首页 > 最新文献

Syntax最新文献

英文 中文
The syntax of Greek split reciprocals 希腊文拆分倒数的句法
Pub Date : 2024-06-11 DOI: 10.1111/synt.12289
Lefteris Paparounas, Martin Salzmann
We provide the first detailed description and analysis of the syntax of the understudied Greek split reciprocal reconstruction. As in other languages, the reciprocal appears to be bipartite consisting of a quantificational distributor (‘the one’) and a reciprocator (‘the other’). We show that, in Greek, this bipartiteness runs deep: the two parts are syntactically independent, with the reciprocator having the syntax of a Condition A anaphor, and the distributor behaving as a floating quantifier. Once we turn to how these elements establish relations between themselves and their antecedent, we find that Greek reciprocals resist a movement‐ or Agree‐based analysis, since both elements can occur in positions inaccessible to movement/Agree. Given that the reciprocator can occur in embedded subject position, the Greek data also argue against recent attempts to reduce the binding domain to phases, instead supporting a more traditional definition of the binding domain in terms of the smallest XP containing the anaphor and a subject. Finally, we show that the morphosyntactic properties of the bipartite construction can be connected to independent properties of its two component parts and that these can, in turn, be related to interpretive aspects of reciprocity.
我们首次详细描述和分析了未被充分研究的希腊语分裂互斥重构句法。与其他语言一样,倒数词似乎是由一个量词分词('the one')和一个倒数词('the other')两部分组成的。我们的研究表明,在希腊语中,这种二元性是深层次的:这两个部分在句法上是独立的,倒数者的句法是条件 A 的拟声词,而分配者则是浮动量词。一旦我们转而研究这些元素如何在它们自己和它们的先行词之间建立关系,我们就会发现希腊语的往复式抵制基于动作或同意的分析,因为这两个元素都可能出现在动作/同意无法触及的位置。鉴于互易成分可以出现在嵌入主语的位置,希腊语数据也反驳了最近将结合域缩小到阶段的尝试,而支持用包含拟词和主语的最小 XP 来定义结合域这一更传统的方法。最后,我们证明了二元结构的形态句法属性可以与它的两个组成部分的独立属性联系起来,而这些属性又可以与互惠性的解释方面联系起来。
{"title":"The syntax of Greek split reciprocals","authors":"Lefteris Paparounas, Martin Salzmann","doi":"10.1111/synt.12289","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/synt.12289","url":null,"abstract":"We provide the first detailed description and analysis of the syntax of the understudied Greek split reciprocal reconstruction. As in other languages, the reciprocal appears to be bipartite consisting of a quantificational distributor (‘the one’) and a reciprocator (‘the other’). We show that, in Greek, this bipartiteness runs deep: the two parts are syntactically independent, with the reciprocator having the syntax of a Condition A anaphor, and the distributor behaving as a floating quantifier. Once we turn to how these elements establish relations between themselves and their antecedent, we find that Greek reciprocals resist a movement‐ or Agree‐based analysis, since both elements can occur in positions inaccessible to movement/Agree. Given that the reciprocator can occur in embedded subject position, the Greek data also argue against recent attempts to reduce the binding domain to phases, instead supporting a more traditional definition of the binding domain in terms of the smallest XP containing the anaphor and a subject. Finally, we show that the morphosyntactic properties of the bipartite construction can be connected to independent properties of its two component parts and that these can, in turn, be related to interpretive aspects of reciprocity.","PeriodicalId":501329,"journal":{"name":"Syntax","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141357172","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A head movement analysis of second position clitics: The case of Russian polar particle li 第二位置咬字词的头部运动分析:俄语极化词 "li "的情况
Pub Date : 2024-05-18 DOI: 10.1111/synt.12288
Philip Shushurin
Russian polar particle li is usually analyzed as a second position clitic, constrained to appear at the linearly second position in the clause. I suggest that this requirement is a consequence of head movement: li is generated in —a polarity projection—merged directly above the associated polar constituent (X). This constituent must head‐move and left‐adjoin to li. The complex head (X+li) is largely equivalent to a wh‐word: at later stages of the derivation, it is attracted to the left periphery of the clause. li can be seen as an analog of a wh‐morpheme, which merges with different morphemes to form a wh‐word. Treating X+li as a complex head allows us to reduce the second position requirement of li to the left edge requirement on the X+li, a requirement often postulated for wh‐words. I provide further evidence for the hypothesis by showing the complementarity of li and negation.
