首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research最新文献

英文 中文
Characterization of Vocal Motor Control Using Laryngeal Kinematics in Individuals With Hyperfunctional Voice Disorders.
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-04-08 Epub Date: 2025-03-19 DOI: 10.1044/2025_JSLHR-24-00598
Hasini R Weerathunge, Jenny Vojtech, Courtney J Dunsmuir, Sarah J Cocroft, Manuel E Díaz-Cádiz, Victoria McKenna, Cara E Stepp

Purpose: High-speed videoendoscopy was used to investigate how underlying laryngeal motor control strategies differ in individuals with and without hyperfunctional voice disorders (HVDs). Three laryngeal kinematic measures were defined to characterize laryngeal motor control: kinematic stiffness, spatiotemporal index, and asymmetry index.

Method: Twenty-eight adults with HVDs and 28 age- and sex-matched controls produced repeated utterances of /ifi/ at three different gesture rates (50, 65, and 80 beats per minute) and three self-induced vocal effort levels (mild, moderate, and maximum effort) to elicit a range of linguistic contexts for the vocal targets produced. The glottal angle profiles of /ifi/ productions were extracted to calculate three kinematic measures of laryngeal motor control: kinematic stiffness (estimating laryngeal muscle tension), spatiotemporal index (estimating production variability), and asymmetry index (estimating movement asymmetry).

Results: Individuals with HVDs exhibited statistically significantly higher kinematic stiffness during varying effort levels and higher spatiotemporal indices and asymmetry indices compared to controls, indicating higher laryngeal muscle tension, production variability, and movement asymmetry, respectively.

Conclusion: Laryngeal kinematics suggest differing underlying motor control strategies in individuals with HVD relative to controls, which may inform better understanding of the etiology of HVDs.

Supplemental material: https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.28550387.

目的:通过高速视频内窥镜检查,研究嗓音功能亢进症(HVDs)患者和非嗓音功能亢进症患者的喉运动控制策略有何不同。我们定义了三种喉运动学测量方法来描述喉运动控制的特征:运动学硬度、时空指数和不对称指数:方法:28 名患有 HVDs 的成人和 28 名年龄和性别匹配的对照者在三种不同的手势频率(每分钟 50、65 和 80 次)和三种自我发声努力水平(轻度、中度和最大努力)下重复发出 /ifi/ 的语音,以激发发声目标的一系列语言环境。提取/ifi/发声的声门角度曲线,计算喉部运动控制的三个运动学测量指标:运动学硬度(估计喉部肌肉紧张度)、时空指数(估计发声变异性)和不对称指数(估计运动不对称度):结果:与对照组相比,HVDs 患者在不同用力水平下的运动僵硬度、时空指数和不对称指数均明显高于对照组,分别表明喉部肌肉张力、发音变异性和运动不对称程度更高:喉部运动学表明,相对于对照组,HVD患者的潜在运动控制策略有所不同,这可能有助于更好地了解HVD的病因。补充材料:https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.28550387。
{"title":"Characterization of Vocal Motor Control Using Laryngeal Kinematics in Individuals With Hyperfunctional Voice Disorders.","authors":"Hasini R Weerathunge, Jenny Vojtech, Courtney J Dunsmuir, Sarah J Cocroft, Manuel E Díaz-Cádiz, Victoria McKenna, Cara E Stepp","doi":"10.1044/2025_JSLHR-24-00598","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2025_JSLHR-24-00598","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>High-speed videoendoscopy was used to investigate how underlying laryngeal motor control strategies differ in individuals with and without hyperfunctional voice disorders (HVDs). Three laryngeal kinematic measures were defined to characterize laryngeal motor control: kinematic stiffness, spatiotemporal index, and asymmetry index.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Twenty-eight adults with HVDs and 28 age- and sex-matched controls produced repeated utterances of /ifi/ at three different gesture rates (50, 65, and 80 beats per minute) and three self-induced vocal effort levels (mild, moderate, and maximum effort) to elicit a range of linguistic contexts for the vocal targets produced. The glottal angle profiles of /ifi/ productions were extracted to calculate three kinematic measures of laryngeal motor control: kinematic stiffness (estimating laryngeal muscle tension), spatiotemporal index (estimating production variability), and asymmetry index (estimating movement asymmetry).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Individuals with HVDs exhibited statistically significantly higher kinematic stiffness during varying effort levels and higher spatiotemporal indices and asymmetry indices compared to controls, indicating higher laryngeal muscle tension, production variability, and movement asymmetry, respectively.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Laryngeal kinematics suggest differing underlying motor control strategies in individuals with HVD relative to controls, which may inform better understanding of the etiology of HVDs.</p><p><strong>Supplemental material: </strong>https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.28550387.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"1743-1757"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143665328","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining Potential Mediators of the Relationship Between Developmental Language Disorder and Executive Function Performance in Preschoolers.
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-04-08 Epub Date: 2025-03-12 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00549
Leah L Kapa

Purpose: The goal of this study was to examine potential mediators of the relationship between developmental language disorder (DLD) status and executive function performance.

