming to propel it towards a frontal target. Five movement amplitudes were compared, ranging from 11.5 cm to 27.5 cm in steps of 4 cm. Kinematic analysis evaluated motions of the handle, disc, and arm joints. Results showed that greater movement amplitudes led to longer acceleration phases, with delayed peak velocities at the handle, shoulder and elbow, leading to higher contact and peak linear velocities of the handle, and higher angular velocities at the shoulder and elbow. Manipulation of movement amplitude led to no evidence for effects on either disc directional accuracy or variability. Results also revealed no evidence for differences in variability of contact velocity, peak velocity and time of peak velocity across movement amplitudes in the shoulder, elbow, and wrist. Our results indicated that greater movement amplitudes in hitting a spatial target lead to increased contact velocity while not affecting directional accuracy or movement variability.</p>","PeriodicalId":50125,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Motor Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"43-53"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142479311","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Effects of Constraints on the Variability of Throwing Patterns in Young Children. 限制因素对幼儿投掷模式多变性的影响
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-24 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2024.2416242
Hamid Salehi, Parisa Kalantari

This research examined how changes in task constraints impacted the throwing patterns of children. The study involved 24 children, with an equal number of males and females, aged 5 and 6. The primary task constraints were the orientation of the target (horizontal or vertical hoops) and the size of the ball (diameters of 6 cm or 12 cm). We observed throwing patterns and analyzed kinematic changes in the preferred throws' components. Initially, some children transitioned from using two hands to using one hand, and from underhand to overarm throws, particularly when using the larger balls. However, the preferred pattern for most children was one-hand overarm throwing. The kinematic analysis revealed that the participants adapted their throwing technique based on the size of the ball and the orientation of the hoop. The most significant adjustments occurred in the forearm component in response to changes in the target orientation. Notably, when aiming for a vertical hoop, distinct modifications were observed, including elevating the humerus and pulling the hand backward. These findings support the dynamical systems theory, which explains how movement patterns vary during motor development. The study also discussed the potential benefits of using constraints for skill acquisition in physical education settings.

这项研究探讨了任务限制的变化如何影响儿童的投掷模式。研究涉及 24 名 5 岁和 6 岁的儿童,男女儿童人数相等。主要的任务限制是目标的方向(水平或垂直环)和球的大小(直径为 6 厘米或 12 厘米)。我们观察了投掷模式,并分析了首选投掷部分的运动学变化。最初,一些儿童从双手投掷过渡到单手投掷,从暗手投掷过渡到上臂投掷,尤其是在使用较大的球时。然而,大多数儿童的首选模式是单手投掷。运动学分析表明,参与者会根据球的大小和篮圈的方向调整投掷技巧。最重要的调整发生在前臂部分,以应对目标方向的变化。值得注意的是,当瞄准一个垂直的篮圈时,观察到了明显的调整,包括抬高肱骨和向后拉手。这些发现支持动态系统理论,该理论解释了运动模式在运动发展过程中如何变化。该研究还讨论了在体育教育环境中使用约束条件进行技能学习的潜在益处。
{"title":"The Effects of Constraints on the Variability of Throwing Patterns in Young Children.","authors":"Hamid Salehi, Parisa Kalantari","doi":"10.1080/00222895.2024.2416242","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00222895.2024.2416242","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This research examined how changes in task constraints impacted the throwing patterns of children. The study involved 24 children, with an equal number of males and females, aged 5 and 6. The primary task constraints were the orientation of the target (horizontal or vertical hoops) and the size of the ball (diameters of 6 cm or 12 cm). We observed throwing patterns and analyzed kinematic changes in the preferred throws' components. Initially, some children transitioned from using two hands to using one hand, and from underhand to overarm throws, particularly when using the larger balls. However, the preferred pattern for most children was one-hand overarm throwing. The kinematic analysis revealed that the participants adapted their throwing technique based on the size of the ball and the orientation of the hoop. The most significant adjustments occurred in the forearm component in response to changes in the target orientation. Notably, when aiming for a vertical hoop, distinct modifications were observed, including elevating the humerus and pulling the hand backward. These findings support the dynamical systems theory, which explains how movement patterns vary during motor development. The study also discussed the potential benefits of using constraints for skill acquisition in physical education settings.</p>","PeriodicalId":50125,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Motor Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"87-95"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142512116","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Smartphone Proprioception for Ankle Navigation (SPAN): Reliability and Effect of Position Exposure Time. 用于踝关节导航的智能手机直觉(SPAN):可靠性和位置暴露时间的影响。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-03 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2024.2416231
Xiaojian Shi, Charlotte Ganderton, Roger Adams, Jia Han, Doa El-Ansary, Oren Tirosh