俄语中的极性微粒 li 通常被分析为第二位置助动词,必须出现在分句中的线性第二位置。我认为这一要求是头部移动的结果:li 是在极性投影中产生的,直接合并在相关极性成分 (X) 的上方。这个成分必须头部移动并左连接到 li。复合词头(X+li)在很大程度上等同于一个 wh-词:在派生的后期,它被吸引到分句的左外围。把 X+li 看作复合词头,我们就可以把 li 的第二位置要求简化为对 X+li 的左边缘要求,这也是我们经常假设的对 wh-word 的要求。我通过证明 li 和否定的互补性为这一假设提供了进一步的证据。
{"title":"A head movement analysis of second position clitics: The case of Russian polar particle li","authors":"Philip Shushurin","doi":"10.1111/synt.12288","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/synt.12288","url":null,"abstract":"Russian polar particle <jats:italic>li</jats:italic> is usually analyzed as a second position clitic, constrained to appear at the linearly second position in the clause. I suggest that this requirement is a consequence of head movement: <jats:italic>li</jats:italic> is generated in —a polarity projection—merged directly above the associated polar constituent (X). This constituent must head‐move and left‐adjoin to <jats:italic>li</jats:italic>. The complex head (X+li) is largely equivalent to a <jats:italic>wh</jats:italic>‐word: at later stages of the derivation, it is attracted to the left periphery of the clause. <jats:italic>li</jats:italic> can be seen as an analog of a <jats:italic>wh</jats:italic>‐morpheme, which merges with different morphemes to form a <jats:italic>wh</jats:italic>‐word. Treating X+li as a complex head allows us to reduce the second position requirement of <jats:italic>li</jats:italic> to the left edge requirement on the X+li, a requirement often postulated for <jats:italic>wh</jats:italic>‐words. I provide further evidence for the hypothesis by showing the complementarity of <jats:italic>li</jats:italic> and negation.","PeriodicalId":501329,"journal":{"name":"Syntax","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141061294","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cross‐clausal scrambling and subject case in Balkar: On multiple specifiers and the locality of overt and covert movement 巴尔卡语中的跨因果混淆和主语情况:关于多重指示词以及公开和隐蔽运动的位置性
Pub Date : 2024-03-29 DOI: 10.1111/synt.12286
Tatiana Bondarenko, Colin Davis
We use fieldwork data about cross‐clausal scrambling in Balkar (Turkic) to clarify the nature of movement and its constraints. Balkar has a variety of embedded nominalized clauses, with different subject cases and possibilities for movement. Clauses with nominative (nom) subjects permit cross‐clausal object extraction but not subject extraction. In contrast, clauses with accusative subjects permit both such movements, although movement of the subject is required for object extraction. Finally, clauses with genitive subjects permit only subject extraction. We argue that these facts provide evidence for the following proposals: (i) multiple specifiers are usually possible provided that tucking‐in applies; (ii) the highest of a phase's multiple specifiers is privileged for accessibility; (iii) movement is constrained by anti‐locality (a ban on short movements); and (iv) Balkar DPs do not permit multiple specifiers. These factors are intertwined informatively in Balkar, and are supported by additional facts about possessors, binding, and covert movement.
