首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research最新文献

英文 中文
A Randomized Controlled Trial of Treatment Distribution and Biofeedback Effects on Speech Production in School-Age Children With Apraxia of Speech. 治疗分布和生物反馈对学龄期言语失调症儿童言语产生影响的随机对照试验。
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-26 Epub Date: 2023-08-23 DOI: 10.1044/2023_JSLHR-22-00622
Jonathan L Preston, Nicole F Caballero, Megan C Leece, Dongliang Wang, Benedette M Herbst, Nina R Benway

Purpose: This study examines how ultrasound biofeedback and intensive treatment distribution affect speech sound generalization during an evidence-based treatment, Speech Motor Chaining, for children with persisting speech errors associated with childhood apraxia of speech (CAS).

Method: In a 2 × 2 factorial randomized controlled trial, children ages 9-17 years meeting CAS criteria were randomized to receive (a) a distributed treatment (20 sessions twice weekly over 10 weeks) or intensive treatment (20 hr in 5 weeks, with 10 hr in Week 1) and (b) treatment with or without biofeedback. Due to the COVID pandemic, some participants were randomized to distributed/intensive telepractice treatment only. The primary outcome was percent target sounds correct on untreated phrases (i.e., generalization) at the 10-week time point. More than 50,000 narrow phonetic transcriptions were analyzed.

Results: Forty-eight participants completed treatment. Intensive treatment significantly increased generalization at all time points. The effect of biofeedback was significant at 5 weeks from the start of treatment but not significant at the primary 10-week time point. However, when comparing each group immediately after their 20 hr of treatment finished, generalization was significantly greater in intensive over distributed treatment and greater in ultrasound over no-ultrasound treatment (with a significant interaction favoring intensive treatment with ultrasound). Only the advantage of intensive treatment remained significant 5 weeks after groups finished treatment. There was no significant difference between face-to-face and telepractice modalities.

Conclusions: When the number of treatment hours is fixed, an intensive schedule of Speech Motor Chaining facilitated greater improvement than a distributed schedule. Ultrasound biofeedback initially accelerated learning, but the benefits may dissipate as treatment continues or after it ends.

