首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research最新文献

英文 中文
Revisiting Dysarthria Treatment Across Languages: The Hybrid Approach. 重新审视构音障碍的跨语言治疗:混合方法。
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 Epub Date: 2023-12-06 DOI: 10.1044/2023_JSLHR-23-00629
Erika S Levy, Gemma Moya-Galé

Purpose: Ten years after Miller and Lowit's (2014) groundbreaking book providing a cross-linguistic perspective on motor speech disorders, we ask where we are regarding dysarthria treatment across languages in two specific populations: adults with Parkinson's disease (PD) and children with cerebral palsy (CP).

Method: In this commentary, we consider preliminary evidence for both language-independent and language-specific approaches to treatment and propose a hybrid approach to speech treatment across languages, centered on the individual with dysarthria who speaks any given language.

Conclusions: Treatment research on individuals with dysarthria secondary to PD and CP is advancing, but several areas remain to be explored. Next steps are suggested for addressing the paucity and complexity of cross-linguistic speech treatment research.

目的:在Miller和Lowit(2014)开创性的书提供了跨语言视角的运动语言障碍十年后,我们想知道我们在两个特定人群中跨语言治疗构音障碍的情况:患有帕金森病(PD)的成年人和患有脑瘫(CP)的儿童。方法:在这篇评论中,我们考虑了语言独立和语言特定治疗方法的初步证据,并提出了一种跨语言的混合语言治疗方法,以说任何给定语言的构音障碍患者为中心。结论:PD和CP继发构音障碍个体的治疗研究正在取得进展,但仍有几个领域有待探索。针对跨语言语音治疗研究的缺乏和复杂性,提出了下一步的建议。
{"title":"Revisiting Dysarthria Treatment Across Languages: The Hybrid Approach.","authors":"Erika S Levy, Gemma Moya-Galé","doi":"10.1044/2023_JSLHR-23-00629","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2023_JSLHR-23-00629","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Ten years after Miller and Lowit's (2014) groundbreaking book providing a cross-linguistic perspective on motor speech disorders, we ask where we are regarding dysarthria treatment across languages in two specific populations: adults with Parkinson's disease (PD) and children with cerebral palsy (CP).</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In this commentary, we consider preliminary evidence for both language-independent and language-specific approaches to treatment and propose a hybrid approach to speech treatment across languages, centered on the individual with dysarthria who speaks any given language.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Treatment research on individuals with dysarthria secondary to PD and CP is advancing, but several areas remain to be explored. Next steps are suggested for addressing the paucity and complexity of cross-linguistic speech treatment research.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138500144","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Higher Frequency of Stuttered Disfluencies Negatively Affects Communicative Participation in Parkinson's Disease. 帕金森病患者口吃不流利的频率越高,对其参与交流的负面影响就越大。
IF 2.6 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00018
Eloïse Gooch,Tracy R Melzer,Kyla-Louise Horne,Sophie Grenfell,Leslie Livingston,Toni Pitcher,John C Dalrymple-Alford,Tim J Anderson,Megan J McAuliffe,Catherine Theys
PURPOSEUp to 90% of people with Parkinson's disease (PD) develop communication difficulties over the course of the disease. While the negative effect of dysarthria on communicative participation has been well-documented, the impact of the occurrence of acquired stuttered disfluencies on communication in different speech situations is unknown. This study aimed to determine if the frequency of occurrence of stuttered disfluencies affects communicative participation in individuals with PD, and whether such a relationship is mediated by examiner- and self-rated measures of disease severity.METHODConversational speech samples were collected from 100 people with PD aged 53-91 years to calculate the frequency of occurrence of stuttered disfluencies. Participants completed the Communicative Participation Item Bank to assess participation in communicative situations. Information on overall speech, cognitive, and motor performance was collected using both self-rated and examiner-rated methods.RESULTSParticipants with PD presented with 0.2%-9.9% stuttered disfluencies during conversation. Overall, participants with PD reported their communicative participation to be impacted "a little" (19.5 ± 7.0), but there was considerable interindividual variation. A higher frequency of stuttered disfluencies was associated with significantly lower communicative participation (ρ = -0.32, p < .01). In addition, examiner-rated frequency of stuttered disfluencies (p < .01), speech (p < .01), and motor severity (p = .04) were all significant predictors of communicative participation. Using self-ratings, speech (p < .01) and cognitive (p < .01) measures significantly predicted communicative participation.CONCLUSIONSIn people with PD, communicative participation was significantly worse for those with a higher frequency of stuttered disfluencies. Examiner- and self-rated measures of disease severity contributed different information related to communicative constraints. Together, these results highlight the importance of individualized and holistic speech therapy that considers a wide variety of symptoms, including stuttered disfluencies, to ensure positive functional outcomes.SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALhttps://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26850169.
目的高达90%的帕金森病(PD)患者在患病期间会出现交流障碍。虽然构音障碍对参与交流的负面影响已得到充分证实,但后天出现的口吃不流利现象对不同言语环境中交流的影响尚不清楚。本研究旨在确定口吃不流利的发生频率是否会影响帕金森氏症患者的交流参与,以及这种关系是否会受到检查者和自我疾病严重程度评估的影响。参与者填写了 "交流参与项目库"(Communicative Participation Item Bank),以评估其在交流情境中的参与情况。结果帕金森氏症患者在交谈过程中出现 0.2%-9.9% 口吃不流利的情况。总体而言,患有帕金森氏症的参与者表示他们的交流参与受到了 "一点 "影响(19.5 ± 7.0),但个体间差异很大。口吃不流利的频率越高,参与交流的程度就越低(ρ = -0.32,p < .01)。此外,考官评定的口吃不流利频率(p < .01)、言语(p < .01)和运动严重程度(p = .04)均可显著预测交流参与度。通过自我评分,言语(p < .01)和认知(p < .01)指标对交流参与度有明显的预测作用。检查者和患者自我评定的疾病严重程度提供了与交流限制相关的不同信息。总之,这些结果凸显了个体化和整体语言治疗的重要性,这种治疗应考虑包括口吃不流利在内的各种症状,以确保取得积极的功能性成果。补充材料https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26850169。
{"title":"Higher Frequency of Stuttered Disfluencies Negatively Affects Communicative Participation in Parkinson's Disease.","authors":"Eloïse Gooch,Tracy R Melzer,Kyla-Louise Horne,Sophie Grenfell,Leslie Livingston,Toni Pitcher,John C Dalrymple-Alford,Tim J Anderson,Megan J McAuliffe,Catherine Theys","doi":"10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00018","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00018","url":null,"abstract":"PURPOSEUp to 90% of people with Parkinson's disease (PD) develop communication difficulties over the course of the disease. While the negative effect of dysarthria on communicative participation has been well-documented, the impact of the occurrence of acquired stuttered disfluencies on communication in different speech situations is unknown. This study aimed to determine if the frequency of occurrence of stuttered disfluencies affects communicative participation in individuals with PD, and whether such a relationship is mediated by examiner- and self-rated measures of disease severity.METHODConversational speech samples were collected from 100 people with PD aged 53-91 years to calculate the frequency of occurrence of stuttered disfluencies. Participants completed the Communicative Participation Item Bank to assess participation in communicative situations. Information on overall speech, cognitive, and motor performance was collected using both self-rated and examiner-rated methods.RESULTSParticipants with PD presented with 0.2%-9.9% stuttered disfluencies during conversation. Overall, participants with PD reported their communicative participation to be impacted \"a little\" (19.5 ± 7.0), but there was considerable interindividual variation. A higher frequency of stuttered disfluencies was associated with significantly lower communicative participation (ρ = -0.32, p < .01). In addition, examiner-rated frequency of stuttered disfluencies (p < .01), speech (p < .01), and motor severity (p = .04) were all significant predictors of communicative participation. Using self-ratings, speech (p < .01) and cognitive (p < .01) measures significantly predicted communicative participation.CONCLUSIONSIn people with PD, communicative participation was significantly worse for those with a higher frequency of stuttered disfluencies. Examiner- and self-rated measures of disease severity contributed different information related to communicative constraints. Together, these results highlight the importance of individualized and holistic speech therapy that considers a wide variety of symptoms, including stuttered disfluencies, to ensure positive functional outcomes.SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALhttps://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26850169.","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142268526","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Vigilant Attention During Cognitive and Language Processing in Aphasia. 失语症患者认知和语言处理过程中的警觉注意
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 Epub Date: 2024-08-28 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00168
Dannielle Hibshman, Ellyn A Riley