我们利用巴尔卡尔语(突厥语)中跨因果关系争用的田野调查数据来阐明移动的性质及其制约因素。巴尔卡语有多种嵌入式名词化从句,其主语情况和移动可能性各不相同。带名词性(nom)主语的从句允许跨句提取宾语,但不允许提取主语。与此相反,带有指称主语的从句允许这两种移动,尽管主语的移动是提取宾语的必要条件。最后,带有属词主语的从句只允许提取主语。我们认为,这些事实为以下建议提供了证据:(i) 只要 "掖入"(tucking-in)适用,多重指称通常是可能的;(ii) 一个相位的多重指称中最高的指称在可及性方面具有优先权;(iii) 移动受到反位置性(禁止短移动)的限制;(iv) 巴尔卡语 DP 不允许多重指称。这些因素在巴尔卡语中交织在一起,信息丰富,并得到了关于拥有者、约束力和隐蔽移动的其他事实的支持。
{"title":"Cross‐clausal scrambling and subject case in Balkar: On multiple specifiers and the locality of overt and covert movement","authors":"Tatiana Bondarenko, Colin Davis","doi":"10.1111/synt.12286","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/synt.12286","url":null,"abstract":"We use fieldwork data about cross‐clausal scrambling in Balkar (Turkic) to clarify the nature of movement and its constraints. Balkar has a variety of embedded nominalized clauses, with different subject cases and possibilities for movement. Clauses with nominative (nom) subjects permit cross‐clausal object extraction but not subject extraction. In contrast, clauses with accusative subjects permit both such movements, although movement of the subject is required for object extraction. Finally, clauses with genitive subjects permit only subject extraction. We argue that these facts provide evidence for the following proposals: (i) multiple specifiers are usually possible provided that tucking‐in applies; (ii) the highest of a phase's multiple specifiers is privileged for accessibility; (iii) movement is constrained by anti‐locality (a ban on short movements); and (iv) Balkar DPs do not permit multiple specifiers. These factors are intertwined informatively in Balkar, and are supported by additional facts about possessors, binding, and covert movement.","PeriodicalId":501329,"journal":{"name":"Syntax","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140367090","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Movement in disguise: Morphology as a diagnostic for verb movement in Algonquian 变相的运动:形态学作为阿尔冈基语动词移动的诊断方法
Pub Date : 2024-03-27 DOI: 10.1111/synt.12281
Ksenia Bogomolets, Paula Fenger, Adrian Stegovec
This paper argues for a unification of two seemingly unrelated phenomena from unrelated language families: Verb Second in Germanic, and Conjunct versus Independent Order in Algonquian. It is argued that both reflect the possibility of the verb moving to C. While in Germanic this results in word order differences, in Algonquian V‐to‐C movement is detectable only via morphological alternations in agreement morphology. Under this view, Conjunct/Independent agreement and V2 are merely distinct reflexes of the same underlying process. This opens up new avenues of research in relation to V‐to‐C movement, framing it as a parametric option with potentially very different surface results in different languages depending on the setting of other parameters.
本文论证了将两个互不相关语系中看似互不相关的现象统一起来的观点:日耳曼语中的动词第二顺序和阿尔冈基语中的连词顺序与独立顺序。在日耳曼语中,这导致了词序的差异,而在阿尔冈古语中,V 到 C 的移动只能通过协议形态的形态交替来检测。根据这一观点,连接/独立协议和 V2 只是同一基本过程的不同反射。这就为 V 到 C 的移动开辟了新的研究途径,将其视为一种参数选项,根据其他参数的设置,在不同的语言中可能会产生截然不同的表面结果。
{"title":"Movement in disguise: Morphology as a diagnostic for verb movement in Algonquian","authors":"Ksenia Bogomolets, Paula Fenger, Adrian Stegovec","doi":"10.1111/synt.12281","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/synt.12281","url":null,"abstract":"This paper argues for a unification of two seemingly unrelated phenomena from unrelated language families: Verb Second in Germanic, and Conjunct versus Independent Order in Algonquian. It is argued that both reflect the possibility of the verb moving to C. While in Germanic this results in word order differences, in Algonquian V‐to‐C movement is detectable only via morphological alternations in agreement morphology. Under this view, Conjunct/Independent agreement and V2 are merely distinct reflexes of the same underlying process. This opens up new avenues of research in relation to V‐to‐C movement, framing it as a parametric option with potentially very different surface results in different languages depending on the setting of other parameters.","PeriodicalId":501329,"journal":{"name":"Syntax","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140376600","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Verb echo answers and ellipsis operations: A reply to Sato and Hayashi (2018) 动词呼应答案和省略操作:对 Sato 和 Hayashi (2018) 的答复
Pub Date : 2024-03-25 DOI: 10.1111/synt.12274
Hidekazu Tanaka
Recent research suggests that verb echo answers (VEAs) to a polar question in Japanese are derived by sluicing: the V head moves to C, and the complement of C, TP, undergoes deletion, stranding the verbal complex in C. Two pieces of evidence are provided for the view: adjunct inclusive interpretation and voice mismatch, neither of which is conclusive, as shown here. One consequence of the sluicing analysis is that VEAs reverse scope. We show that the semantic shift has nothing to do with the scope reversal. Our conclusion is that VEAs are unambiguously derived by multiple applications of argument ellipsis. We also advance an alternative explanation of the obligatory wide scope for focus phrases.