目的:本研究探讨了超声生物反馈和强化治疗分布如何影响基于证据的治疗过程中的语音概括,即语音运动链,针对与儿童言语失用症(CAS)相关的持续言语错误儿童。方法:在一项2×2因子随机对照试验中,符合CAS标准的9-17岁儿童随机接受(a)分布式治疗(10周内每周20次)或强化治疗(5周20小时,第1周10小时)和(b)有或无生物反馈治疗。由于新冠肺炎疫情,一些参与者被随机分配到仅接受分布式/强化远程实践治疗。主要结果是在10周时间点,未经治疗的短语(即泛化)的目标发音正确率为%。分析了50000多个窄语音转录。结果:48名参与者完成了治疗。强化治疗显著提高了所有时间点的泛化能力。生物反馈的效果在治疗开始后5周显著,但在最初的10周时间点不显著。然而,当比较每组在20小时治疗结束后立即进行的比较时,强化超分散治疗的泛化能力明显更强,超声治疗的泛化力明显更强(有利于超声强化治疗的显著相互作用)。只有强化治疗的优势在各组完成治疗5周后仍然显著。面对面和远程实践方式之间没有显著差异。结论:当治疗小时数固定时,密集的言语运动链时间表比分散的时间表有助于更大的改善。超声波生物反馈最初加速了学习,但随着治疗的继续或结束,其益处可能会消失。
{"title":"A Randomized Controlled Trial of Treatment Distribution and Biofeedback Effects on Speech Production in School-Age Children With Apraxia of Speech.","authors":"Jonathan L Preston, Nicole F Caballero, Megan C Leece, Dongliang Wang, Benedette M Herbst, Nina R Benway","doi":"10.1044/2023_JSLHR-22-00622","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2023_JSLHR-22-00622","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>This study examines how ultrasound biofeedback and intensive treatment distribution affect speech sound generalization during an evidence-based treatment, Speech Motor Chaining, for children with persisting speech errors associated with childhood apraxia of speech (CAS).</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In a 2 × 2 factorial randomized controlled trial, children ages 9-17 years meeting CAS criteria were randomized to receive (a) a distributed treatment (20 sessions twice weekly over 10 weeks) or intensive treatment (20 hr in 5 weeks, with 10 hr in Week 1) and (b) treatment with or without biofeedback. Due to the COVID pandemic, some participants were randomized to distributed/intensive telepractice treatment only. The primary outcome was percent target sounds correct on untreated phrases (i.e., generalization) at the 10-week time point. More than 50,000 narrow phonetic transcriptions were analyzed.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Forty-eight participants completed treatment. Intensive treatment significantly increased generalization at all time points. The effect of biofeedback was significant at 5 weeks from the start of treatment but not significant at the primary 10-week time point. However, when comparing each group immediately after their 20 hr of treatment finished, generalization was significantly greater in intensive over distributed treatment and greater in ultrasound over no-ultrasound treatment (with a significant interaction favoring intensive treatment with ultrasound). Only the advantage of intensive treatment remained significant 5 weeks after groups finished treatment. There was no significant difference between face-to-face and telepractice modalities.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>When the number of treatment hours is fixed, an intensive schedule of Speech Motor Chaining facilitated greater improvement than a distributed schedule. Ultrasound biofeedback initially accelerated learning, but the benefits may dissipate as treatment continues or after it ends.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"3414-3436"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10061459","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Tipping the Scales: Indiscriminate Use of Interval Scales to Rate Diverse Dysarthric Features. 天平倾斜:不加区分地使用区间量表来评定各种肢体畸形特征。
IF 2.6 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-18 DOI: 10.1044/2024_jslhr-23-00785
Kaila L Stipancic,Brooke-Mai Whelan,Lauren Laur,Yunxin Zhao,Andrea Rohl,Inyong Choi,Mili Kuruvilla-Dugdale
PURPOSEError related to incorrect use of rating scales is problematic in the assessment and treatment of dysarthria. The main purpose of this project was to determine scale fit for cardinal speech features of hypokinetic dysarthria. A secondary aim was to determine rater reliability for the two different scales explored.METHODForty-three speakers with Parkinson's disease (PD) and 25 neurologically healthy control talkers were recorded reading sentences from the Speech Intelligibility Test. Twenty-two healthy female listeners used both an equal appearing interval (EAI) scale and a direct magnitude estimation (DME) scale to rate five perceptual speech features (i.e., overall speech severity, articulatory imprecision, reduced loudness, short rushes of speech, and monotony) from these recordings. Regression analyses were used to determine the linearity of the relationship between the means of the EAI and DME ratings. Inter- and intrarater reliability was calculated using intraclass correlation coefficients and Spearman's correlation coefficients, respectively, for both EAI and DME ratings.RESULTSThere was a linear relationship between EAI and DME means for monotony, indicating it is a metathetic dimension. Curvilinear relationships were observed between the EAI and DME means for the other four features, indicating prothetic dimensions. Intra- and interrater reliability values were similar for EAI and DME ratings.DISCUSSIONOverall, results of this work suggest that DME is the best fit for scaling several hypokinetic dysarthria features, and not the conventionally used EAI scale. Prothetic dimensions best scaled by DME include overall speech severity, articulatory imprecision, reduced loudness, and short rushes of speech. Monotony was the only feature found to be a metathetic dimension and would be best scaled using EAI or DME. Findings call for rethinking the widespread use of EAI scales for rating perceptual features as part of the assessment and treatment of motor speech disorders.
目的在构音障碍的评估和治疗过程中,与不正确使用评分量表有关的错误是个问题。本项目的主要目的是确定量表与构音障碍的主要言语特征是否匹配。方法录制了 43 名患有帕金森病 (PD) 的说话者和 25 名神经系统健康的对照组说话者朗读语音可懂度测试中的句子。22 名健康女性听者使用等效出现间隔 (EAI) 量表和直接幅度估计 (DME) 量表对这些录音中的五种言语感知特征(即总体言语严重程度、发音不准确、响度降低、言语短促和单调)进行评分。回归分析用于确定 EAI 和 DME 评级平均值之间的线性关系。使用类内相关系数和斯皮尔曼相关系数分别计算了 EAI 和 DME 评级的类间和类内可靠性。在其他四个特征的 EAI 和 DME 平均值之间观察到曲线关系,这表明是移觉维度。总之,这项研究的结果表明,DME 最适合对几种运动不足性构音障碍特征进行标度,而不是传统使用的 EAI 量表。最适合使用 DME 量表的合成维度包括总体言语严重程度、发音不准确、响度降低和言语短促。单调性是唯一一个被发现属于元感觉维度的特征,最好使用 EAI 或 DME 进行评分。研究结果要求重新考虑广泛使用 EAI 量表来评定感知特征,作为运动性言语障碍评估和治疗的一部分。
{"title":"Tipping the Scales: Indiscriminate Use of Interval Scales to Rate Diverse Dysarthric Features.","authors":"Kaila L Stipancic,Brooke-Mai Whelan,Lauren Laur,Yunxin Zhao,Andrea Rohl,Inyong Choi,Mili Kuruvilla-Dugdale","doi":"10.1044/2024_jslhr-23-00785","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/2024_jslhr-23-00785","url":null,"abstract":"PURPOSEError related to incorrect use of rating scales is problematic in the assessment and treatment of dysarthria. The main purpose of this project was to determine scale fit for cardinal speech features of hypokinetic dysarthria. A secondary aim was to determine rater reliability for the two different scales explored.METHODForty-three speakers with Parkinson's disease (PD) and 25 neurologically healthy control talkers were recorded reading sentences from the Speech Intelligibility Test. Twenty-two healthy female listeners used both an equal appearing interval (EAI) scale and a direct magnitude estimation (DME) scale to rate five perceptual speech features (i.e., overall speech severity, articulatory imprecision, reduced loudness, short rushes of speech, and monotony) from these recordings. Regression analyses were used to determine the linearity of the relationship between the means of the EAI and DME ratings. Inter- and intrarater reliability was calculated using intraclass correlation coefficients and Spearman's correlation coefficients, respectively, for both EAI and DME ratings.RESULTSThere was a linear relationship between EAI and DME means for monotony, indicating it is a metathetic dimension. Curvilinear relationships were observed between the EAI and DME means for the other four features, indicating prothetic dimensions. Intra- and interrater reliability values were similar for EAI and DME ratings.DISCUSSIONOverall, results of this work suggest that DME is the best fit for scaling several hypokinetic dysarthria features, and not the conventionally used EAI scale. Prothetic dimensions best scaled by DME include overall speech severity, articulatory imprecision, reduced loudness, and short rushes of speech. Monotony was the only feature found to be a metathetic dimension and would be best scaled using EAI or DME. Findings call for rethinking the widespread use of EAI scales for rating perceptual features as part of the assessment and treatment of motor speech disorders.","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":"209 1","pages":"1-13"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142252517","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Predictive Measures in Child Language Development: The Role of Familial History and Early Expressive Vocabulary. 儿童语言发展的预测措施:家族史和早期表达性词汇的作用。
IF 2.6 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-18 DOI: 10.1044/2024_jslhr-23-00815