This study investigated ankle discriminative acuity and performance and measurement consistency for tests undertaken with different joint position exposure times (PETs). Twenty-four participants were tested using a novel Smartphone Proprioception for Ankle Navigation (SPAN) under four PETs, i.e., 0.25s, 0.5s, 0.75s and 1s, delivered in a random sequence, and then re-tested within one week. The results indicated a PET main effect (F = 10.12, p = 0.004, partial ƞ2 = 0.14), and limb preference main effect (F = 5.39, p = 0.03, partial ƞ2 = 0.19), without significant interactions (p > 0.05). Ankle proprioception improved with prolonged PET, with the non-dominant side outperforming the dominant side. A PET of 0.25s showed good to excellent reliability, with intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) of 0.897 (95%CI: 0.761, 0.955) and 0.885 (95%CI: 0.736, 0.951), with standard errors of measurements (SEM) between 0.030 and 0.035, and minimum detectable change at 90% (MDC90) between 0.070 and 0.082, compared to poor to moderate reliability at the other three longer PETs (ICCs =0.352-0.736). The findings suggested the prolongation of PET can improve ankle proprioceptive performance but can amplify the inter-occasion variability, likely due to increased cognitive analysis with longer stimulus sampling. SPAN may thus be a cost-effective and accessible apparatus for clinical practice.

本研究调查了在不同关节位置暴露时间(PET)下进行测试时踝关节的辨别敏锐度、性能和测量一致性。24 名参与者使用新型智能手机踝关节感知导航(SPAN)进行了测试,测试采用四种 PET,即 0.25 秒、0.5 秒、0.75 秒和 1 秒,以随机顺序进行,然后在一周内进行复测。结果显示 PET 主效应(F = 10.12,p = 0.004,部分ƞ2 = 0.14)和肢体偏好主效应(F = 5.39,p = 0.03,部分ƞ2 = 0.19),没有显著的交互作用(p > 0.05)。随着 PET 时间的延长,踝关节本体感觉有所改善,非优势侧优于优势侧。0.25 秒的 PET 显示出良好至卓越的可靠性,类内相关系数 (ICC) 分别为 0.897(95%CI:0.761,0.955)和 0.885(95%CI:0.736,0.951),测量标准误差 (SEM) 介于 0.030 和 0.035 之间,测量误差 (SEM) 介于 0.030 和 0.035 之间。相比之下,其他三个较长 PET 的可靠性为差到中等(ICC =0.352-0.736)。研究结果表明,延长 PET 时间可以提高踝关节本体感觉性能,但会扩大事件间的变异性,这可能是由于刺激取样时间延长导致认知分析增加所致。因此,SPAN 可能是临床实践中一种具有成本效益且易于使用的仪器。
{"title":"Smartphone Proprioception for Ankle Navigation (SPAN): Reliability and Effect of Position Exposure Time.","authors":"Xiaojian Shi, Charlotte Ganderton, Roger Adams, Jia Han, Doa El-Ansary, Oren Tirosh","doi":"10.1080/00222895.2024.2416231","DOIUrl":"10.1080/00222895.2024.2416231","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study investigated ankle discriminative acuity and performance and measurement consistency for tests undertaken with different joint position exposure times (PETs). Twenty-four participants were tested using a novel Smartphone Proprioception for Ankle Navigation (SPAN) under four PETs, i.e., 0.25s, 0.5s, 0.75s and 1s, delivered in a random sequence, and then re-tested within one week. The results indicated a PET main effect (<i>F</i> = 10.12, <i>p</i> = 0.004, partial ƞ2 = 0.14), and limb preference main effect (<i>F</i> = 5.39, <i>p</i> = 0.03, partial ƞ2 = 0.19), without significant interactions (<i>p</i> > 0.05). Ankle proprioception improved with prolonged PET, with the non-dominant side outperforming the dominant side. A PET of 0.25s showed good to excellent reliability, with intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) of 0.897 (95%CI: 0.761, 0.955) and 0.885 (95%CI: 0.736, 0.951), with standard errors of measurements (SEM) between 0.030 and 0.035, and minimum detectable change at 90% (MDC<sub>90</sub>) between 0.070 and 0.082, compared to poor to moderate reliability at the other three longer PETs (ICCs =0.352-0.736). The findings suggested the prolongation of PET can improve ankle proprioceptive performance but can amplify the inter-occasion variability, likely due to increased cognitive analysis with longer stimulus sampling. SPAN may thus be a cost-effective and accessible apparatus for clinical practice.</p>","PeriodicalId":50125,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Motor Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"54-60"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142569810","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Motor Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1