最近的研究表明,日语中极性问题的动词应答(VEAs)是通过 "滑动"(sluicing)产生的:V 头移动到 C,C 的补语 TP 被删除,动词复合体被搁置在 C 中。分流分析的一个结果是,VEA 的范围发生了逆转。我们的研究表明,语义转换与范围倒置无关。我们的结论是,VEA 是通过多次应用省略论据而明确推导出来的。我们还对焦点短语的强制性宽范围提出了另一种解释。
{"title":"Verb echo answers and ellipsis operations: A reply to Sato and Hayashi (2018)","authors":"Hidekazu Tanaka","doi":"10.1111/synt.12274","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/synt.12274","url":null,"abstract":"Recent research suggests that verb echo answers (VEAs) to a polar question in Japanese are derived by sluicing: the V head moves to C, and the complement of C, TP, undergoes deletion, stranding the verbal complex in C. Two pieces of evidence are provided for the view: adjunct inclusive interpretation and voice mismatch, neither of which is conclusive, as shown here. One consequence of the sluicing analysis is that VEAs reverse scope. We show that the semantic shift has nothing to do with the scope reversal. Our conclusion is that VEAs are unambiguously derived by multiple applications of argument ellipsis. We also advance an alternative explanation of the obligatory wide scope for focus phrases.","PeriodicalId":501329,"journal":{"name":"Syntax","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140298083","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Just pair‐merge 只需配对合并
Pub Date : 2024-03-15 DOI: 10.1111/synt.12284
Ken Safir
Two structure‐building operations are currently posited in minimalist theory: an operation forming sets (set merge), and an operation forming ordered pairs (pair‐merge). I argue that pair‐merge is sufficient to generate syntactic relations, so set merge, also called simple merge, should be eliminated from syntactic theory on grounds of simplicity. This conclusion requires reevaluating the relationship between structure‐building and labeling of constituents for the expression of syntactic relations, because labeling plays a crucial role in this comparison of theories according to the simplicity metric. An existing labeling hypothesis, specifically Chomsky's Labeling Algorithm, is shown not to have any advantage claimed for it by comparison with the just pair‐merge hypothesis proposed here. An advantage of the only pair‐merge hypothesis is that it provides a more principled origin for the inherent asymmetry in the c‐command relation that does not follow from a theory that includes set merge.