Elena Capelli,Chiara Dondena,Maria Luisa Lorusso,Sara Mascheretti,Raffaella Pozzoli,Antonio Salandi,Massimo Molteni,Valentina Riva,Chiara Cantiani
PURPOSEPrediction of developmental language disorder in children under 3 years of age is challenging. Among early risk factors, research has focused on having a positive familial history (FH+) for language or literacy problems and on late language emergence, that is, late-talker (LT) status. The interaction between these two risk factors and their cumulative effect is still debated. Here, we (a) investigate the effect of FH+ on 24-month language development, (b) test for cumulative effects of FH+ status and early language delay on 36-month language outcomes, and (c) disentangle the direct and indirect effects of familial history (FH) on the language outcome.METHODOne hundred eighty-five Italian children were followed up longitudinally between 24 and 36 months of age (64 FH+ and 121 FH-) through parental questionnaires and direct child assessment.RESULTSAt the age of 24 months, the FH+ group showed worse expressive vocabulary and higher prevalence of LT. At the age of 36 months, main effects of LT and FH were identified on lexical and phonological performances, respectively. Interestingly, significant interaction effects were identified on expressive vocabulary and phonological processing. Path analysis highlights that FH had a direct effect on later measures of phonology, whereas its effect on 36-month lexical abilities was indirect, via measures of expressive vocabulary at 24 months.CONCLUSIONSThe study suggests specific predictive roles of FH and LT status on language development. Interestingly, FH+ seems to represent an additive risk for LT children. The use of cumulative risk measures is confirmed as a powerful approach to identify those children with the highest probability of developing persistent language difficulties.SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALhttps://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26790580.
目的预测 3 岁以下儿童的语言发育障碍具有挑战性。在早期风险因素中,研究主要集中在有语言或识字问题的阳性家族史(FH+)和晚期语言出现,即晚期说话者(LT)状态。这两种风险因素之间的相互作用及其累积效应仍存在争议。在此,我们(a) 调查了家族史+对 24 个月语言发展的影响,(b) 测试了家族史+状态和早期语言延迟对 36 个月语言结果的累积效应,(c) 区分了家族史(FH)对语言结果的直接和间接影响。结果24个月大时,FH+组儿童的词汇表达能力较差,LT发生率较高。36 个月大时,LT 和 FH 分别对词汇和语音表现产生主效应。有趣的是,在词汇表达和语音处理方面还发现了明显的交互效应。路径分析显示,FH 对后来的语音测量有直接影响,而其对 36 个月大的词汇能力的影响则是间接的,是通过测量 24 个月大时的表达词汇量来实现的。有趣的是,FH+似乎代表了LT儿童的附加风险。研究证实,使用累积风险测量法是识别那些最有可能发展为持续性语言障碍的儿童的有效方法。补充材料https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26790580。
{"title":"Predictive Measures in Child Language Development: The Role of Familial History and Early Expressive Vocabulary.","authors":"Elena Capelli,Chiara Dondena,Maria Luisa Lorusso,Sara Mascheretti,Raffaella Pozzoli,Antonio Salandi,Massimo Molteni,Valentina Riva,Chiara Cantiani","doi":"10.1044/2024_jslhr-23-00815","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/2024_jslhr-23-00815","url":null,"abstract":"PURPOSEPrediction of developmental language disorder in children under 3 years of age is challenging. Among early risk factors, research has focused on having a positive familial history (FH+) for language or literacy problems and on late language emergence, that is, late-talker (LT) status. The interaction between these two risk factors and their cumulative effect is still debated. Here, we (a) investigate the effect of FH+ on 24-month language development, (b) test for cumulative effects of FH+ status and early language delay on 36-month language outcomes, and (c) disentangle the direct and indirect effects of familial history (FH) on the language outcome.METHODOne hundred eighty-five Italian children were followed up longitudinally between 24 and 36 months of age (64 FH+ and 121 FH-) through parental questionnaires and direct child assessment.RESULTSAt the age of 24 months, the FH+ group showed worse expressive vocabulary and higher prevalence of LT. At the age of 36 months, main effects of LT and FH were identified on lexical and phonological performances, respectively. Interestingly, significant interaction effects were identified on expressive vocabulary and phonological processing. Path analysis highlights that FH had a direct effect on later measures of phonology, whereas its effect on 36-month lexical abilities was indirect, via measures of expressive vocabulary at 24 months.CONCLUSIONSThe study suggests specific predictive roles of FH and LT status on language development. Interestingly, FH+ seems to represent an additive risk for LT children. The use of cumulative risk measures is confirmed as a powerful approach to identify those children with the highest probability of developing persistent language difficulties.SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALhttps://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26790580.","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":"3 1","pages":"1-19"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142252522","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Measuring the Effects of Cognitive Capacity on Sentence Comprehension: Evidence From Elementary School-Age Children. 测量认知能力对句子理解的影响:小学学龄儿童的证据。
IF 2.6 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-18 DOI: 10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00155
Beula M Magimairaj,Naveen K Nagaraj,Ronald B Gillam
PURPOSEOur aim was to (a) develop a sentence comprehension measure that distinguished between cognitive capacity and syntactic knowledge in school-age children and (b) examine the relationship between comprehension performance and cognitive variables (working memory capacity and retrieval from long-term memory).METHODWe developed and administered a picture selection sentence comprehension task to 122 school-age children representing varied cognitive abilities. We evaluated comprehension accuracy and response time in two syntactically identical conditions but with different cognitive demands incorporated in picture foils-one with low demand using superfluous adjectives and another with high demand using contrastive adjectives. Children also completed tasks measuring working memory capacity and long-term memory retrieval.RESULTSComprehension accuracy was significantly lower, and response times were longer in the high-cognitive demand condition compared to the low-demand condition. Errors frequently involved incorrect attribute selection in the high-demand condition that included contrastive adjectives in picture foils, while reversal errors prevailed in the low-demand condition, which included superfluous adjectives. Accuracy correlated positively with the memory variables. Hierarchical regression analysis showed that after adjusting for comprehension in the low-cognitive demand condition (38.60% variance), memory variables accounted for 4.50% additional variance in the high-demand condition with only working memory capacity as the unique predictor.CONCLUSIONSThe significant role of working memory capacity in comprehending sentences with high cognitive demand indicated the recruitment of active attention and verbal rehearsal. Data support the newly developed measure's potential for assessing cognitive skills integral to sentence comprehension in school-age children.SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALhttps://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26767063.
我们的目的是:(a) 开发一种能区分学龄儿童认知能力和句法知识的句子理解测量方法;(b) 研究理解成绩与认知变量(工作记忆能力和长时记忆检索)之间的关系。我们在两种句法相同但认知要求不同的图片衬托条件下评估了理解的准确性和反应时间,一种是使用多余形容词的低要求条件,另一种是使用对比形容词的高要求条件。结果与低需求条件相比,高认知需求条件下的理解准确率明显较低,反应时间较长。在高需求条件下,由于图片衬托物中包含了对比性形容词,错误往往涉及错误的属性选择,而在低需求条件下,由于包含了多余的形容词,错误则普遍存在。准确性与记忆变量呈正相关。层次回归分析表明,在对低认知需求条件下的理解能力(38.60% 的方差)进行调整后,记忆变量在高认知需求条件下产生了 4.50% 的额外方差,只有工作记忆能力是唯一的预测因素。数据支持了新开发的测量方法在评估学龄儿童句子理解中不可或缺的认知技能方面的潜力。补充材料https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26767063。