Purpose: Persons with aphasia (PWA) experience differences in attention after stroke, potentially impacting cognitive/language performance. This secondary analysis investigated physiologically measured vigilant attention during linguistic and nonlinguistic processing in PWA and control participants.

Method: To evaluate performance and attention in a language task, seven PWA read sentences aloud (linguistic task) and were compared to a previous data set of 10 controls and 10 PWA. To evaluate performance and attention in a language-independent task, 11 controls and nine PWA completed the Bivalent Shape Task (nonlinguistic task). Continuous electroencephalogram (EEG) data were collected during each session. A previously validated EEG algorithm classified vigilant-attention state for each experiment trial into high, moderate, distracted, or no attention. Dependent measures were task accuracy and amount of time spent in each attention state (measured by the number of trials).

Results: PWA produced significantly more errors than controls on the linguistic task, but groups performed similarly on the nonlinguistic task. During the linguistic task, controls spent significantly more time than PWA in a moderate-attention state, but no statistically significant differences were found between groups for other attention states. For the nonlinguistic task, amount of time controls and PWA spent in each attention state was more evenly distributed. When directly comparing attention patterns between linguistic and nonlinguistic tasks, PWA showed significantly more time in a high-attention state during the linguistic task as compared to the nonlinguistic task; however, controls showed no significant differences between linguistic and nonlinguistic tasks.

Conclusions: This study provides new evidence that PWA experience a heightened state of vigilant attention when language processing demands are higher (during a linguistic task) than when language demands are lower (during a nonlinguistic task). Collectively, results of this study suggest that when processing language, PWA may allocate more attentional resources than when completing other kinds of cognitive tasks.