极简理论目前假定有两种结构构建操作:一种是形成集合的操作(集合合并),另一种是形成有序对的操作(对合并)。我认为成对合并足以生成句法关系,因此集合并(又称简单合并)应该以简单为由从句法理论中剔除。要得出这一结论,就必须重新评估为表达句法关系而进行的结构构建与成分标注之间的关系,因为标注在根据简单性尺度进行的理论比较中起着至关重要的作用。现有的标注假说,特别是乔姆斯基的标注算法,与本文提出的只对合并假说相比,并没有任何优势可言。仅成对合并假说的一个优点是,它为 C 命令关系中固有的不对称提供了一个更原则性的起源,而这种不对称并不是从包含集合合并的理论中产生的。
{"title":"Just pair‐merge","authors":"Ken Safir","doi":"10.1111/synt.12284","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/synt.12284","url":null,"abstract":"Two structure‐building operations are currently posited in minimalist theory: an operation forming sets (set merge), and an operation forming ordered pairs (pair‐merge). I argue that pair‐merge is sufficient to generate syntactic relations, so set merge, also called simple merge, should be eliminated from syntactic theory on grounds of simplicity. This conclusion requires reevaluating the relationship between structure‐building and labeling of constituents for the expression of syntactic relations, because labeling plays a crucial role in this comparison of theories according to the simplicity metric. An existing labeling hypothesis, specifically Chomsky's Labeling Algorithm, is shown not to have any advantage claimed for it by comparison with the just pair‐merge hypothesis proposed here. An advantage of the only pair‐merge hypothesis is that it provides a more principled origin for the inherent asymmetry in the c‐command relation that does not follow from a theory that includes set merge.","PeriodicalId":501329,"journal":{"name":"Syntax","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140146622","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Nominal licensing via dependent case: The view from pseudo noun incorporation in Wolof 通过从属情况进行名词许可:从沃洛夫语中的伪名词并入看问题
Pub Date : 2024-03-14 DOI: 10.1111/synt.12282
Suzana Fong
Bare nominals in Wolof can occur in the object position and they must be adjacent to the verb that subcategorizes for them. This is a property usually attributed to pseudo noun incorporation (PNI). However, there are two circumstances under which this adjacency requirement is obviated: a DP is introduced between the subject and the PNI‐ed object, or the latter is ‐moved. While these are disparate phenomena, a dependent case analysis of nominal licensing can account for PNI in Wolof. I assume that all nominals must be licensed with case, with case assignment being calculated in terms of dependent case. If this is not possible, a last resort strategy arises, namely surface adjacency with the verb. This analysis provides a unified analysis of PNI in Wolof.
沃洛夫语中的裸名词可以出现在宾语位置上,而且必须与对其进行次分类的动词相邻。这一特性通常归因于伪名词并入(PNI)。但是,在两种情况下,这种相邻要求可以被取消:在主语和 PNI 所指的宾语之间引入了一个 DP,或者后者被移动了。虽然这两种现象互不相同,但通过从属情况分析名义许可可以解释沃洛夫语中的 PNI。我假设所有的名词都必须有大小写许可,而大小写的分配是根据从属大小写来计算的。如果做不到这一点,就会出现最后一种策略,即与动词的表面邻接。这一分析提供了沃洛夫语中 PNI 的统一分析。
{"title":"Nominal licensing via dependent case: The view from pseudo noun incorporation in Wolof","authors":"Suzana Fong","doi":"10.1111/synt.12282","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/synt.12282","url":null,"abstract":"Bare nominals in Wolof can occur in the object position and they must be adjacent to the verb that subcategorizes for them. This is a property usually attributed to pseudo noun incorporation (PNI). However, there are two circumstances under which this adjacency requirement is obviated: a DP is introduced between the subject and the PNI‐ed object, or the latter is ‐moved. While these are disparate phenomena, a dependent case analysis of nominal licensing can account for PNI in Wolof. I assume that all nominals must be licensed with case, with case assignment being calculated in terms of dependent case. If this is not possible, a last resort strategy arises, namely surface adjacency with the verb. This analysis provides a unified analysis of PNI in Wolof.","PeriodicalId":501329,"journal":{"name":"Syntax","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140146678","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Coordination and binary branching 协调和二进制分支
Pub Date : 2024-03-08 DOI: 10.1111/synt.12285
Adam Przepiórkowski
In “Subordination and Binary Branching”, a recent (2023) Syntax paper, Ad Neeleman and colleagues proposed a new analysis of subordination. The main aim of this paper is to refute that analysis, using data from the coordination of unlike categories and unlike grammatical functions. Additionally, building on Neeleman et al.'s observations about the arbitrarily -ary—not just binary—nature of coordination, I sketch a more Minimalist approach to subordination and coordination that is devoid of the problems that Neeleman et al.'s analysis faces, but otherwise covers a similar range of data. On this approach, “subordination” is a synonym of “result of PairMerge” and “coordination” is a synonym of “result of SetMerge”, where SetMerge is understood as an operation creating an arbitrary set, as opposed to the usual more specialized Merge operation, which creates a binary set.
