首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Neuropsychology最新文献

英文 中文
Time-course of phonetic (motor speech) encoding in utterance production. 语音(运动语音)编码在话语产生中的时间过程。
IF 3.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-09 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2023.2279739
Marina Laganaro

Speaking involves the preparation of the linguistic content of an utterance and of the motor programs leading to articulation. The temporal dynamics of linguistic versus motor-speech (phonetic) encoding is highly debated: phonetic encoding has been associated either to the last quarter of an utterance preparation time (∼150ms before articulation), or to virtually the entire planning time, simultaneously with linguistic encoding. We (i) review the evidence on the time-course of motor-speech encoding based on EEG/MEG event-related (ERP) studies and (ii) strive to replicate the early effects of phonological-phonetic factors in referential word production by reanalysing a large EEG/ERP dataset. The review indicates that motor-speech encoding is engaged during at least the last 300ms preceding articulation (about half of a word planning lag). By contrast, the very early involvement of phonological-phonetic factors could be replicated only partially and is not as robust as in the second half of the utterance planning time-window.

口语包括准备话语的语言内容和导致发音的运动程序。语言与运动语音(语音)编码的时间动态备受争议:语音编码要么与话语准备时间的最后四分之一(发音前~150ms)有关,要么与语言编码同时与几乎整个计划时间有关。我们(i)回顾了基于EEG/MEG事件相关(ERP)研究的运动语音编码时间过程的证据,以及(ii)通过重新分析大型EEG/ERP数据集,努力复制语音因素在指称词产生中的早期影响。综述表明,运动语音编码至少在发音前的最后300毫秒(约为单词规划滞后的一半)进行。相比之下,语音-语音因素的早期参与只能部分复制,并且不像话语计划时间窗口的后半段那样稳健。
{"title":"Time-course of phonetic (motor speech) encoding in utterance production.","authors":"Marina Laganaro","doi":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2279739","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2279739","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Speaking involves the preparation of the linguistic content of an utterance and of the motor programs leading to articulation. The temporal dynamics of linguistic versus motor-speech (phonetic) encoding is highly debated: phonetic encoding has been associated either to the last quarter of an utterance preparation time (∼150ms before articulation), or to virtually the entire planning time, simultaneously with linguistic encoding. We (i) review the evidence on the time-course of motor-speech encoding based on EEG/MEG event-related (ERP) studies and (ii) strive to replicate the early effects of phonological-phonetic factors in referential word production by reanalysing a large EEG/ERP dataset. The review indicates that motor-speech encoding is engaged during at least the last 300ms preceding articulation (about half of a word planning lag). By contrast, the very early involvement of phonological-phonetic factors could be replicated only partially and is not as robust as in the second half of the utterance planning time-window.</p>","PeriodicalId":50670,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72015949","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A developmental account of the role of sequential dependencies in typical and atypical language learners. 顺序依赖在典型和非典型语言学习者中的发展作用。
IF 3.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-14 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2023.2275837
Lisa Goffman, LouAnn Gerken

The Gerken lab has shown that infants are able to learn sound patterns that obligate local sequential dependencies that are no longer readily accessible to adults. The Goffman lab has shown that children with developmental language disorder (DLD) exhibit deficits in learning sequential dependencies that influence the acquisition of words and grammar, as well as other types of domain general sequences. Thus, DLD appears to be an impaired ability to detect and deploy sequential dependencies over multiple domains. We meld these two lines of research to propose a novel account in which sequential dependency learning is required for many phonological and morphosyntactic patterns in natural language and is also central to the language and domain general deficits that are attested in DLD. However, patterns that are not dependent on sequential dependencies but rather on networks of stored forms are learnable by children with DLD as well as by adults.