{"title":"Measuring the Effects of Cognitive Capacity on Sentence Comprehension: Evidence From Elementary School-Age Children.","authors":"Beula M Magimairaj,Naveen K Nagaraj,Ronald B Gillam","doi":"10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00155","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00155","url":null,"abstract":"PURPOSEOur aim was to (a) develop a sentence comprehension measure that distinguished between cognitive capacity and syntactic knowledge in school-age children and (b) examine the relationship between comprehension performance and cognitive variables (working memory capacity and retrieval from long-term memory).METHODWe developed and administered a picture selection sentence comprehension task to 122 school-age children representing varied cognitive abilities. We evaluated comprehension accuracy and response time in two syntactically identical conditions but with different cognitive demands incorporated in picture foils-one with low demand using superfluous adjectives and another with high demand using contrastive adjectives. Children also completed tasks measuring working memory capacity and long-term memory retrieval.RESULTSComprehension accuracy was significantly lower, and response times were longer in the high-cognitive demand condition compared to the low-demand condition. Errors frequently involved incorrect attribute selection in the high-demand condition that included contrastive adjectives in picture foils, while reversal errors prevailed in the low-demand condition, which included superfluous adjectives. Accuracy correlated positively with the memory variables. Hierarchical regression analysis showed that after adjusting for comprehension in the low-cognitive demand condition (38.60% variance), memory variables accounted for 4.50% additional variance in the high-demand condition with only working memory capacity as the unique predictor.CONCLUSIONSThe significant role of working memory capacity in comprehending sentences with high cognitive demand indicated the recruitment of active attention and verbal rehearsal. Data support the newly developed measure's potential for assessing cognitive skills integral to sentence comprehension in school-age children.SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALhttps://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26767063.","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":"8 1","pages":"1-16"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142268524","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
English Vowel Perception in Spanish-English Bilingual Preschoolers: Multiple-Talker Input Is Only Beneficial for Children With High Language Exposure Levels. 西班牙语-英语双语学龄前儿童的英语元音感知:多语种输入只对语言接触水平高的儿童有益。
IF 2.6 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-18 DOI: 10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00044
Simona Montanari,Jeremy Steffman,Robert Mayr
PURPOSESThis study examines English vowel perception in Spanish-English bilingual preschoolers, comparing bilinguals' perception patterns to those of monolinguals and examining how child internal (age) and external variables (input quantity and input diversity) predict perceptual performance.METHODSixty children between 3;6 and 5;6 (years;months) of age participated in the study, 28 of whom were Spanish-English bilinguals and 32 English monolinguals. Perception was assessed through a forced-choice minimal-pair identification task in which children heard synthesized audio stimuli (i.e., "sheep" and "ship") that varied systematically along the /i-ɪ/ continuum and were asked to match them with one of two pictures. The data were analyzed with Bayesian mixed-effects logistic regression analyses, modeling responses as a function of continuum step, language background (monolingual or bilingual), age, English exposure (i.e., input quantity), and number of English input providers (i.e., input diversity).RESULTSThe results indicate that, despite displaying nonnative English stop voicing perception in a previous study, the same bilingual children showed English /i-ɪ/ perception patterns that did not differ from those of monolinguals. While age did not predict vowel perception, input quantity and diversity jointly interacted to moderate how well children perceived the /i-ɪ/ contrast. Specifically, diverse input promoted perceptual performance in children who received high levels of English exposure-and who presumably had more advanced English language skills, whereas it limited perceptual performance in children with more limited English exposure and skills.CONCLUSIONSThis study shows that bilingual children can show monolingual-like perception patterns for some sounds while displaying nontarget perception for others. This is the first study to demonstrate that language exposure mediates the role of input diversity on speech sound development, suggesting that varied input can be more or less beneficial for speech sound development based on the learner's language learning stage.
目的:本研究探讨了西班牙语-英语双语学龄前儿童对英语元音的感知,将双语儿童的感知模式与单语儿童的感知模式进行了比较,并研究了儿童的内部变量(年龄)和外部变量(输入量和输入多样性)如何预测儿童的感知表现。方法:60 名年龄在 3;6 到 5;6 (岁;月)之间的儿童参加了本研究,其中 28 名是西班牙语-英语双语儿童,32 名是英语单语儿童。研究人员通过强迫选择最小配对识别任务评估了儿童的感知能力。在该任务中,儿童听到的合成音频刺激(即 "羊 "和 "船")沿着/i-ɪ/连续体有系统地变化,并被要求将它们与两幅图片中的一幅匹配起来。数据采用贝叶斯混合效应逻辑回归分析法进行分析,将反应建模为连续阶梯、语言背景(单语或双语)、年龄、英语接触(即输入量)和英语输入提供者数量(即输入多样性)的函数。结果结果表明,尽管在之前的研究中显示了非母语的英语停止发声感知,但同样的双语儿童表现出的英语 /i-ɪ/ 感知模式与单语儿童并无不同。虽然年龄不能预测元音感知,但输入量和多样性共同作用,调节了儿童对/i-ɪ/对比的感知程度。具体来说,对于英语接触水平较高且英语技能可能较高的儿童来说,多样化的输入会促进他们的感知能力,而对于英语接触水平和技能较为有限的儿童来说,多样化的输入则会限制他们的感知能力。这是第一项证明语言接触对输入多样性对语音发展的作用具有中介作用的研究,表明根据学习者的语言学习阶段,不同的输入对语音发展或多或少有好处。
{"title":"English Vowel Perception in Spanish-English Bilingual Preschoolers: Multiple-Talker Input Is Only Beneficial for Children With High Language Exposure Levels.","authors":"Simona Montanari,Jeremy Steffman,Robert Mayr","doi":"10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00044","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00044","url":null,"abstract":"PURPOSESThis study examines English vowel perception in Spanish-English bilingual preschoolers, comparing bilinguals' perception patterns to those of monolinguals and examining how child internal (age) and external variables (input quantity and input diversity) predict perceptual performance.METHODSixty children between 3;6 and 5;6 (years;months) of age participated in the study, 28 of whom were Spanish-English bilinguals and 32 English monolinguals. Perception was assessed through a forced-choice minimal-pair identification task in which children heard synthesized audio stimuli (i.e., \"sheep\" and \"ship\") that varied systematically along the /i-ɪ/ continuum and were asked to match them with one of two pictures. The data were analyzed with Bayesian mixed-effects logistic regression analyses, modeling responses as a function of continuum step, language background (monolingual or bilingual), age, English exposure (i.e., input quantity), and number of English input providers (i.e., input diversity).RESULTSThe results indicate that, despite displaying nonnative English stop voicing perception in a previous study, the same bilingual children showed English /i-ɪ/ perception patterns that did not differ from those of monolinguals. While age did not predict vowel perception, input quantity and diversity jointly interacted to moderate how well children perceived the /i-ɪ/ contrast. Specifically, diverse input promoted perceptual performance in children who received high levels of English exposure-and who presumably had more advanced English language skills, whereas it limited perceptual performance in children with more limited English exposure and skills.CONCLUSIONSThis study shows that bilingual children can show monolingual-like perception patterns for some sounds while displaying nontarget perception for others. This is the first study to demonstrate that language exposure mediates the role of input diversity on speech sound development, suggesting that varied input can be more or less beneficial for speech sound development based on the learner's language learning stage.","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":"26 1","pages":"1-17"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142252518","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Decomposing Variation in Vocabulary and Listening Comprehension Task Performance in Spanish and English Into Person, Ecological, and Assessment Differences for Spanish-English Bilingual Children in the United States. 将美国西英双语儿童在西班牙语和英语词汇和听力理解任务中的表现差异分解为个人差异、生态差异和评估差异。
IF 2.6 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-18 DOI: 10.1044/2024_jslhr-23-00702
Young-Suk Grace Kim,Nuria Gutierrez,Yaacov Petscher
PURPOSEWe investigated the contributions of person, ecological, and assessment characteristics to one's performance on vocabulary and listening comprehension tasks in English and Spanish. Person characteristics included English learner status, ecological characteristics included instructional program enrollment (bilingual vs. English immersion) and poverty status, and assessment characteristics included the language of assessment (Spanish vs. English) and linguistic grain size (vocabulary vs. listening comprehension).METHODData were from 208 Spanish-English emergent bilingual children in Grade 1 in the United States and were analyzed using explanatory item response models.RESULTSSubstantial variance in the tasks across both languages was attributable to the random effects of assessment/item (74%) and person (26%). English learner status was a significant student predictor of language performance, and the instructional program was a significant ecological predictor of language performance. Regarding assessment characteristics, both the language of assessment and linguistic grain size explained the variance in performance. There were no differences in performance on English assessments between children in the Spanish-English bilingual program and children in the English immersion program. However, on the Spanish tasks, children in the bilingual program outperformed their peers in the English immersion program.CONCLUSIONThese findings highlight the importance of considering not only person/child characteristics but also ecological and assessment characteristics for Spanish-English bilingual children's performance on oral language tasks.SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALhttps://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26972749.