目的:失语症患者(PWA)在中风后会出现注意力差异,这可能会影响认知/语言表现。这项二次分析调查了 PWA 和对照组参与者在语言和非语言处理过程中生理测量的警觉注意力:为了评估语言任务中的表现和注意力,7 名 PWA 参与者朗读了句子(语言任务),并与之前由 10 名对照组和 10 名 PWA 参与者组成的数据集进行了比较。为了评估与语言无关的任务中的表现和注意力,11 名对照组和 9 名 PWA 完成了二价形状任务(非语言任务)。每次训练都会收集连续的脑电图(EEG)数据。先前经过验证的脑电图算法将每次实验的警觉-注意力状态分为高度、中度、分散或无注意力。衡量标准是任务准确性和在每种注意状态下花费的时间(以试验次数衡量):结果:在语言任务中,PWA 产生的错误明显多于对照组,但在非语言任务中,两组表现相似。在语言任务中,对照组在中度注意状态下花费的时间明显多于 PWA,但在其他注意状态下,组间差异无统计学意义。在非语言任务中,对照组和 PWA 在每种注意状态下所花费的时间分布较为均匀。在直接比较语言任务和非语言任务之间的注意模式时,与非语言任务相比,PWA 在语言任务中处于高度注意状态的时间明显更长;但是,对照组在语言任务和非语言任务之间没有明显差异:本研究提供了新的证据,表明当语言处理需求较高时(语言任务期间),PWA 的警觉注意状态会比语言需求较低时(非语言任务期间)更强。总之,本研究的结果表明,与完成其他类型的认知任务相比,在处理语言时,PWA 可能会分配更多的注意力资源。
{"title":"Vigilant Attention During Cognitive and Language Processing in Aphasia.","authors":"Dannielle Hibshman, Ellyn A Riley","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00168","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00168","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Persons with aphasia (PWA) experience differences in attention after stroke, potentially impacting cognitive/language performance. This secondary analysis investigated physiologically measured vigilant attention during linguistic and nonlinguistic processing in PWA and control participants.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>To evaluate performance and attention in a language task, seven PWA read sentences aloud (linguistic task) and were compared to a previous data set of 10 controls and 10 PWA. To evaluate performance and attention in a language-independent task, 11 controls and nine PWA completed the Bivalent Shape Task (nonlinguistic task). Continuous electroencephalogram (EEG) data were collected during each session. A previously validated EEG algorithm classified vigilant-attention state for each experiment trial into high, moderate, distracted, or no attention. Dependent measures were task accuracy and amount of time spent in each attention state (measured by the number of trials).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>PWA produced significantly more errors than controls on the linguistic task, but groups performed similarly on the nonlinguistic task. During the linguistic task, controls spent significantly more time than PWA in a moderate-attention state, but no statistically significant differences were found between groups for other attention states. For the nonlinguistic task, amount of time controls and PWA spent in each attention state was more evenly distributed. When directly comparing attention patterns between linguistic and nonlinguistic tasks, PWA showed significantly more time in a high-attention state during the linguistic task as compared to the nonlinguistic task; however, controls showed no significant differences between linguistic and nonlinguistic tasks.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>This study provides new evidence that PWA experience a heightened state of vigilant attention when language processing demands are higher (during a linguistic task) than when language demands are lower (during a nonlinguistic task). Collectively, results of this study suggest that when processing language, PWA may allocate more attentional resources than when completing other kinds of cognitive tasks.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11427425/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142086384","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does Native Language Matter in Perceptual Ratings of Dysarthria? 母语对构音障碍的感知评分重要吗?
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 Epub Date: 2024-04-25 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00668
Yunjung Kim, Austin Thompson, Seung Jin Lee

Purpose: Despite the general agreement that dysarthria characteristics are largely language-independent, few efforts have attempted a systematic comparison across languages. To examine the role of native languages in the perception of speech characteristics of dysarthria secondary to Parkinson's disease (PD), auditory-perceptual ratings of dysarthria, and confidence level of the judgments were compared between two listener groups: language-matched and language-crossed.

Method: A total of 60 listeners (35 native speakers of Korean and 25 native speakers of American English) estimated speech abnormality for 20 speech dimensions using a visual analog scale method for both language-matched and language-crossed speech stimuli. Speech stimuli were passage readings of the respective languages obtained from individuals with and without PD.

Results: For speech dimension ratings, eight of 20 speech dimensions revealed significant differences in response to PD speech between the two listener groups, for most of which, language-crossed listeners' estimation was lower (i.e., more impaired) than language-matched listeners. For confidence-level ratings, language-matched listeners were less confident in the ratings of speakers with PD compared to the language-crossed listeners.

Conclusions: The data support both language-universal and language-specific aspects in perceiving dysarthria characteristics, such that native language plays a role, especially when rating articulatory- and rhythmic-related characteristics. The findings are discussed with respect to the role of linguistic information, such as phonetic inventories and prosodic structures, in perceiving dysarthria characteristics.

目的:尽管普遍认为构音障碍的特征在很大程度上与语言无关,但很少有人尝试对不同语言进行系统比较。为了研究母语在感知继发于帕金森病(PD)的构音障碍语音特征中的作用,我们比较了两组听者(语言匹配组和语言交叉组)对构音障碍的听觉感知评分以及判断的置信度:共有 60 名听者(35 名以韩语为母语,25 名以美式英语为母语)使用视觉模拟量表法对语言匹配和语言交叉的语音刺激的 20 个语音维度进行了语音异常估计。语音刺激是从患有和未患有帕金森病的人那里获得的各自语言的段落朗读:在语音维度评分方面,20 个语音维度中有 8 个维度显示出两组听者对 PD 语音的反应存在显著差异,其中大部分维度,语言交叉听者的估计值低于语言匹配听者(即受损程度更高)。在置信度评分方面,与语言交叉听者相比,语言匹配听者对患有肢体瘫痪的说话者的评分置信度较低:这些数据证明了在感知构音障碍特征时,语言的普遍性和语言的特殊性都起着作用,尤其是在评定发音和节奏相关特征时。研究结果就语言信息(如语音清单和前音结构)在感知构音障碍特征中的作用进行了讨论。
{"title":"Does Native Language Matter in Perceptual Ratings of Dysarthria?","authors":"Yunjung Kim, Austin Thompson, Seung Jin Lee","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00668","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00668","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Despite the general agreement that dysarthria characteristics are largely language-independent, few efforts have attempted a systematic comparison across languages. To examine the role of native languages in the perception of speech characteristics of dysarthria secondary to Parkinson's disease (PD), auditory-perceptual ratings of dysarthria, and confidence level of the judgments were compared between two listener groups: language-matched and language-crossed.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>A total of 60 listeners (35 native speakers of Korean and 25 native speakers of American English) estimated speech abnormality for 20 speech dimensions using a visual analog scale method for both language-matched and language-crossed speech stimuli. Speech stimuli were passage readings of the respective languages obtained from individuals with and without PD.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>For speech dimension ratings, eight of 20 speech dimensions revealed significant differences in response to PD speech between the two listener groups, for most of which, language-crossed listeners' estimation was lower (i.e., more impaired) than language-matched listeners. For confidence-level ratings, language-matched listeners were less confident in the ratings of speakers with PD compared to the language-crossed listeners.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The data support both language-universal and language-specific aspects in perceiving dysarthria characteristics, such that native language plays a role, especially when rating articulatory- and rhythmic-related characteristics. The findings are discussed with respect to the role of linguistic information, such as phonetic inventories and prosodic structures, in perceiving dysarthria characteristics.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11427426/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140861373","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Audiograms and Prevalence of Hearing Loss in U.S. Children and Adolescents 6-19 Years of Age. 美国 6-19 岁儿童和青少年听力图和听力损失患病率。
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 Epub Date: 2024-07-31 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00050
Larry Humes

Purpose: The aim of this study was to determine median audiograms and prevalence of hearing loss for U.S. 6- to 19-year-olds.