格肯实验室已经证明,婴儿能够学习具有局部顺序依赖性的声音模式,而这种依赖性对成年人来说不再容易获得。Goffman实验室已经证明,患有发展性语言障碍(DLD)的儿童在学习顺序依赖方面表现出缺陷,这影响了单词和语法的习得,以及其他类型的领域一般序列。因此,DLD在多个域中检测和部署顺序依赖项的能力似乎受到了损害。我们将这两条研究路线融合在一起,提出了一种新的解释,其中顺序依赖学习是自然语言中许多语音和形态句法模式所必需的,也是DLD中所证明的语言和领域一般缺陷的核心。然而,不依赖于顺序依赖而是依赖于存储形式网络的模式,对于患有DLD的儿童和成人来说都是可以学习的。
{"title":"A developmental account of the role of sequential dependencies in typical and atypical language learners.","authors":"Lisa Goffman, LouAnn Gerken","doi":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2275837","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2275837","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Gerken lab has shown that infants are able to learn sound patterns that obligate local sequential dependencies that are no longer readily accessible to adults. The Goffman lab has shown that children with developmental language disorder (DLD) exhibit deficits in learning sequential dependencies that influence the acquisition of words and grammar, as well as other types of domain general sequences. Thus, DLD appears to be an impaired ability to detect and deploy sequential dependencies over multiple domains. We meld these two lines of research to propose a novel account in which sequential dependency learning is required for many phonological and morphosyntactic patterns in natural language and is also central to the language and domain general deficits that are attested in DLD. However, patterns that are not dependent on sequential dependencies but rather on networks of stored forms are learnable by children with DLD as well as by adults.</p>","PeriodicalId":50670,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10939949/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"107592690","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Toward the characterization of a visual form of developmental dyslexia: Reduced visuo-attentional capacity for processing multiple stimuli made of separable features. 关于发展性阅读障碍视觉形式的表征:处理由可分离特征组成的多种刺激的视觉注意能力降低。
IF 3.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-12 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2023.2266179
Audrey Vialatte, Eric Chabanat, Agnès Witko, Laure Pisella

Some dyslexics cannot process multiple letters simultaneously. It has been argued that this reduced visuo-attentional (VA) letter span could result from poor reading ability and experience. Here, moving away from reading context, we showed that dyslexic group exhibited slower visual search than normal readers group for "symbols", defined as graphic stimuli made up of separable visual features, but not for filled objects. Slowness in symbol visual search was explained by reduced VA field and atypical ocular behaviour when processing those letter-like stimuli and was associated with reduced VA letter span and impaired elementary visuo-spatial perception. Such a basic visual search deficit can hardly be attributed to poor reading ability and experience. Moreover, because it is specific to letter-like stimuli (i.e., "symbols"), it can specifically hinder reading acquisition. Symbol visual search can easily be tested in the pre-reading phase, opening up prospects for early risk detection and prevention of VA dyslexia.

有些阅读障碍者不能同时处理多个字母。有人认为,视觉注意(VA)字母跨度的减少可能是由于阅读能力和经验差造成的。在这里,脱离阅读语境,我们发现阅读障碍组对“符号”的视觉搜索比正常读者组慢,“符号”被定义为由可分离的视觉特征组成的图形刺激,但对填充的物体则不然。符号视觉搜索的迟缓是由处理这些字母样刺激时VA场减少和非典型的眼部行为解释的,并与VA字母跨度减少和初级视觉空间感知受损有关。这种基本的视觉搜索缺陷很难归因于较差的阅读能力和经验。此外,由于它是特定于字母样刺激(即“符号”)的,它会特别阻碍阅读习得。符号视觉搜索可以很容易地在阅读前阶段进行测试,为VA阅读障碍的早期风险检测和预防开辟了前景。
{"title":"Toward the characterization of a visual form of developmental dyslexia: Reduced visuo-attentional capacity for processing multiple stimuli made of separable features.","authors":"Audrey Vialatte, Eric Chabanat, Agnès Witko, Laure Pisella","doi":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2266179","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2266179","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Some dyslexics cannot process multiple letters simultaneously. It has been argued that this reduced visuo-attentional (VA) letter span could result from poor reading ability and experience. Here, moving away from reading context, we showed that dyslexic group exhibited slower visual search than normal readers group for \"symbols\", defined as graphic stimuli made up of separable visual features, but not for filled objects. Slowness in symbol visual search was explained by reduced VA field and atypical ocular behaviour when processing those letter-like stimuli and was associated with reduced VA letter span and impaired elementary visuo-spatial perception. Such a basic visual search deficit can hardly be attributed to poor reading ability and experience. Moreover, because it is specific to letter-like stimuli (i.e., \"symbols\"), it can specifically hinder reading acquisition. Symbol visual search can easily be tested in the pre-reading phase, opening up prospects for early risk detection and prevention of VA dyslexia.</p>","PeriodicalId":50670,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49684562","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Semantic feature production norms for manipulable objects. 可操作对象的语义特征生成规范。
IF 3.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-12 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2023.2279185
Daniela Valério, Akbar Hussain, Jorge Almeida