Method: Participants included preschoolers, of whom 80 met the diagnostic criteria for DLD and 103 were categorized as having typical language abilities. Participants' nonverbal IQ and receptive vocabulary were assessed via standardized tests, and their executive function was tested using the Dimensional Change Card Sort. Maternal education was collected via caregiver report and served as a measure of socioeconomic status (SES).

Results: Group comparisons confirmed that participants with DLD had lower language scores, nonverbal IQ scores, executive function scores, and SES relative to peers with typical language. Additionally, these variables were significantly positively correlated. Multiple mediation analyses indicated that both nonverbal IQ and receptive vocabulary were significant partial mediators of the effect of DLD status on executive function scores. However, a direct effect of DLD status on executive function remained significant in a model that included the mediators and covaried maternal education.

Conclusions: Group differences in nonverbal IQ and receptive vocabulary can partially explain why children with DLD tend to have lower executive function scores relative to peers with typical language. However, even after accounting for these mediators, there is a significant, large direct effect of DLD status on executive function performance, which suggests that the groups' difference in executive functioning is not fully explained by other variables.

{"title":"Examining Potential Mediators of the Relationship Between Developmental Language Disorder and Executive Function Performance in Preschoolers.","authors":"Leah L Kapa","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00549","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00549","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>The goal of this study was to examine potential mediators of the relationship between developmental language disorder (DLD) status and executive function performance.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Participants included preschoolers, of whom 80 met the diagnostic criteria for DLD and 103 were categorized as having typical language abilities. Participants' nonverbal IQ and receptive vocabulary were assessed via standardized tests, and their executive function was tested using the Dimensional Change Card Sort. Maternal education was collected via caregiver report and served as a measure of socioeconomic status (SES).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Group comparisons confirmed that participants with DLD had lower language scores, nonverbal IQ scores, executive function scores, and SES relative to peers with typical language. Additionally, these variables were significantly positively correlated. Multiple mediation analyses indicated that both nonverbal IQ and receptive vocabulary were significant partial mediators of the effect of DLD status on executive function scores. However, a direct effect of DLD status on executive function remained significant in a model that included the mediators and covaried maternal education.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Group differences in nonverbal IQ and receptive vocabulary can partially explain why children with DLD tend to have lower executive function scores relative to peers with typical language. However, even after accounting for these mediators, there is a significant, large direct effect of DLD status on executive function performance, which suggests that the groups' difference in executive functioning is not fully explained by other variables.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"1853-1865"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143617741","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Progression of Developmental Language Disorder Terminology: A Scoping Review of American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Journals.
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-04-08 Epub Date: 2025-03-12 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00496
Miriam Kornelis, HaeJi Lee, Amy Riegelman, Lizbeth H Finestack

Purpose: A 2017 CATALISE project resulted in consensus on using the term "developmental language disorder" (DLD) to describe children with unexplained language impairment. Since then, it is unclear how researchers have identified DLD and implemented DLD terminology. The current study is a scoping review to better understand the implementation of DLD terminology.

Method: We identified studies (N = 265) published in American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) journals between 2017 and 2024 that included individuals with communication difficulties in the area of language not secondary to another etiology. We extracted key information regarding study purpose and participant identification from each publication.

Results: Across all publications, 58% included the term "DLD" to describe their participant(s), 22% included "specific language impairment" (SLI), 12% included "language impairment" (LI), and 8% included other terms. The majority of publications served to better understand the profile of individuals with DLD, evaluate an intervention, or report on a measurement tool related to identification of the condition. There was significant variation in the developmental domains (e.g., cognitive, social, sensory) considered when diagnosing DLD across research publications.

Conclusions: This scoping review demonstrates the adoption and implementation of the term "DLD" in ASHA publications. Future efforts should prioritize increasing the consistency of identification of DLD in research, advocating for use of DLD terminology in clinical contexts, and supporting clinicians' knowledge, identification, assessment, intervention, and advocacy related to DLD.

Supplemental material: https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.28508936.

{"title":"The Progression of Developmental Language Disorder Terminology: A Scoping Review of American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Journals.","authors":"Miriam Kornelis, HaeJi Lee, Amy Riegelman, Lizbeth H Finestack","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00496","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00496","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>A 2017 CATALISE project resulted in consensus on using the term \"developmental language disorder\" (DLD) to describe children with unexplained language impairment. Since then, it is unclear how researchers have identified DLD and implemented DLD terminology. The current study is a scoping review to better understand the implementation of DLD terminology.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We identified studies (<i>N</i> = 265) published in American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) journals between 2017 and 2024 that included individuals with communication difficulties in the area of language not secondary to another etiology. We extracted key information regarding study purpose and participant identification from each publication.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Across all publications, 58% included the term \"DLD\" to describe their participant(s), 22% included \"specific language impairment\" (SLI), 12% included \"language impairment\" (LI), and 8% included other terms. The majority of publications served to better understand the profile of individuals with DLD, evaluate an intervention, or report on a measurement tool related to identification of the condition. There was significant variation in the developmental domains (e.g., cognitive, social, sensory) considered when diagnosing DLD across research publications.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>This scoping review demonstrates the adoption and implementation of the term \"DLD\" in ASHA publications. Future efforts should prioritize increasing the consistency of identification of DLD in research, advocating for use of DLD terminology in clinical contexts, and supporting clinicians' knowledge, identification, assessment, intervention, and advocacy related to DLD.</p><p><strong>Supplemental material: </strong>https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.28508936.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"1837-1852"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143617748","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Meta-Analysis of Second Language Phonetic Training: Exploring Overall Effect and Moderating Factors.
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-04-08 Epub Date: 2025-03-19 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00432
Yao Yao, Mengjie He, Fei Chen, Jiaqiang Zhu