Method: U.S. national audiometric data for 2,709 six- to 19-year-olds from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2017-2020 were analyzed. Analyses were primarily descriptive and relied on distribution-free medians, cumulative distribution functions (CDFs), and population-weighted prevalence estimates. Differences between CDFs were examined using Cohen's h effect size. Results are presented for the age range of 6-19 years. Main effects of sex, age, and race/ethnicity were examined.

Results: Differences in the audiograms between the sexes and among the various race/ethnicity groups were minor, but age group differences were notable. Pure-tone thresholds at 500 and 8000 Hz for the 6- to 9-year age group were 3-5 dB higher (worse) than those of the other age groups. The overall prevalence of hearing loss among 6- to 19-year-olds, defined as either of the pure-tone averages (PTAs) for 500, 1000, and 2000 Hz (PTA512) or 3000, 4000, 6000, and 8000 Hz (PTA3468k) exceeding 15 dB HL in either ear, was 11.24% (95% confidence interval [10.00%, 12.48%]). Binary logistic regression analyses found the odds of hearing loss to be significantly higher for individuals from families with a low income as well as for those with abnormal otoscopy or abnormal tympanometry.

Conclusions: For children and adolescents in the United States, hearing was unaffected by differences in sex and race/ethnicity, with only age impacting pure-tone thresholds and only at the lowest (500 Hz) and highest (8000 Hz) frequencies examined. The overall prevalence of hearing loss was about 11%.

目的:本研究旨在确定美国 6 至 19 岁儿童的听力中位数和听力损失患病率:分析了 2017-2020 年全国健康与营养调查中 2709 名 6 至 19 岁儿童的美国全国听力数据。分析主要是描述性的,依赖于无分布中值、累积分布函数(CDF)和人口加权流行率估计值。CDFs 之间的差异采用 Cohen's h效应大小进行检验。结果显示的年龄范围为 6-19 岁。对性别、年龄和种族/族裔的主要影响进行了检验:结果:不同性别和不同种族/族裔群体之间的听力图差异很小,但年龄组差异明显。6-9 岁年龄组在 500 赫兹和 8000 赫兹的纯音阈值比其他年龄组高 3-5 分贝(较差)。6至19岁儿童听力损失的总体患病率为11.24%(95%置信区间[10.00%, 12.48%]),听力损失的定义是任一耳的500、1000和2000赫兹(PTA512)或3000、4000、6000和8000赫兹(PTA3468k)的纯音平均值(PTA)超过15分贝HL。二元逻辑回归分析发现,来自低收入家庭以及耳镜检查异常或鼓室测量异常者的听力损失几率明显更高:美国儿童和青少年的听力不受性别和种族/人种差异的影响,只有年龄会影响纯音阈值,而且只影响最低(500赫兹)和最高(8000赫兹)频率。听力损失的总体发生率约为 11%。
{"title":"Audiograms and Prevalence of Hearing Loss in U.S. Children and Adolescents 6-19 Years of Age.","authors":"Larry Humes","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00050","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00050","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>The aim of this study was to determine median audiograms and prevalence of hearing loss for U.S. 6- to 19-year-olds.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>U.S. national audiometric data for 2,709 six- to 19-year-olds from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2017-2020 were analyzed. Analyses were primarily descriptive and relied on distribution-free medians, cumulative distribution functions (CDFs), and population-weighted prevalence estimates. Differences between CDFs were examined using Cohen's <i>h</i> effect size. Results are presented for the age range of 6-19 years. Main effects of sex, age, and race/ethnicity were examined.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Differences in the audiograms between the sexes and among the various race/ethnicity groups were minor, but age group differences were notable. Pure-tone thresholds at 500 and 8000 Hz for the 6- to 9-year age group were 3-5 dB higher (worse) than those of the other age groups. The overall prevalence of hearing loss among 6- to 19-year-olds, defined as either of the pure-tone averages (PTAs) for 500, 1000, and 2000 Hz (PTA512) or 3000, 4000, 6000, and 8000 Hz (PTA3468k) exceeding 15 dB HL in either ear, was 11.24% (95% confidence interval [10.00%, 12.48%]). Binary logistic regression analyses found the odds of hearing loss to be significantly higher for individuals from families with a low income as well as for those with abnormal otoscopy or abnormal tympanometry.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>For children and adolescents in the United States, hearing was unaffected by differences in sex and race/ethnicity, with only age impacting pure-tone thresholds and only at the lowest (500 Hz) and highest (8000 Hz) frequencies examined. The overall prevalence of hearing loss was about 11%.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141861621","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring the Activation of Target Words in Picture Naming in Children Who Stutter: Evidence From Event-Related Potentials. 探索口吃儿童在图片命名中目标词的激活:来自事件相关电位的证据
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 Epub Date: 2024-07-26 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00570
Nathan D Maxfield

Purpose: Target word activation in picture naming was explored in children who stutter (CWS) and typically fluent children (TFC) using event-related potentials (ERPs).

Method: A total of 18 CWS and 16 TFC completed a task combining picture naming and probe word identification. On each trial, a picture-to-be-named was followed by an auditory probe word-to-be-identified; the probe was identical (Identity condition) or Unrelated to the picture name. ERPs were recorded from probe onset. Attenuation of the N400 ERP component was predicted to Identity versus Unrelated trials (N400 priming). Between-groups differences in amplitude, timing, and topography of N400 priming were explored.

Results: Naming was more accurate on Identity versus Unrelated trials. Probe word identification accuracy was not affected by Condition. N400 priming was detected, indicating that self-generated picture names facilitated semantic processing of identical probes. This effect was larger in amplitude in CWS versus TFC. Unexpectedly, an N400-preceding, frontally maximal, positive-going ERP component-associated with expectancy processing-was larger in amplitude to Unrelated versus Identity trials. This effect was smaller in CWS versus TFC.