Feature generation tasks and feature databases are important for understanding how knowledge is organized in semantic memory, as they reflect not only the kinds of information that individuals hold about objects but also how objects are conceptually represented. Traditionally, semantic norms focus on a variety of object categories and, as a result, have a small number of concepts per semantic category. Here, our main goal is to create a more fine-grained feature database exclusively for one category of objects-manipulable objects. This database contributes to the understanding of within-category, content-specific processing. To achieve this, we asked 130 participants to freely generate features for 80 manipulable objects and another group of 32 participants to generate action features for the same objects. We then compared our databases with other published semantic norms and found high similarity between them. In our databases, we calculated the similarity between objects in terms of visual, functional, encyclopaedic, and action feature types using Spearman correlation, Baker's gamma index, and cophenetic correlation. We discovered that objects were grouped in a distinctive and meaningful way according to feature type. Finally, we tested the validity of our databases by asking three groups of participants to perform a feature verification experiment while manipulating production frequency. Our results demonstrate that participants can recognize and associate the features of our databases with specific manipulable objects. Participants were faster to verify high-frequency features than low-frequency features. Overall, our data provide important insights into how we process manipulable objects and can be used to further inform cognitive and neural theories of object processing and identification.

特征生成任务和特征数据库对于理解知识在语义记忆中的组织方式非常重要,因为它们不仅反映了个体对对象的信息类型,而且反映了对象在概念上是如何表示的。传统上,语义规范关注于各种对象类别,因此每个语义类别的概念数量很少。这里,我们的主要目标是为一类对象(可操作对象)创建一个更细粒度的特征数据库。该数据库有助于理解类别内特定于内容的处理。为了实现这一目标,我们要求130名参与者自由地为80个可操作对象生成特征,另一组32名参与者为相同的对象生成动作特征。然后,我们将我们的数据库与其他已发布的语义规范进行比较,发现它们之间具有很高的相似性。在我们的数据库中,我们使用Spearman关联、Baker's gamma指数和cophenetic关联计算了对象之间在视觉、功能、百科全书和动作特征类型方面的相似性。我们发现,根据特征类型,物体以一种独特而有意义的方式分组。最后,我们通过要求三组参与者在操纵生产频率的同时执行特征验证实验来测试数据库的有效性。我们的结果表明,参与者可以识别并将我们的数据库的特征与特定的可操作对象联系起来。参与者验证高频特征的速度要快于低频特征。总的来说,我们的数据为我们如何处理可操作的物体提供了重要的见解,并可用于进一步告知物体处理和识别的认知和神经理论。
{"title":"Semantic feature production norms for manipulable objects.","authors":"Daniela Valério, Akbar Hussain, Jorge Almeida","doi":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2279185","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2279185","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Feature generation tasks and feature databases are important for understanding how knowledge is organized in semantic memory, as they reflect not only the kinds of information that individuals hold about objects but also how objects are conceptually represented. Traditionally, semantic norms focus on a variety of object categories and, as a result, have a small number of concepts per semantic category. Here, our main goal is to create a more fine-grained feature database exclusively for one category of objects-manipulable objects. This database contributes to the understanding of within-category, content-specific processing. To achieve this, we asked 130 participants to freely generate features for 80 manipulable objects and another group of 32 participants to generate action features for the same objects. We then compared our databases with other published semantic norms and found high similarity between them. In our databases, we calculated the similarity between objects in terms of visual, functional, encyclopaedic, and action feature types using Spearman correlation, Baker's gamma index, and cophenetic correlation. We discovered that objects were grouped in a distinctive and meaningful way according to feature type. Finally, we tested the validity of our databases by asking three groups of participants to perform a feature verification experiment while manipulating production frequency. Our results demonstrate that participants can recognize and associate the features of our databases with specific manipulable objects. Participants were faster to verify high-frequency features than low-frequency features. Overall, our data provide important insights into how we process manipulable objects and can be used to further inform cognitive and neural theories of object processing and identification.</p>","PeriodicalId":50670,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138435413","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Provoked overt recognition in acquired prosopagnosia using multiple different images of famous faces. 使用多个不同的著名面孔图像在获得性面容失认中引发明显识别。
IF 3.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-12 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2023.2269648
David Pitcher, Rebekah Caulfield, A Mike Burton