在最近(2023 年)的一篇语法论文 "从属关系和二元分支 "中,Ad Neeleman 及其同事提出了对从属关系的新分析。本文的主要目的是利用不同类别和不同语法功能的协调数据来反驳这一分析。此外,基于 Neeleman 等人对协调的任意 n$$ n$$-ary 而不仅仅是二元性质的观察,我对从属关系和协调勾勒出一种更简约的方法,这种方法没有 Neeleman 等人的分析所面临的问题,但在其他方面涵盖了类似的数据范围。根据这种方法,"从属关系 "是 "PairMerge 的结果 "的同义词,而 "协调关系 "是 "SetMerge 的结果 "的同义词,其中 SetMerge 被理解为创建任意集合的操作,而不是通常的更专业的合并操作,后者创建的是二元集合。
{"title":"Coordination and binary branching","authors":"Adam Przepiórkowski","doi":"10.1111/synt.12285","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/synt.12285","url":null,"abstract":"In “Subordination and Binary Branching”, a recent (2023) <i>Syntax</i> paper, Ad Neeleman and colleagues proposed a new analysis of subordination. The main aim of this paper is to refute that analysis, using data from the coordination of unlike categories and unlike grammatical functions. Additionally, building on Neeleman et al.'s observations about the arbitrarily <mjx-container aria-label=\"n\" ctxtmenu_counter=\"0\" ctxtmenu_oldtabindex=\"1\" jax=\"CHTML\" role=\"application\" sre-explorer- style=\"font-size: 103%; position: relative;\" tabindex=\"0\"><mjx-math aria-hidden=\"true\"><mjx-semantics><mjx-mrow><mjx-mi data-semantic-annotation=\"clearspeak:simple\" data-semantic-font=\"italic\" data-semantic- data-semantic-role=\"latinletter\" data-semantic-speech=\"n\" data-semantic-type=\"identifier\"><mjx-c></mjx-c></mjx-mi></mjx-mrow></mjx-semantics></mjx-math><mjx-assistive-mml aria-hidden=\"true\" display=\"inline\" unselectable=\"on\"><math altimg=\"/cms/asset/60476e55-ac2f-450a-95c0-b4949eb73e91/synt12285-math-0001.png\" xmlns=\"http://www.w3.org/1998/Math/MathML\"><semantics><mrow><mi data-semantic-=\"\" data-semantic-annotation=\"clearspeak:simple\" data-semantic-font=\"italic\" data-semantic-role=\"latinletter\" data-semantic-speech=\"n\" data-semantic-type=\"identifier\">n</mi></mrow>$$ n $$</annotation></semantics></math></mjx-assistive-mml></mjx-container>-ary—not just binary—nature of coordination, I sketch a more Minimalist approach to subordination and coordination that is devoid of the problems that Neeleman et al.'s analysis faces, but otherwise covers a similar range of data. On this approach, “subordination” is a synonym of “result of PairMerge” and “coordination” is a synonym of “result of SetMerge”, where SetMerge is understood as an operation creating an arbitrary set, as opposed to the usual more specialized Merge operation, which creates a binary set.","PeriodicalId":501329,"journal":{"name":"Syntax","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140075275","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the definition of Merge 关于合并的定义
Pub Date : 2024-03-08 DOI: 10.1111/synt.12287
Erik Zyman
Two fundamental tasks of syntactic inquiry are to identify the elementary structure-building operations and to determine what properties they have and why. This article aims to bring us closer to those goals by investigating Merge. Two recent definitions of Merge are evaluated. It is argued that both have significant strengths but also some drawbacks, and that set-theoretic definitions of Merge in general face conceptual problems. It is proposed that Merge is not set-theoretic but graph-theoretic in nature: the syntactic objects it operates on and creates are (bare-phrase-structure-compliant) phrase-structure trees. Two new formal definitions of Merge are proposed and evaluated. One obeys the No-Tampering Condition but makes it unclear why Merge() satisfies only one selectional feature of