Conclusions: A larger N400 priming effect in CWS versus TFC reflects a tendency toward more extensive semantic processing in picture naming in CWS. A smaller Condition effect on frontally maximal, positive-going, N400-preceding ERP activity in CWS versus TFC indicates a reduced ability to form expectancies about the lexical and/or phonological identity of probe words in CWS. Both effects may point to inefficient activation of target words in picture naming in CWS.

目的:使用事件相关电位(ERPs)研究口吃儿童(CWS)和典型流利儿童(TFC)在图片命名中的目标词激活情况:共有 18 名 CWS 和 16 名 TFC 完成了一项结合图片命名和探测词识别的任务。在每次试验中,一幅待命名的图片之后是一个待识别的听觉探究词;探究词与图片名称相同(同一条件)或不相关。从探针开始时记录 ERP。N400ERP分量的衰减被预测为 "同一性 "与 "非相关性 "试验(N400引物)。研究探讨了 N400 引物在振幅、时间和地形方面的组间差异:结果:在 "同一性 "与 "非相关性 "试验中,命名更准确。探究词识别的准确性不受条件影响。检测到了 N400 引物,表明自创图片名称促进了对相同试题的语义处理。这种效应在 CWS 和 TFC 中的振幅更大。出乎意料的是,与期望处理相关的 N400 前导、前部最大、正向 ERP 成分在无关试验与身份试验中的振幅更大。这种效应在 CWS 与 TFC 中较小:结论:CWS与TFC相比,N400引物效应更大,这反映出CWS在图片命名中倾向于进行更广泛的语义加工。CWS与TFC相比,前部最大、正向、N400前导ERP活动的条件效应较小,这表明CWS对探究词的词性和/或语音特征形成预期的能力较弱。这两种效应都可能表明 CWS 在图片命名中对目标词的激活效率较低。
{"title":"Exploring the Activation of Target Words in Picture Naming in Children Who Stutter: Evidence From Event-Related Potentials.","authors":"Nathan D Maxfield","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00570","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00570","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Target word activation in picture naming was explored in children who stutter (CWS) and typically fluent children (TFC) using event-related potentials (ERPs).</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>A total of 18 CWS and 16 TFC completed a task combining picture naming and probe word identification. On each trial, a picture-to-be-named was followed by an auditory probe word-to-be-identified; the probe was identical (Identity condition) or Unrelated to the picture name. ERPs were recorded from probe onset. Attenuation of the N400 ERP component was predicted to Identity versus Unrelated trials (N400 priming). Between-groups differences in amplitude, timing, and topography of N400 priming were explored.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Naming was more accurate on Identity versus Unrelated trials. Probe word identification accuracy was not affected by Condition. N400 priming was detected, indicating that self-generated picture names facilitated semantic processing of identical probes. This effect was larger in amplitude in CWS versus TFC. Unexpectedly, an N400-preceding, frontally maximal, positive-going ERP component-associated with expectancy processing-was larger in amplitude to Unrelated versus Identity trials. This effect was smaller in CWS versus TFC.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>A larger N400 priming effect in CWS versus TFC reflects a tendency toward more extensive semantic processing in picture naming in CWS. A smaller Condition effect on frontally maximal, positive-going, N400-preceding ERP activity in CWS versus TFC indicates a reduced ability to form expectancies about the lexical and/or phonological identity of probe words in CWS. Both effects may point to inefficient activation of target words in picture naming in CWS.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11427420/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141768002","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evaluating Stuttering Self-Stigma and Its Relationship to Adverse Impact in Children and Adolescents With the Child Stuttering Self-Stigma Scale. 用儿童口吃自我耻辱感量表评估儿童和青少年的口吃自我耻辱感及其与不良影响的关系。
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 Epub Date: 2024-08-14 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00069
Chelsea A Johnson, Katelyn L Gerwin, Seth E Tichenor, Michael P Boyle, Bridget Walsh

Purpose: Self-stigma occurs when a person internalizes and applies stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination to themselves. For adults who stutter, self-stigma is linked to negative outcomes and reduced quality of life. The development of self-stigma in people who stutter is not well understood. The aim of this study is to evaluate stuttering self-stigma in school-age children and adolescents and explore potential relationships to stuttering's overall adverse impact.

Method: One hundred one children and adolescents who stutter, aged 10-18 years, completed the Overall Assessment of the Speaker's Experience of Stuttering (OASES), a measure of adverse impact related to stuttering, and the Childhood Self-Stigma of Stuttering Scale (Child 4S), our novel adapted version of the Self-Stigma of Stuttering Scale (4S) created for this study. The Child 4S comprises three subscales measuring three stages of self-stigma: Awareness, Agreement, and Application. Each stage was evaluated for relationships with child age and the adverse impact of stuttering.

Results: We found a range of self-stigma scores among children and adolescents who stutter. Child age did not correlate with Awareness and Agreement; however, older children and adolescents reported greater Application. All stages of self-stigma strongly predicted adverse impact as measured by the OASES, and latter stages of the model were stronger predictors than earlier stages.

Conclusions: Children as young as 10 years old may experience stuttering self-stigma, and the application of self-stigma increases in adolescence, a critical period in the development of personal identity. Importantly, all stages of self-stigma predicted adverse impact related to stuttering, with latter stages being stronger predictors than earlier ones consistent with the progressive model of self-stigma being tested. The findings highlight the need for targeted, early intervention to mitigate downstream effects of stuttering self-stigma.

Supplemental material: https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26352556.