Provoked overt recognition refers to the fact that patients with acquired prosopagnosia can sometimes recognize faces when presented in arrays of individuals from the same category (e.g., actors or politicians). We ask whether a prosopagnosic patient might experience recognition when presented with multiple different images of the same face simultaneously. Over two sessions, patient Herschel, a 66-year-old British man with acquired prosopagnosia, viewed face images individually or in arrays. On several occasions he failed to recognize single photos of an individual but successfully identified that person when the same photos were presented together. For example, Herschel failed to recognize any individual images of King Charles or Paul McCartney but recognised both in arrays of the same photos. Like reports based on category membership, overt recognition was transient and inconsistent. These findings are discussed in terms of models of covert recognition, alongside more recent research on within-person variability for face perception.

被激发的公开识别是指,获得性失认症患者有时可以在同一类别的个人(如演员或政客)阵列中识别人脸。我们询问失认症患者在同时看到同一张脸的多个不同图像时是否会经历识别。患者赫歇尔是一名66岁的英国男子,患有后天性面容失认症,在两次治疗中,他分别或按阵列查看了面部图像。有几次,他没能认出一个人的照片,但当同一张照片放在一起时,他成功地认出了那个人。例如,赫歇尔没有识别出查尔斯国王或保罗·麦卡特尼的任何个人图像,但在相同的照片阵列中识别出了这两张图像。与基于类别成员身份的报告一样,公开承认是短暂的,前后不一。这些发现是在隐蔽识别模型的基础上讨论的,同时还有最近关于人脸感知的人内变异性的研究。
{"title":"Provoked overt recognition in acquired prosopagnosia using multiple different images of famous faces.","authors":"David Pitcher, Rebekah Caulfield, A Mike Burton","doi":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2269648","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2269648","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Provoked overt recognition refers to the fact that patients with acquired prosopagnosia can sometimes recognize faces when presented in arrays of individuals from the same category (e.g., actors or politicians). We ask whether a prosopagnosic patient might experience recognition when presented with multiple different images of the same face simultaneously. Over two sessions, patient Herschel, a 66-year-old British man with acquired prosopagnosia, viewed face images individually or in arrays. On several occasions he failed to recognize single photos of an individual but successfully identified that person when the same photos were presented together. For example, Herschel failed to recognize any individual images of King Charles or Paul McCartney but recognised both in arrays of the same photos. Like reports based on category membership, overt recognition was transient and inconsistent. These findings are discussed in terms of models of covert recognition, alongside more recent research on within-person variability for face perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":50670,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10791066/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41240603","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
"Looking at nothing": An implicit ocular motor index of face recognition in developmental prosopagnosia. “什么都不看”:发育性失认症中人脸识别的内隐眼运动指数。
IF 3.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-23 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2023.2250510
Aida Rahavi, Manuela Malaspina, Andrea Albonico, Jason J S Barton

Subjects often look towards to previous location of a stimulus related to a task even when that stimulus is no longer visible. In this study we asked whether this effect would be preserved or reduced in subjects with developmental prosopagnosia. Participants learned faces presented in video-clips and then saw a brief montage of four faces, which was replaced by a screen with empty boxes, at which time they indicated whether the learned face had been present in the montage. Control subjects were more likely to look at the blank location where the learned face had appeared, on both hit and miss trials, though the effect was larger on hit trials. Prosopagnosic subjects showed a reduced effect, though still better on hit than on miss trials. We conclude that explicit accuracy and our implicit looking at nothing effect are parallel effects reflecting the strength of the neural activity underlying face recognition.