句法研究的两项基本任务是识别基本的结构构建操作,并确定这些操作具有哪些特性及其原因。本文旨在通过研究 "合并"(Merge)使我们更接近这些目标。本文评估了最近关于 "合并 "的两个定义。文章认为,这两个定义都有很大的优点,但也有一些缺点,而且一般来说,集合论的合并定义都面临概念上的问题。有学者提出,Merge 的本质不是集合论,而是图论:它所操作和创建的句法对象是(符合裸词组结构的)词组结构树。本文提出并评估了两种新的合并形式定义。其中一个符合 "不篡改条件"(No-Tampering Condition),但不清楚为什么 Merge(α,β$$ alpha, beta $$)只能满足 α$$ alpha $$的一个选择特征,而不是所有特征。另一种方法解释了这一观察结果,但狭隘地违反了 "不篡改条件"。由此可以看出,"合并 "是一种图论操作,而不是集合论操作。
{"title":"On the definition of Merge","authors":"Erik Zyman","doi":"10.1111/synt.12287","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/synt.12287","url":null,"abstract":"Two fundamental tasks of syntactic inquiry are to identify the elementary structure-building operations and to determine what properties they have and why. This article aims to bring us closer to those goals by investigating Merge. Two recent definitions of Merge are evaluated. It is argued that both have significant strengths but also some drawbacks, and that set-theoretic definitions of Merge in general face conceptual problems. It is proposed that Merge is not set-theoretic but graph-theoretic in nature: the syntactic objects it operates on and creates are (bare-phrase-structure-compliant) phrase-structure trees. Two new formal definitions of Merge are proposed and evaluated. One obeys the No-Tampering Condition but makes it unclear why Merge(<mjx-container aria-label=\"alpha comma beta\" ctxtmenu_counter=\"0\" ctxtmenu_oldtabindex=\"1\" jax=\"CHTML\" role=\"application\" sre-explorer- style=\"font-size: 103%; position: relative;\" tabindex=\"0\"><mjx-math aria-hidden=\"true\"><mjx-semantics><mjx-mrow data-semantic-children=\"0,1,2\" data-semantic-content=\"1\" data-semantic- data-semantic-role=\"sequence\" data-semantic-speech=\"alpha comma beta\" data-semantic-type=\"punctuated\"><mjx-mi data-semantic-annotation=\"clearspeak:simple\" data-semantic-font=\"italic\" data-semantic- data-semantic-parent=\"3\" data-semantic-role=\"greekletter\" data-semantic-type=\"identifier\"><mjx-c></mjx-c></mjx-mi><mjx-mo data-semantic- data-semantic-operator=\"punctuated\" data-semantic-parent=\"3\" data-semantic-role=\"comma\" data-semantic-type=\"punctuation\" rspace=\"3\" style=\"margin-left: 0.056em;\"><mjx-c></mjx-c></mjx-mo><mjx-mi data-semantic-annotation=\"clearspeak:simple\" data-semantic-font=\"italic\" data-semantic- data-semantic-parent=\"3\" data-semantic-role=\"greekletter\" data-semantic-type=\"identifier\"><mjx-c></mjx-c></mjx-mi></mjx-mrow></mjx-semantics></mjx-math><mjx-assistive-mml aria-hidden=\"true\" display=\"inline\" unselectable=\"on\"><math altimg=\"/cms/asset/912f0c60-02ca-4dc0-8efd-fcd161777bda/synt12287-math-0001.png\" xmlns=\"http://www.w3.org/1998/Math/MathML\"><semantics><mrow data-semantic-=\"\" data-semantic-children=\"0,1,2\" data-semantic-content=\"1\" data-semantic-role=\"sequence\" data-semantic-speech=\"alpha comma beta\" data-semantic-type=\"punctuated\"><mi data-semantic-=\"\" data-semantic-annotation=\"clearspeak:simple\" data-semantic-font=\"italic\" data-semantic-parent=\"3\" data-semantic-role=\"greekletter\" data-semantic-type=\"identifier\">α</mi><mo data-semantic-=\"\" data-semantic-operator=\"punctuated\" data-semantic-parent=\"3\" data-semantic-role=\"comma\" data-semantic-type=\"punctuation\">,</mo><mi data-semantic-=\"\" data-semantic-annotation=\"clearspeak:simple\" data-semantic-font=\"italic\" data-semantic-parent=\"3\" data-semantic-role=\"greekletter\" data-semantic-type=\"identifier\">β</mi></mrow>$$ alpha, beta $$</annotation></semantics></math></mjx-assistive-mml></mjx-container>) satisfies only one selectional feature of <mjx-container aria-label=\"alpha\" ctxtmenu_counter=\"1\" ctxtmenu_oldtabindex=\"1\" jax=\"CHTML\"","PeriodicalId":501329,"journal":{"name":"Syntax","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140075233","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How to apply multiple scrambling in Japanese 如何在日语中应用多重加扰