目的:当一个人将刻板印象、偏见和歧视内化并应用于自身时,就会产生自我污名。对于成人口吃患者来说,自我烙印与负面结果和生活质量下降有关。口吃患者自我污名的形成原因尚不十分清楚。本研究的目的是评估学龄儿童和青少年的口吃自我烙印,并探讨其与口吃的整体不良影响之间的潜在关系:100名年龄在10-18岁之间的口吃儿童和青少年完成了 "说话者口吃经历总体评估"(OASES)和 "儿童口吃自我耻辱感量表"(Childhood Self-Stigma of Stuttering Scale,简称Child 4S)。儿童 4S 由三个分量表组成,分别测量自我污名的三个阶段:意识、同意和应用。我们对每个阶段与儿童年龄和口吃不良影响的关系进行了评估:结果:我们发现,口吃儿童和青少年的自我污名得分有高有低。儿童的年龄与 "认识 "和 "认同 "没有关联;但是,年龄较大的儿童和青少年报告的 "应用 "较多。所有阶段的自我污名都能强烈预测由 OASES 测定的不良影响,而模型的后一阶段比前一阶段的预测性更强:结论:年仅 10 岁的儿童就可能经历口吃自我烙印,而在青春期,自我烙印的应用会增加,而青春期正是个人身份发展的关键时期。重要的是,所有阶段的自我鄙视都能预测与口吃有关的不良影响,后一阶段比前一阶段更强,这与正在测试的自我鄙视渐进模型一致。研究结果突出表明,有必要进行有针对性的早期干预,以减轻口吃自我污名的下游影响。补充材料:https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26352556。
{"title":"Evaluating Stuttering Self-Stigma and Its Relationship to Adverse Impact in Children and Adolescents With the Child Stuttering Self-Stigma Scale.","authors":"Chelsea A Johnson, Katelyn L Gerwin, Seth E Tichenor, Michael P Boyle, Bridget Walsh","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00069","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00069","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Self-stigma occurs when a person internalizes and applies stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination to themselves. For adults who stutter, self-stigma is linked to negative outcomes and reduced quality of life. The development of self-stigma in people who stutter is not well understood. The aim of this study is to evaluate stuttering self-stigma in school-age children and adolescents and explore potential relationships to stuttering's overall adverse impact.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>One hundred one children and adolescents who stutter, aged 10-18 years, completed the Overall Assessment of the Speaker's Experience of Stuttering (OASES), a measure of adverse impact related to stuttering, and the Childhood Self-Stigma of Stuttering Scale (Child 4S), our novel adapted version of the Self-Stigma of Stuttering Scale (4S) created for this study. The Child 4S comprises three subscales measuring three stages of self-stigma: Awareness, Agreement, and Application. Each stage was evaluated for relationships with child age and the adverse impact of stuttering.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>We found a range of self-stigma scores among children and adolescents who stutter. Child age did not correlate with Awareness and Agreement; however, older children and adolescents reported greater Application. All stages of self-stigma strongly predicted adverse impact as measured by the OASES, and latter stages of the model were stronger predictors than earlier stages.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Children as young as 10 years old may experience stuttering self-stigma, and the application of self-stigma increases in adolescence, a critical period in the development of personal identity. Importantly, all stages of self-stigma predicted adverse impact related to stuttering, with latter stages being stronger predictors than earlier ones consistent with the progressive model of self-stigma being tested. The findings highlight the need for targeted, early intervention to mitigate downstream effects of stuttering self-stigma.</p><p><strong>Supplemental material: </strong>https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26352556.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11427442/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141983829","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contributions of Speech Timing and Articulatory Precision to Listener Perceptions of Intelligibility and Naturalness in Parkinson's Disease. 帕金森病患者的语音计时和发音精确度对听者可理解性和自然度感知的贡献。
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 Epub Date: 2024-08-08 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00802
Saul A Frankford, Alex Estrada, Cara E Stepp

Purpose: Parkinson's disease (PD) results in hypokinetic dysarthria in as many as 90% of cases. Among the most distinctive features of hypokinetic dysarthria are atypical timing and articulatory imprecision in speech production. Here, we examined the contributions of perceived speech timing typicality and articulatory precision, both on their own and while controlling for the other, on intelligibility and naturalness in speakers with PD.

Method: Twenty speakers with PD and four healthy older adults read aloud the first paragraph of the Rainbow Passage. Twenty inexperienced listeners with typical hearing listened to these recordings and rated intelligibility, naturalness, timing typicality, and articulatory precision using separate visual analog scales. Ratings were averaged across listeners and entered into linear regression models with intelligibility and naturalness as dependent variables and timing typicality and articulatory precision as independent variables in each.

Results: Articulatory precision, but not timing typicality, was positively correlated with intelligibility on its own, but neither was associated with intelligibility after accounting for the other. Both timing typicality and articulatory precision were positively correlated with naturalness on their own as well as after controlling for the other variable.

Conclusion: These results contribute to the overall understanding of speech factors associated with intelligibility and naturalness in speakers with PD and indicate that considering the unique contributions of related perceptual constructs may provide more information than bivariate relationships alone.