目的我们研究了个人、生态和评估特征对英语和西班牙语词汇和听力理解任务成绩的影响。个人特征包括英语学习者身份,生态特征包括教学计划注册情况(双语与英语浸入式)和贫困状况,评估特征包括评估语言(西班牙语与英语)和语言粒度(词汇与听力理解)。数据来自美国 208 名一年级的西班牙语-英语新兴双语儿童,并使用解释性项目反应模型进行了分析。结果两种语言任务的巨大差异可归因于评估/项目(74%)和人(26%)的随机效应。)英语学习者身份是预测语言成绩的重要学生因素,而教学计划则是预测语言成绩的重要生态因素。在评估特征方面,评估语言和语言粒度都能解释成绩的差异。参加西班牙语-英语双语项目的儿童和参加英语浸入式项目的儿童在英语评估中的表现没有差异。结论:这些研究结果突出表明,对于西班牙语-英语双语儿童的口语任务表现,不仅要考虑个人/儿童特征,还要考虑生态和评估特征。
{"title":"Decomposing Variation in Vocabulary and Listening Comprehension Task Performance in Spanish and English Into Person, Ecological, and Assessment Differences for Spanish-English Bilingual Children in the United States.","authors":"Young-Suk Grace Kim,Nuria Gutierrez,Yaacov Petscher","doi":"10.1044/2024_jslhr-23-00702","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/2024_jslhr-23-00702","url":null,"abstract":"PURPOSEWe investigated the contributions of person, ecological, and assessment characteristics to one's performance on vocabulary and listening comprehension tasks in English and Spanish. Person characteristics included English learner status, ecological characteristics included instructional program enrollment (bilingual vs. English immersion) and poverty status, and assessment characteristics included the language of assessment (Spanish vs. English) and linguistic grain size (vocabulary vs. listening comprehension).METHODData were from 208 Spanish-English emergent bilingual children in Grade 1 in the United States and were analyzed using explanatory item response models.RESULTSSubstantial variance in the tasks across both languages was attributable to the random effects of assessment/item (74%) and person (26%). English learner status was a significant student predictor of language performance, and the instructional program was a significant ecological predictor of language performance. Regarding assessment characteristics, both the language of assessment and linguistic grain size explained the variance in performance. There were no differences in performance on English assessments between children in the Spanish-English bilingual program and children in the English immersion program. However, on the Spanish tasks, children in the bilingual program outperformed their peers in the English immersion program.CONCLUSIONThese findings highlight the importance of considering not only person/child characteristics but also ecological and assessment characteristics for Spanish-English bilingual children's performance on oral language tasks.SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALhttps://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26972749.","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":"14 1","pages":"1-15"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142252519","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effect of Stellate Ganglion Block on Dysphagia and Cognitive Impairment in Cerebral Small Vessel Disease: A Randomized Controlled Study. 星状神经节阻滞对脑小血管疾病患者吞咽困难和认知障碍的影响:随机对照研究》。
IF 2.6 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-13 DOI: 10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00145
Hongji Zeng,Weijia Zhao,Lixuan Fang,Huoying Pan,Shaochun Huang,Xi Zeng
BACKGROUNDSCerebral small vessel disease (CSVD) can potentially and frequently lead to dysphagia and cognitive impairment. Stellate ganglion block (SGB) can alleviate the symptoms by regulating neural pathways and improving cerebral blood circulation.OBJECTIVESThis study aimed to explore the clinical effect of SGB on airway protection, dysphagia, cognitive impairment, and activities of daily living (ADL) in CSVD patients.METHODThis was a randomized controlled study conducted from February 2021 to May 2023, including 84 CSVD patients with dysphagia and cognitive impairment. The participants were randomly divided into the SGB group (n = 42) and the untreated group (n = 42). All received standard-of-care rehabilitation for 20 days. The SGB group received SGB once a day additionally. Assessments were conducted on Day 1 and Day 20, respectively. The Penetration-Aspiration Scale (PAS) was primary outcome. Modified Barium Swallow Impairment (MBSImp), Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Modified Barthel Index (MBI) and adverse events were secondary outcomes. This study was registered at ClinicalTrials.gov, Identifier: NCT06176404.RESULTSThere were two dropout cases in the untreated group. Time effect with statistical significance was observed in all assessments (p < .05). Group effect with statistical significance was observed in the PAS (z = -17.283, p < .001), MBSImp-Oral (z = -3.382, p = .001), MBSImp-Pharyngeal (z = -2.639, p = .008), MMSE (F = 7.450, p = .008), and MBI (F = 6.408, p = .013). During the treatment, there were no severe adverse events.CONCLUSIONSGB can significantly and safely improve airway protection, dysphagia, cognitive function, and ADL in CSVD patients with dysphagia and cognitive impairment who received standard-of-care rehabilitation.
背景脑小血管病(CSVD)可能并经常导致吞咽困难和认知障碍。星状神经节阻滞(SGB)可通过调节神经通路和改善脑血液循环来缓解症状。目的本研究旨在探讨SGB对CSVD患者气道保护、吞咽困难、认知障碍和日常生活活动(ADL)的临床影响。方法这是一项随机对照研究,研究时间为2021年2月至2023年5月,共纳入84名伴有吞咽困难和认知障碍的CSVD患者。参与者被随机分为SGB组(42人)和未治疗组(42人)。所有患者都接受了为期 20 天的标准康复治疗。SGB组每天额外接受一次SGB治疗。分别在第 1 天和第 20 天进行评估。吞咽-吐气量表(PAS)是主要结果。改良钡吞咽功能障碍(MBSImp)、迷你精神状态检查(MMSE)、改良巴特尔指数(MBI)和不良事件为次要结果。本研究已在 ClinicalTrials.gov 注册,标识符为结果未治疗组有两例辍学。在所有评估中均观察到具有统计学意义的时间效应(P < .05)。在PAS(z = -17.283,p < .001)、MBSImp-Oral(z = -3.382,p = .001)、MBSImp-Pharyngeal(z = -2.639,p = .008)、MMSE(F = 7.450,p = .008)和MBI(F = 6.408,p = .013)中观察到具有统计学意义的组间效应。结论 GB 可以显著安全地改善接受标准康复治疗的吞咽困难和认知障碍 CSVD 患者的气道保护、吞咽困难、认知功能和日常活动能力。
{"title":"Effect of Stellate Ganglion Block on Dysphagia and Cognitive Impairment in Cerebral Small Vessel Disease: A Randomized Controlled Study.","authors":"Hongji Zeng,Weijia Zhao,Lixuan Fang,Huoying Pan,Shaochun Huang,Xi Zeng","doi":"10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00145","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00145","url":null,"abstract":"BACKGROUNDSCerebral small vessel disease (CSVD) can potentially and frequently lead to dysphagia and cognitive impairment. Stellate ganglion block (SGB) can alleviate the symptoms by regulating neural pathways and improving cerebral blood circulation.OBJECTIVESThis study aimed to explore the clinical effect of SGB on airway protection, dysphagia, cognitive impairment, and activities of daily living (ADL) in CSVD patients.METHODThis was a randomized controlled study conducted from February 2021 to May 2023, including 84 CSVD patients with dysphagia and cognitive impairment. The participants were randomly divided into the SGB group (n = 42) and the untreated group (n = 42). All received standard-of-care rehabilitation for 20 days. The SGB group received SGB once a day additionally. Assessments were conducted on Day 1 and Day 20, respectively. The Penetration-Aspiration Scale (PAS) was primary outcome. Modified Barium Swallow Impairment (MBSImp), Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Modified Barthel Index (MBI) and adverse events were secondary outcomes. This study was registered at ClinicalTrials.gov, Identifier: NCT06176404.RESULTSThere were two dropout cases in the untreated group. Time effect with statistical significance was observed in all assessments (p < .05). Group effect with statistical significance was observed in the PAS (z = -17.283, p < .001), MBSImp-Oral (z = -3.382, p = .001), MBSImp-Pharyngeal (z = -2.639, p = .008), MMSE (F = 7.450, p = .008), and MBI (F = 6.408, p = .013). During the treatment, there were no severe adverse events.CONCLUSIONSGB can significantly and safely improve airway protection, dysphagia, cognitive function, and ADL in CSVD patients with dysphagia and cognitive impairment who received standard-of-care rehabilitation.","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":"45 1","pages":"1-13"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142252520","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of Speech Cues on Acoustics and Intelligibility of Korean-Speaking Children With Cerebral Palsy. 语音线索对说韩语的脑瘫儿童的声学和可理解性的影响。
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 Epub Date: 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00457
Younghwa M Chang, Pil-Yeon Jeong, KyungHae Hwang, Bo-Yeon Ihn, Megan J McAuliffe, Hyunsub Sim, Erika S Levy