Pub Date : 2024-03-06 DOI: 10.1111/synt.12283
Jun Abe
In this paper, I argue that the phenomena of multiple scrambling in Japanese are best captured by assuming different derivations, depending on whether they involve long-distance or clause-internal scrambling. I argue that long-distance multiple scrambling involves remnant VP scrambling, on the assumption that long-distance scrambling necessarily produces a focus chain, which thus prohibits separate applications of scrambling in multiple scrambling configurations. The evidence comes from (i) long-distance multiple scrambling of wh-phrases that behaves like wh-movement, and (ii) long-distance multiple scrambling of quantificational phrases (QPs) that behaves as if these QPs constitute a single QP. As for clause-internal multiple scrambling, I argue that it may involve separate applications of scrambling as well as remnant VP scrambling. The evidence comes from scope facts that involve two scrambled QPs: when the two QPs undergo remnant VP scrambling, they both take scope in their original positions; when they are separately scrambled, they show a different pattern of reconstruction with respect to scope.
在本文中,我认为日语中的多重加扰现象最好通过假设不同的派生来捕捉,这取决于它们是涉及长距加扰还是分句内部加扰。我认为远距离多重加扰涉及残余副词加扰,其假设是远距离加扰必然产生焦点链,因此禁止在多重加扰配置中单独应用加扰。证据来自(i)wh-短语的长距离多重加扰,其行为类似于wh-移动;(ii)量词短语(QPs)的长距离多重加扰,其行为类似于这些量词短语构成了一个单一的QPs。至于分句内部的多重加扰,我认为它可能涉及单独的加扰应用以及残余的 VP 加扰。证据来自涉及两个被扰乱的 QPs 的范围事实:当两个 QPs 经过残余 VP 扰乱时,它们都在原来的位置上占据范围;当它们分别被扰乱时,它们在范围方面显示出不同的重构模式。
{"title":"How to apply multiple scrambling in Japanese","authors":"Jun Abe","doi":"10.1111/synt.12283","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/synt.12283","url":null,"abstract":"In this paper, I argue that the phenomena of multiple scrambling in Japanese are best captured by assuming different derivations, depending on whether they involve long-distance or clause-internal scrambling. I argue that long-distance multiple scrambling involves remnant VP scrambling, on the assumption that long-distance scrambling necessarily produces a focus chain, which thus prohibits separate applications of scrambling in multiple scrambling configurations. The evidence comes from (i) long-distance multiple scrambling of <i>wh</i>-phrases that behaves like <i>wh</i>-movement, and (ii) long-distance multiple scrambling of quantificational phrases (QPs) that behaves as if these QPs constitute a single QP. As for clause-internal multiple scrambling, I argue that it may involve separate applications of scrambling as well as remnant VP scrambling. The evidence comes from scope facts that involve two scrambled QPs: when the two QPs undergo remnant VP scrambling, they both take scope in their original positions; when they are separately scrambled, they show a different pattern of reconstruction with respect to scope.","PeriodicalId":501329,"journal":{"name":"Syntax","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140045471","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Syntax
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1