目的:多达 90% 的帕金森病(PD)患者会出现运动功能减退性构音障碍。构音不足最显著的特征是说话的时间不典型和发音不准确。在此,我们研究了感知到的语音时间典型性和发音精确性对患有帕金森氏症的说话者的可懂度和自然度的影响:方法:20 名患有帕金森氏症的演讲者和 4 名健康的老年人朗读《彩虹传》的第一段。二十名听力正常但缺乏经验的听众聆听了这些录音,并使用不同的视觉模拟量表对可懂度、自然度、时间典型性和发音精确度进行评分。对所有听者的评分进行平均,并将其输入线性回归模型,将可懂度和自然度作为因变量,将时间典型性和发音精确度作为自变量:结果:发音精确度本身与可懂度呈正相关,但时间典型性与可懂度不呈正相关。时间典型性和发音精确度本身都与自然度呈正相关,在控制了其他变量后也是如此:这些结果有助于全面了解与帕金森病患者的可懂度和自然度相关的语音因素,并表明考虑相关知觉建构的独特贡献可能比单独的二元关系提供更多信息。
{"title":"Contributions of Speech Timing and Articulatory Precision to Listener Perceptions of Intelligibility and Naturalness in Parkinson's Disease.","authors":"Saul A Frankford, Alex Estrada, Cara E Stepp","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00802","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00802","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Parkinson's disease (PD) results in hypokinetic dysarthria in as many as 90% of cases. Among the most distinctive features of hypokinetic dysarthria are atypical timing and articulatory imprecision in speech production. Here, we examined the contributions of perceived speech timing typicality and articulatory precision, both on their own and while controlling for the other, on intelligibility and naturalness in speakers with PD.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Twenty speakers with PD and four healthy older adults read aloud the first paragraph of the Rainbow Passage. Twenty inexperienced listeners with typical hearing listened to these recordings and rated intelligibility, naturalness, timing typicality, and articulatory precision using separate visual analog scales. Ratings were averaged across listeners and entered into linear regression models with intelligibility and naturalness as dependent variables and timing typicality and articulatory precision as independent variables in each.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Articulatory precision, but not timing typicality, was positively correlated with intelligibility on its own, but neither was associated with intelligibility after accounting for the other. Both timing typicality and articulatory precision were positively correlated with naturalness on their own as well as after controlling for the other variable.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>These results contribute to the overall understanding of speech factors associated with intelligibility and naturalness in speakers with PD and indicate that considering the unique contributions of related perceptual constructs may provide more information than bivariate relationships alone.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11427422/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141908224","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Determinants of Multilevel Discourse Outcomes in Anomia Treatment for Aphasia. 失语症失认症治疗中多层次话语结果的决定因素。
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 Epub Date: 2024-08-15 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00030
Robert Cavanaugh, Michael Walsh Dickey, William D Hula, Davida Fromm, Jennifer Golovin, Julie Wambaugh, Gerasimos Fergadiotis, William S Evans

Purpose: Individuals with aphasia identify discourse-level communication (i.e., language in use) as a high priority for treatment. The central premise of most aphasia treatments is that restoring language at the phoneme, word, and/or sentence level will generalize to discourse. However, treatment-related changes in discourse-level communication are modest, are poorly understood, and vary greatly among individuals with aphasia. In response, this study consisted of a multilevel discourse analysis of archival, monologic discourse outcomes across two high-intensity Semantic Feature Analysis (SFA) clinical trials. Aim 1 evaluated changes in theoretically motivated discourse outcomes representing lexical-semantic processing, lexical diversity, grammatical complexity, and discourse informativeness. Aim 2 explored the potential moderating role of nonlanguage cognitive factors (semantic memory, divided attention, and executive function) on discourse outcomes.

Method: This study was a retrospective analysis of archival monologic discourse outcomes after intensive SFA for n = 60 (Aim 1) and a subset n = 44 (Aim 2). Outcome measures included lexical-semantic processing (% semantic errors), lexical diversity (moving average type-token ratio), grammatical complexity (mean utterance length), and discourse informativeness (% correct information units). Bayesian generalized mixed-effects models were used to examine changes across four study time points: enrollment, entry, exit, and 1-month follow-up.

Results: The present study found no evidence for meaningful or statistically reliable improvements in monologue discourse performance after SFA when measured using standard, general-topic discourse stimuli. There was weak and inconsistent evidence that nonlanguage cognitive factors may play a role in moderating treatment response.

Conclusions: These findings indicate a clear need to pair theoretically informed treatments designed to facilitate generalization to discourse with intentional measurement paradigms designed to capture it. Furthermore, there is a clear need to examine how established treatments, restorative or compensatory, can better facilitate generalization to discourse-level communication. These priorities are critical for meaningfully improving everyday communication and reducing the profound communication and psychosocial consequences of aphasia.

Supplemental material: https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26524081.

目的:失语症患者认为话语层面的交流(即使用中的语言)是治疗的重中之重。大多数失语症治疗方法的核心前提是,恢复音素、单词和/或句子层面的语言能力将推广到话语层面。然而,与治疗相关的话语水平交流变化不大,人们对其了解甚少,而且不同的失语症患者之间差异很大。为此,本研究对两项高强度语义特征分析(SFA)临床试验中的档案、单一话语结果进行了多层次话语分析。研究目的 1 评估了代表词汇-语义加工、词汇多样性、语法复杂性和话语信息性的理论性话语结果的变化。目的 2 探讨非语言认知因素(语义记忆、注意力分散和执行功能)对话语结果的潜在调节作用:本研究是对强化 SFA 后的单语话语结果档案进行的回顾性分析,研究对象为 n = 60(目标 1)和一个子集 n = 44(目标 2)。结果测量包括词汇-语义处理(语义错误百分比)、词汇多样性(移动平均类型-标记词比率)、语法复杂性(平均语篇长度)和话语信息性(正确信息单位百分比)。贝叶斯广义混合效应模型用于研究四个研究时间点的变化:入学、入职、离职和 1 个月的随访:本研究发现,在使用标准、一般主题的话语刺激进行测量时,没有证据表明 SFA 后独白话语的表现得到了有意义或统计上可靠的提高。有微弱且不一致的证据表明,非语言认知因素可能对治疗反应起调节作用:这些研究结果表明,显然有必要将旨在促进语篇泛化的有理论依据的治疗方法与旨在捕捉语篇泛化的有意测量范式相结合。此外,显然有必要研究现有的恢复性或补偿性治疗方法如何能更好地促进泛化到话语层面的交流。这些优先事项对于切实改善日常交流、减少失语症对交流和社会心理造成的深远影响至关重要。补充材料:https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26524081。
{"title":"Determinants of Multilevel Discourse Outcomes in Anomia Treatment for Aphasia.","authors":"Robert Cavanaugh, Michael Walsh Dickey, William D Hula, Davida Fromm, Jennifer Golovin, Julie Wambaugh, Gerasimos Fergadiotis, William S Evans","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00030","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00030","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Individuals with aphasia identify discourse-level communication (i.e., language in use) as a high priority for treatment. The central premise of most aphasia treatments is that restoring language at the phoneme, word, and/or sentence level will generalize to discourse. However, treatment-related changes in discourse-level communication are modest, are poorly understood, and vary greatly among individuals with aphasia. In response, this study consisted of a multilevel discourse analysis of archival, monologic discourse outcomes across two high-intensity Semantic Feature Analysis (SFA) clinical trials. Aim 1 evaluated changes in theoretically motivated discourse outcomes representing lexical-semantic processing, lexical diversity, grammatical complexity, and discourse informativeness. Aim 2 explored the potential moderating role of nonlanguage cognitive factors (semantic memory, divided attention, and executive function) on discourse outcomes.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>This study was a retrospective analysis of archival monologic discourse outcomes after intensive SFA for <i>n</i> = 60 (Aim 1) and a subset <i>n</i> = 44 (Aim 2). Outcome measures included lexical-semantic processing (% semantic errors), lexical diversity (moving average type-token ratio), grammatical complexity (mean utterance length), and discourse informativeness (% correct information units). Bayesian generalized mixed-effects models were used to examine changes across four study time points: enrollment, entry, exit, and 1-month follow-up.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The present study found no evidence for meaningful or statistically reliable improvements in monologue discourse performance after SFA when measured using standard, general-topic discourse stimuli. There was weak and inconsistent evidence that nonlanguage cognitive factors may play a role in moderating treatment response.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>These findings indicate a clear need to pair theoretically informed treatments designed to facilitate generalization to discourse with intentional measurement paradigms designed to capture it. Furthermore, there is a clear need to examine how established treatments, restorative or compensatory, can better facilitate generalization to discourse-level communication. These priorities are critical for meaningfully improving everyday communication and reducing the profound communication and psychosocial consequences of aphasia.</p><p><strong>Supplemental material: </strong>https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26524081.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11427423/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141989503","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Characteristics of Speech Auditory Brainstem Response in Preschool Children With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. 患有注意力缺陷/多动症的学龄前儿童的言语听觉脑干反应特征。
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 Epub Date: 2024-07-31 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00454
Yuying Sun, Jia Zhou, Huiqin Zhu, Panting Liu, Huanxi Lin, Zhenglu Xiao, Xinyue Yu, Jun Qian, Meiling Tong, Xia Chi, Qin Hong