Purpose: Reduced speech intelligibility is often a hallmark of children with dysarthria secondary to cerebral palsy (CP), but effects of speech strategies for increasing intelligibility are understudied, especially in children who speak languages other than English. This study examined the effects of (the Korean translation of) two cues, "speak with your big mouth" and "speak with your strong voice," on speech acoustics and intelligibility of Korean-speaking children with CP.

Method: Fifteen Korean-speaking children with CP repeated words and sentences in habitual, big mouth, and strong voice conditions. Acoustic analyses were performed and intelligibility was assessed by means of 90 blinded listeners' ease-of-understanding (EoU) ratings and percentage of words correctly transcribed (PWC).

Results: In response to both cues, children's vocal intensity and utterance duration increased significantly and differentially, whereas their vowel space area gains did not reach statistical significance. EoU increased significantly in the big mouth condition at word, but not sentence, level, whereas in the strong voice condition, EoU increased significantly at both levels. PWC increases were not statistically significant. Considerable variability in children's responses to cues was noted overall.

Conclusions: Korean-speaking children with CP modify their speech styles differentially when provided with cues aimed to increase their articulatory working space and vocal intensity. The results provide preliminary support for the use of the strong voice cue, in particular, to increase EoU. While the findings do not offer conclusive evidence of the intelligibility benefits of these cues, investigation with a larger sample size should provide further insight into optimal cueing strategies for increasing intelligibility in this population. Implications for language-specific versus language-independent treatment approaches are discussed.

Supplemental material: https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.25521052.