受试者通常会寻找与任务相关的刺激的先前位置,即使该刺激不再可见。在这项研究中,我们询问了发育性失认症受试者是否会保留或减少这种影响。参与者学习了视频剪辑中出现的人脸,然后看到了四张人脸的简短蒙太奇,被一个带有空框的屏幕取代,此时他们指示蒙太奇中是否存在学习到的人脸。在命中和未命中试验中,对照受试者更有可能看到学习过的面孔出现的空白位置,尽管命中试验的影响更大。生药受试者的效果有所下降,尽管在命中试验中仍然优于未命中试验。我们得出的结论是,显式准确率和我们的隐式不看任何东西的效应是平行的,反映了人脸识别背后神经活动的强度。
{"title":"\"Looking at nothing\": An implicit ocular motor index of face recognition in developmental prosopagnosia.","authors":"Aida Rahavi,&nbsp;Manuela Malaspina,&nbsp;Andrea Albonico,&nbsp;Jason J S Barton","doi":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2250510","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2250510","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Subjects often look towards to previous location of a stimulus related to a task even when that stimulus is no longer visible. In this study we asked whether this effect would be preserved or reduced in subjects with developmental prosopagnosia. Participants learned faces presented in video-clips and then saw a brief montage of four faces, which was replaced by a screen with empty boxes, at which time they indicated whether the learned face had been present in the montage. Control subjects were more likely to look at the blank location where the learned face had appeared, on both hit and miss trials, though the effect was larger on hit trials. Prosopagnosic subjects showed a reduced effect, though still better on hit than on miss trials. We conclude that explicit accuracy and our implicit looking at nothing effect are parallel effects reflecting the strength of the neural activity underlying face recognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":50670,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10060379","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship between semantic and episodic memory: evidence from a case of severe anterograde amnesia. 语义记忆和情节记忆之间的关系:来自一例严重顺行性健忘症的证据。
IF 3.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-25 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2023.2250532
Clément Polin, Aurélie Lacroix, Claire Boutet, Fabien Schneider, Leslie Cartz-Piver, Cécile Diebolt, Jean-Pierre Clément, Benjamin Calvet

It is increasingly being recognized that new declarative, consciously accessible information can be learned in anterograde amnesia, but it is not clear whether this learning is supported by episodic or semantic memory. We report a case of a 55-year-old man who experienced severe amnesia after limited damage to the medial temporal lobe following neurosurgical complications. His general cognitive performance and knowledge of new French words and public events that occurred before and after the onset of amnesia were assessed. Performance remained satisfactory on post-morbid vocabulary and public events, with a drop in performance observed for very recent public events only, while knowledge of very recent vocabulary was comparable to that of the control subjects. The implications of these findings for our understanding of the underlying learning mechanisms are discussed. This is the first report of acquisition of consciously accessible postmorbid knowledge of public events in a patient with severe amnesia.

人们越来越认识到,新的陈述性、有意识地可获得的信息可以在顺行性健忘症中学习,但尚不清楚这种学习是否得到情节记忆或语义记忆的支持。我们报告了一个55岁的男性病例,他在神经外科并发症后内侧颞叶受到有限损伤,经历了严重的健忘症。评估了他的一般认知表现、对法语新词的了解以及健忘症发作前后发生的公共事件。在病态后词汇和公共活动方面的表现仍然令人满意,仅在最近的公共活动中观察到表现下降,而对最近词汇的了解与对照受试者相当。讨论了这些发现对我们理解潜在学习机制的影响。这是第一份关于严重健忘症患者获得有意识的公共事件后知识的报告。
{"title":"The relationship between semantic and episodic memory: evidence from a case of severe anterograde amnesia.","authors":"Clément Polin,&nbsp;Aurélie Lacroix,&nbsp;Claire Boutet,&nbsp;Fabien Schneider,&nbsp;Leslie Cartz-Piver,&nbsp;Cécile Diebolt,&nbsp;Jean-Pierre Clément,&nbsp;Benjamin Calvet","doi":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2250532","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2250532","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is increasingly being recognized that new declarative, consciously accessible information can be learned in anterograde amnesia, but it is not clear whether this learning is supported by episodic or semantic memory. We report a case of a 55-year-old man who experienced severe amnesia after limited damage to the medial temporal lobe following neurosurgical complications. His general cognitive performance and knowledge of new French words and public events that occurred before and after the onset of amnesia were assessed. Performance remained satisfactory on post-morbid vocabulary and public events, with a drop in performance observed for very recent public events only, while knowledge of very recent vocabulary was comparable to that of the control subjects. The implications of these findings for our understanding of the underlying learning mechanisms are discussed. This is the first report of acquisition of consciously accessible postmorbid knowledge of public events in a patient with severe amnesia.</p>","PeriodicalId":50670,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10073321","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
From intermediate shape-centered representations to the perception of oriented shapes: response to commentaries. 从以形状为中心的中间表征到定向形状的感知:对评论的回应。
IF 3.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-29 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2023.2250511
Gilles Vannuscorps, Albert Galaburda, Alfonso Caramazza