Objective: This study aimed to investigate the characteristics of auditory processing (AP) in preschool children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) using the speech auditory brainstem response (speech-ABR), which provides insights into the AP of speech signals in the central auditory nervous system (CANS).

Method: A total of 84 preschool children diagnosed with ADHD, aged 4-6 years, were matched with 84 typically developing (TD) children based on gender and age. All children underwent speech-ABR testing, cognitive assessment using the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Fourth Edition or the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition, and a continuous performance test.

Results: Children with ADHD exhibited significantly longer latencies of speech-ABR waveforms V, A, and D compared to TD children. Multiple linear regression analysis showed that the latencies of speech-ABR waves V, A, and D were affected by the presence of ADHD, but not by the full-scale intelligence quotient.

Conclusions: This study revealed that preschool children with ADHD exhibited abnormal AP of speech signals in their CANS. The findings suggest that speech-ABR can be utilized as a reliable measure to evaluate AP ability in this population, as it remains unaffected by cognitive or attentional factors. The transient response (V, A) of speech-ABR was found to be a significant predictor of ADHD in a clinical setting. Early assessment of AP abnormalities via speech-ABR is recommended in preschool-age children to develop targeted interventions for ADHD.

Supplemental material: https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26376502.

研究目的本研究旨在利用言语听觉脑干反应(speech-ABR)调查学龄前注意力缺陷/多动症(ADHD)儿童的听觉处理(AP)特征:方法:根据性别和年龄,将 84 名被诊断为多动症的 4-6 岁学龄前儿童与 84 名发育正常(TD)儿童进行配对。所有儿童都接受了言语-ABR测试、使用韦氏学前和小学智力量表第四版或韦氏儿童智力量表第四版进行的认知评估以及连续性表现测试:结果:与TD儿童相比,ADHD儿童的言语-ABR波形V、A和D的潜伏期明显较长。多元线性回归分析表明,言语-ABR波形V、A和D的潜伏期受多动症的影响,但不受全面智商的影响:本研究揭示了学龄前多动症儿童在其 CANS 中表现出异常的言语信号 AP。研究结果表明,言语-ABR 可以作为评估该人群 AP 能力的可靠方法,因为它不受认知或注意力因素的影响。研究发现,在临床环境中,言语-ABR 的瞬态反应(V、A)可显著预测多动症。建议通过言语-ABR对学龄前儿童的 AP 异常进行早期评估,以制定有针对性的多动症干预措施。补充材料:https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26376502。
{"title":"Characteristics of Speech Auditory Brainstem Response in Preschool Children With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder.","authors":"Yuying Sun, Jia Zhou, Huiqin Zhu, Panting Liu, Huanxi Lin, Zhenglu Xiao, Xinyue Yu, Jun Qian, Meiling Tong, Xia Chi, Qin Hong","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00454","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00454","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This study aimed to investigate the characteristics of auditory processing (AP) in preschool children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) using the speech auditory brainstem response (speech-ABR), which provides insights into the AP of speech signals in the central auditory nervous system (CANS).</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>A total of 84 preschool children diagnosed with ADHD, aged 4-6 years, were matched with 84 typically developing (TD) children based on gender and age. All children underwent speech-ABR testing, cognitive assessment using the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Fourth Edition or the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition, and a continuous performance test.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Children with ADHD exhibited significantly longer latencies of speech-ABR waveforms V, A, and D compared to TD children. Multiple linear regression analysis showed that the latencies of speech-ABR waves V, A, and D were affected by the presence of ADHD, but not by the full-scale intelligence quotient.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>This study revealed that preschool children with ADHD exhibited abnormal AP of speech signals in their CANS. The findings suggest that speech-ABR can be utilized as a reliable measure to evaluate AP ability in this population, as it remains unaffected by cognitive or attentional factors. The transient response (V, A) of speech-ABR was found to be a significant predictor of ADHD in a clinical setting. Early assessment of AP abnormalities via speech-ABR is recommended in preschool-age children to develop targeted interventions for ADHD.</p><p><strong>Supplemental material: </strong>https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26376502.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141861622","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1