目的:语言清晰度降低通常是脑性瘫痪(CP)继发构音障碍儿童的一个特征,但提高语言清晰度的语言策略的效果却未得到充分研究,尤其是对使用英语以外语言的儿童。本研究考察了 "用你的大嘴说话 "和 "用你的大嗓门说话 "这两种提示(韩语译文)对讲韩语的 CP 儿童的语音声学和可懂度的影响:方法:15 名讲韩语的 CP 儿童在习惯、大口和强声条件下重复单词和句子。我们进行了声学分析,并通过 90 名盲听者的易懂程度(EoU)评分和单词正确转录百分比(PWC)评估了可懂度:在两种提示下,儿童的发声强度和发音持续时间都有不同程度的显著增加,而元音空间面积的增加则没有达到统计学意义。在大嘴巴条件下,EoU 在单词(而不是句子)水平上有明显增加,而在强音条件下,EoU 在两个水平上都有明显增加。PWC 的增长在统计学上没有意义。总体而言,儿童对提示的反应存在很大差异:结论:说韩语的 CP 儿童在接受旨在增加其发音工作空间和声音强度的提示时,会以不同的方式改变其说话方式。研究结果初步支持了使用强音提示,特别是增加EoU。虽然研究结果并不能为这些提示的可懂度益处提供确凿证据,但通过对更大样本量的调查,应能进一步了解提高这类人群可懂度的最佳提示策略。本文还讨论了针对语言的治疗方法与不依赖语言的治疗方法之间的差异。补充材料:https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.25521052。
{"title":"Effects of Speech Cues on Acoustics and Intelligibility of Korean-Speaking Children With Cerebral Palsy.","authors":"Younghwa M Chang, Pil-Yeon Jeong, KyungHae Hwang, Bo-Yeon Ihn, Megan J McAuliffe, Hyunsub Sim, Erika S Levy","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00457","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00457","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Reduced speech intelligibility is often a hallmark of children with dysarthria secondary to cerebral palsy (CP), but effects of speech strategies for increasing intelligibility are understudied, especially in children who speak languages other than English. This study examined the effects of (the Korean translation of) two cues, \"speak with your big mouth\" and \"speak with your strong voice,\" on speech acoustics and intelligibility of Korean-speaking children with CP.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Fifteen Korean-speaking children with CP repeated words and sentences in habitual, big mouth, and strong voice conditions. Acoustic analyses were performed and intelligibility was assessed by means of 90 blinded listeners' ease-of-understanding (EoU) ratings and percentage of words correctly transcribed (PWC).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>In response to both cues, children's vocal intensity and utterance duration increased significantly and differentially, whereas their vowel space area gains did not reach statistical significance. EoU increased significantly in the big mouth condition at word, but not sentence, level, whereas in the strong voice condition, EoU increased significantly at both levels. PWC increases were not statistically significant. Considerable variability in children's responses to cues was noted overall.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Korean-speaking children with CP modify their speech styles differentially when provided with cues aimed to increase their articulatory working space and vocal intensity. The results provide preliminary support for the use of the strong voice cue, in particular, to increase EoU. While the findings do not offer conclusive evidence of the intelligibility benefits of these cues, investigation with a larger sample size should provide further insight into optimal cueing strategies for increasing intelligibility in this population. Implications for language-specific versus language-independent treatment approaches are discussed.</p><p><strong>Supplemental material: </strong>https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.25521052.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"2856-2871"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140853434","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Accuracy of Speech Sound Analysis: Comparison of an Automatic Artificial Intelligence Algorithm With Clinician Assessment. 语音声音分析的准确性:人工智能自动算法与临床医生评估的比较。
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 Epub Date: 2024-08-22 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00009
Micalle Carl, Eduard Rudyk, Yair Shapira, Heather Leavy Rusiewicz, Michal Icht

Purpose: Automatic speech analysis (ASA) and automatic speech recognition systems are increasingly being used in the treatment of speech sound disorders (SSDs). When utilized as a home practice tool or in the absence of the clinician, the ASA system has the potential to facilitate treatment gains. However, the feedback accuracy of such systems varies, a factor that may impact these gains. The current research analyzes the feedback accuracy of a novel ASA algorithm (Amplio Learning Technologies), in comparison to clinician judgments.

Method: A total of 3,584 consonant stimuli, produced by 395 American English-speaking children and adolescents with SSDs (age range: 4-18 years), were analyzed with respect to automatic classification of the ASA algorithm, clinician-ASA agreement, and interclinician agreement. Further analysis of results as related to phoneme acquisition categories (early-, middle-, and late-acquired phonemes) was conducted.

Results: Agreement between clinicians and ASA classification for sounds produced accurately was above 80% for all phonemes, with some variation based on phoneme acquisition category (early, middle, late). This variation was also noted for ASA classification into "acceptable," "unacceptable," and "unknown" (which means no determination of phoneme accuracy) categories, as well as interclinician agreement. Clinician-ASA agreement was reduced for misarticulated sounds.

Conclusions: The initial findings of Amplio's novel algorithm are promising for its potential use within the context of home practice, as it demonstrates high feedback accuracy for correctly produced sounds. Furthermore, complexity of sound influences consistency of perception, both by clinicians and by automated platforms, indicating variable performance of the ASA algorithm across phonemes. Taken together, the ASA algorithm may be effective in facilitating speech sound practice for children with SSDs, even in the absence of the clinician.

目的:自动语音分析(ASA)和自动语音识别系统越来越多地用于治疗言语发音障碍(SSD)。当作为家庭练习工具或在临床医生不在场的情况下使用时,自动语音分析系统有可能促进治疗效果的提高。然而,此类系统的反馈准确性各不相同,这可能是影响治疗效果的一个因素。目前的研究分析了一种新型 ASA 算法(Amplio Learning Technologies)的反馈准确性,并与临床医生的判断进行了比较:方法:研究人员分析了 395 名患有 SSD 的美式英语儿童和青少年(年龄范围:4-18 岁)共发出的 3,584 个辅音刺激,分析了 ASA 算法的自动分类、临床医生与 ASA 的一致性以及临床医生之间的一致性。此外,还进一步分析了与音素习得类别(早期、中期和晚期习得音素)相关的结果:结果:在所有音素中,临床医生与 ASA 分类对准确发音的一致率均在 80% 以上,但因音素习得类别(早期、中期和晚期)的不同而存在一定差异。这种差异也体现在 ASA 对 "可接受"、"不可接受 "和 "未知"(即无法确定音素的准确性)类别的分类上,以及临床医生之间的一致性上。在错误发音方面,临床医师与 ASA 的一致性有所降低:Amplio新算法的初步研究结果表明,正确发音的反馈准确率很高,因此有望用于家庭练习。此外,声音的复杂性会影响临床医生和自动平台的感知一致性,这表明 ASA 算法在不同音素上的表现各不相同。综上所述,即使在没有临床医生在场的情况下,ASA 算法也能有效促进 SSD 儿童的语音练习。
{"title":"Accuracy of Speech Sound Analysis: Comparison of an Automatic Artificial Intelligence Algorithm With Clinician Assessment.","authors":"Micalle Carl, Eduard Rudyk, Yair Shapira, Heather Leavy Rusiewicz, Michal Icht","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00009","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00009","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Automatic speech analysis (ASA) and automatic speech recognition systems are increasingly being used in the treatment of speech sound disorders (SSDs). When utilized as a home practice tool or in the absence of the clinician, the ASA system has the potential to facilitate treatment gains. However, the feedback accuracy of such systems varies, a factor that may impact these gains. The current research analyzes the feedback accuracy of a novel ASA algorithm (Amplio Learning Technologies), in comparison to clinician judgments.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>A total of 3,584 consonant stimuli, produced by 395 American English-speaking children and adolescents with SSDs (age range: 4-18 years), were analyzed with respect to automatic classification of the ASA algorithm, clinician-ASA agreement, and interclinician agreement. Further analysis of results as related to phoneme acquisition categories (early-, middle-, and late-acquired phonemes) was conducted.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Agreement between clinicians and ASA classification for sounds produced accurately was above 80% for all phonemes, with some variation based on phoneme acquisition category (early, middle, late). This variation was also noted for ASA classification into \"acceptable,\" \"unacceptable,\" and \"unknown\" (which means no determination of phoneme accuracy) categories, as well as interclinician agreement. Clinician-ASA agreement was reduced for misarticulated sounds.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The initial findings of Amplio's novel algorithm are promising for its potential use within the context of home practice, as it demonstrates high feedback accuracy for correctly produced sounds. Furthermore, complexity of sound influences consistency of perception, both by clinicians and by automated platforms, indicating variable performance of the ASA algorithm across phonemes. Taken together, the ASA algorithm may be effective in facilitating speech sound practice for children with SSDs, even in the absence of the clinician.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"3004-3021"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142037748","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Eye-Tracking Methodology to Detect Differences in Attention to Faces Between Developmental Language Disorder and Autism. 用眼动跟踪法检测发育性语言障碍和自闭症患者对人脸的注意力差异。
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 Epub Date: 2024-08-28 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00109
Julia Vacas, Adoración Antolí, Araceli Sánchez-Raya, Carolina Pérez-Dueñas, Fátima Cuadrado