In this response paper, we start by addressing the main points made by the commentators on the target article's main theoretical conclusions: the existence and characteristics of the intermediate shape-centered representations (ISCRs) in the visual system, their emergence from edge detection mechanisms operating on different types of visual properties, and how they are eventually reunited in higher order frames of reference underlying conscious visual perception. We also address the much-commented issue of the possible neural mechanisms of the ISCRs. In the final section, we address more specific and general comments, questions, and suggestions which, albeit very interesting, were less directly focused on the main conclusions of the target paper.

在这篇回应文章中,我们首先讨论了评论员对目标文章的主要理论结论提出的要点:视觉系统中中间形状中心表示(ISCR)的存在和特征,它们是从基于不同类型视觉特性的边缘检测机制中出现的,以及它们最终如何在意识视觉感知的更高阶参考系中重新结合。我们还讨论了ISCRs可能的神经机制这一备受争议的问题。在最后一节中,我们讨论了更具体和一般性的评论、问题和建议,这些评论、问题、建议虽然非常有趣,但不太直接集中在目标文件的主要结论上。
{"title":"From intermediate shape-centered representations to the perception of oriented shapes: response to commentaries.","authors":"Gilles Vannuscorps,&nbsp;Albert Galaburda,&nbsp;Alfonso Caramazza","doi":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2250511","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2250511","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this response paper, we start by addressing the main points made by the commentators on the target article's main theoretical conclusions: the existence and characteristics of the <i>intermediate shape-centered representations</i> (ISCRs) in the visual system, their emergence from edge detection mechanisms operating on different types of visual properties, and how they are eventually reunited in higher order frames of reference underlying conscious visual perception. We also address the much-commented issue of the possible neural mechanisms of the ISCRs. In the final section, we address more specific and general comments, questions, and suggestions which, albeit very interesting, were less directly focused on the main conclusions of the target paper.</p>","PeriodicalId":50670,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10167117","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An access deficit or a deficit in the phonological representations themselves: What can we learn from naming errors? 是读音障碍还是语音表征本身的障碍?我们能从命名错误中学到什么?
IF 3.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-04 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2023.2208745
Aviah Gvion, Michal Biran

Anomic aphasia is characterized by good comprehension and non-word repetition but poor naming. Two sub-types of deficits might be hypothesized: faulty access to preserved phonological representations or preserved access to impaired representations. Phonological errors may occur only when representations are impaired or in post-lexical deficits (conduction aphasia). We analysed the incidence of phonological naming errors of 30 individuals, 25 with anomic aphasia based on poor naming but good repetition and comprehension, and five with conduction aphasia based on poor naming and poor repetition. Individuals with anomic aphasia produced very few phonological errors compared to individuals with conduction aphasia (0-19.1% versus 42-66%). However, six individuals with anomia produced more than 11% phonological errors, suggesting two patterns of deficit: either impaired lexical representations or impaired access to them. The lack of phonological errors in most individuals with anomic aphasia suggests that access to the phonological output lexicon is semantically, not phonologically driven.