Purpose: Developmental language disorder (DLD) and autism sometimes appear as overlapping conditions in behavioral tests. There is much literature on the visual scanning pattern (VSP) of faces in autistic children, but this is scarce regarding those with DLD. The purpose of this study was to compare the VSP of faces in young children with DLD, those with autism, and typically developing peers, assessing the effect of three variables.

Method: Two eye-tracking experiments were designed to assess the effect of the emotion and the poser's gender (Experiment 1) and the poser's age (Experiment 2) on the VSP of participants (Experiment 1: N = 59, age range: 32-74 months; Experiment 2: N = 58, age range: 32-74 months). We operationalized the VSP in terms of attentional orientation, visual preference, and depth of processing of each sort of face. We developed two paired preference tasks in which pairs of images of faces showing different emotions were displayed simultaneously to compete for children's attention.

Results: Data analysis revealed two VSP markers common to both disorders: (a) superficial processing of faces and (b) late orientation to angry and child faces. Moreover, one specific marker for each condition was also found: typical preference for child faces in children with DLD versus diminished preference for them in autistic children.

Conclusions: Considering the similarities found between children with DLD and those with autism, difficulties of children with DLD in attention to faces have been systematically underestimated. Thus, more effort must be made to identify and respond to the needs of this population. Clinical practice may benefit from the potential of eye-tracking methodology and the analysis of the VSP to assess attention to faces in both conditions. This would also contribute to the improvement of early differential diagnosis in the long run.

目的:发育性语言障碍(DLD)和自闭症有时会在行为测试中出现重叠情况。有关自闭症儿童人脸视觉扫描模式(VSP)的文献很多,但有关发育性语言障碍儿童的文献却很少。本研究的目的是比较患有 DLD 的幼儿、患有自闭症的幼儿和发育正常的同龄人对人脸的视觉扫描模式,评估三个变量的影响:方法:设计了两个眼动跟踪实验,以评估情绪、摆姿势者的性别(实验 1)和摆姿势者的年龄(实验 2)对参与者 VSP 的影响(实验 1:N = 59,年龄范围:32-74 个月;实验 2):实验 1:人数 = 59,年龄范围:32-74 个月;实验 2:人数 = 58,年龄范围:32-74 个月):32-74 个月)。我们从注意方向、视觉偏好和对每种面孔的处理深度等方面对 VSP 进行了操作。我们开发了两个配对偏好任务,在这些任务中,显示不同情绪的成对人脸图像同时显示,以争夺儿童的注意力:数据分析显示,两种障碍都有两个共同的 VSP 标记:(a) 对面孔的表层处理;(b) 对愤怒面孔和儿童面孔的晚期定向。此外,还发现了每种情况的一个特定标记:DLD 儿童对儿童面孔的典型偏好与自闭症儿童对儿童面孔的减弱偏好:考虑到 DLD 儿童和自闭症儿童之间的相似性,DLD 儿童在注意面孔方面的困难被系统性地低估了。因此,必须做出更多努力来识别和满足这一群体的需求。临床实践可能会受益于眼动跟踪方法和 VSP 分析的潜力,以评估这两种情况下的面孔注意力。从长远来看,这也将有助于改善早期鉴别诊断。
{"title":"Eye-Tracking Methodology to Detect Differences in Attention to Faces Between Developmental Language Disorder and Autism.","authors":"Julia Vacas, Adoración Antolí, Araceli Sánchez-Raya, Carolina Pérez-Dueñas, Fátima Cuadrado","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00109","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00109","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Developmental language disorder (DLD) and autism sometimes appear as overlapping conditions in behavioral tests. There is much literature on the visual scanning pattern (VSP) of faces in autistic children, but this is scarce regarding those with DLD. The purpose of this study was to compare the VSP of faces in young children with DLD, those with autism, and typically developing peers, assessing the effect of three variables.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Two eye-tracking experiments were designed to assess the effect of the emotion and the poser's gender (Experiment 1) and the poser's age (Experiment 2) on the VSP of participants (Experiment 1: <i>N</i> = 59, age range: 32-74 months; Experiment 2: <i>N</i> = 58, age range: 32-74 months). We operationalized the VSP in terms of attentional orientation, visual preference, and depth of processing of each sort of face. We developed two paired preference tasks in which pairs of images of faces showing different emotions were displayed simultaneously to compete for children's attention.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Data analysis revealed two VSP markers common to both disorders: (a) superficial processing of faces and (b) late orientation to angry and child faces. Moreover, one specific marker for each condition was also found: typical preference for child faces in children with DLD versus diminished preference for them in autistic children.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Considering the similarities found between children with DLD and those with autism, difficulties of children with DLD in attention to faces have been systematically underestimated. Thus, more effort must be made to identify and respond to the needs of this population. Clinical practice may benefit from the potential of eye-tracking methodology and the analysis of the VSP to assess attention to faces in both conditions. This would also contribute to the improvement of early differential diagnosis in the long run.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"3148-3162"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142086382","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1