原子性失语症的特点是理解能力和非词重复能力强,但命名能力差。可以假设有两种亚类型的缺陷:对保留的语音表征的访问错误或对受损表征的访问保留。语音错误可能只发生在表征受损或词汇后障碍(传导性失语症)的情况下。我们分析了 30 名患者的语音命名错误发生率,其中 25 人患有命名不良但复述和理解能力良好的无名失语症,5 人患有命名不良和复述不良的传导性失语症。与传导性失语症患者相比,失音性失语症患者很少出现语音错误(0-19.1% 对 42-66%)。然而,有六名失认症患者的语音错误率超过了 11%,这表明存在两种缺陷模式:词汇表征受损或词汇获取受损。大多数失音症患者都没有语音错误,这表明对语音输出词库的访问是由语义驱动的,而不是由语音驱动的。
{"title":"An access deficit or a deficit in the phonological representations themselves: What can we learn from naming errors?","authors":"Aviah Gvion, Michal Biran","doi":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2208745","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2208745","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Anomic aphasia is characterized by good comprehension and non-word repetition but poor naming. Two sub-types of deficits might be hypothesized: faulty access to preserved phonological representations or preserved access to impaired representations. Phonological errors may occur only when representations are impaired or in post-lexical deficits (conduction aphasia). We analysed the incidence of phonological naming errors of 30 individuals, 25 with anomic aphasia based on poor naming but good repetition and comprehension, and five with conduction aphasia based on poor naming and poor repetition. Individuals with anomic aphasia produced very few phonological errors compared to individuals with conduction aphasia (0-19.1% versus 42-66%). However, six individuals with anomia produced more than 11% phonological errors, suggesting two patterns of deficit: either impaired lexical representations or impaired access to them. The lack of phonological errors in most individuals with anomic aphasia suggests that access to the phonological output lexicon is semantically, not phonologically driven.</p>","PeriodicalId":50670,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10168286","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Precision of phonological errors in aphasia supports resource models of phonological working memory in language production. 失语症患者语音错误的精确性支持语言生产中语音工作记忆的资源模型。
IF 3.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2023.2206012
Jenah Black, Nazbanou Nozari

Working memory (WM) is critical for many cognitive functions including language production. A key feature of WM is its capacity limitation. Two models have been proposed to account for such capacity limitation: slot models and resource models. In recent years, resource models have found support in both visual and auditory perception, but do they also extend to production? We investigate this by analyzing sublexical errors from four individuals with aphasia. Using tools from computational linguistics, we first define the concept of "precision" of sublexical errors. We then demonstrate that such precision decreases with increased working memory load, i.e., word length, as predicted by resource models. Finally, we rule out alternative accounts of this effect, such as articulatory simplification. These data provide the first evidence for the applicability of the resource model to production and further point to the generalizability of this account as a model of resource division in WM.

工作记忆(WM)对包括语言生成在内的许多认知功能至关重要。工作记忆的一个主要特点是容量有限。为了解释这种容量限制,人们提出了两种模型:槽模型和资源模型。近年来,资源模型在视觉和听觉感知中都得到了支持,但它们是否也适用于语言生成呢?我们通过分析四名失语症患者的亚词汇错误来研究这一问题。利用计算语言学的工具,我们首先定义了亚词汇错误的 "精确度 "概念。然后,我们证明这种精确度会随着工作记忆负荷(即单词长度)的增加而降低,正如资源模型所预测的那样。最后,我们排除了这种效应的其他解释,如发音简化。这些数据首次证明了资源模型在生产中的适用性,并进一步证明了该模型作为 WM 中资源分配模型的普遍性。
{"title":"Precision of phonological errors in aphasia supports resource models of phonological working memory in language production.","authors":"Jenah Black, Nazbanou Nozari","doi":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2206012","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02643294.2023.2206012","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Working memory (WM) is critical for many cognitive functions including language production. A key feature of WM is its capacity limitation. Two models have been proposed to account for such capacity limitation: slot models and resource models. In recent years, resource models have found support in both visual and auditory perception, but do they also extend to production? We investigate this by analyzing sublexical errors from four individuals with aphasia. Using tools from computational linguistics, we first define the concept of \"precision\" of sublexical errors. We then demonstrate that such precision decreases with increased working memory load, i.e., word length, as predicted by resource models. Finally, we rule out alternative accounts of this effect, such as articulatory simplification. These data provide the first evidence for the applicability of the resource model to production and further point to the generalizability of this account as a model of resource division in WM.</p>","PeriodicalId":50670,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10336978/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9791577","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Neuropsychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1