Purpose: Phonetic training has been found to be an effective way on second language (L2) learning, but the evidence is not conclusive regarding the effectiveness of different instructional approaches and possible interactions between language learners, training features, and outcome measures. This study aims to meta-analyze existing studies to provide an accurate estimate of the overall effect and investigate factors that may moderate its effectiveness.

Method: We conducted a systematic search in major databases, identified seven potential moderators, and conducted a random-effects model meta-analysis for each variable.

Results: A comprehensive literature search yielded 65 primary studies that involved 2,793 L2 learners, generating 223 effect sizes for between-groups contrasts. Results showed that phonetic training had a large positive effect on the enhancement of L2 phonetic competence, d = 0.762. Subsequent moderator analyses revealed that perceptual training showed a larger mean effect size compared to production training and combined training; phonetic training at the high school level exhibited the largest mean effect size among all educational levels; phonetic training, either perceptual training or production training, had a greater impact on improving learners' perception competence than production competence; and outcome measured by identification tasks generated the largest effect, followed by the combination of discrimination and identification tasks, subjective perception judgment, and discrimination tasks, while objective acoustic measurement yielded the smallest effect size.

Conclusions: Our study provided a quantitative synthesis of studies investigating the efficacy of L2 phonetic training and examined various moderating variables, which indicated the heterogeneity and limitations of research on this topic. The results highlighted the need for further investigation of the potential factors of L2 phonetic training and the relationship between perception and production.

目的:语音训练被认为是第二语言(L2)学习的一种有效方法,但关于不同教学方法的有效性以及语言学习者、训练特点和结果测量之间可能存在的相互作用,目前尚无定论。本研究旨在对现有研究进行元分析,以提供对总体效果的准确估计,并调查可能影响其效果的因素:我们在主要数据库中进行了系统检索,确定了七个潜在的调节因素,并对每个变量进行了随机效应模型元分析:通过全面的文献检索,我们获得了 65 项主要研究,涉及 2,793 名 L2 学习者,产生了 223 个组间对比效应大小。结果显示,语音训练对提高 L2 语音能力有很大的积极作用,d = 0.762。随后的调节因子分析显示,与发音训练和综合训练相比,感知训练的平均效应大小更大;在所有教育水平中,高中阶段的语音训练的平均效应大小最大;语音训练,无论是感知训练还是发音训练,对提高学习者的感知能力的影响都大于发音能力;通过辨别任务测量的结果产生的效应最大,其次是辨别和识别任务的组合、主观感知判断和辨别任务,而客观声学测量产生的效应最小:我们的研究对有关 L2 语音训练效果的研究进行了定量综述,并考察了各种调节变量,这表明了该主题研究的异质性和局限性。研究结果表明,有必要进一步研究第二语言语音训练的潜在因素以及感知与发音之间的关系。
{"title":"A Meta-Analysis of Second Language Phonetic Training: Exploring Overall Effect and Moderating Factors.","authors":"Yao Yao, Mengjie He, Fei Chen, Jiaqiang Zhu","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00432","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00432","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Phonetic training has been found to be an effective way on second language (L2) learning, but the evidence is not conclusive regarding the effectiveness of different instructional approaches and possible interactions between language learners, training features, and outcome measures. This study aims to meta-analyze existing studies to provide an accurate estimate of the overall effect and investigate factors that may moderate its effectiveness.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We conducted a systematic search in major databases, identified seven potential moderators, and conducted a random-effects model meta-analysis for each variable.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>A comprehensive literature search yielded 65 primary studies that involved 2,793 L2 learners, generating 223 effect sizes for between-groups contrasts. Results showed that phonetic training had a large positive effect on the enhancement of L2 phonetic competence, <i>d</i> = 0.762. Subsequent moderator analyses revealed that perceptual training showed a larger mean effect size compared to production training and combined training; phonetic training at the high school level exhibited the largest mean effect size among all educational levels; phonetic training, either perceptual training or production training, had a greater impact on improving learners' perception competence than production competence; and outcome measured by identification tasks generated the largest effect, followed by the combination of discrimination and identification tasks, subjective perception judgment, and discrimination tasks, while objective acoustic measurement yielded the smallest effect size.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Our study provided a quantitative synthesis of studies investigating the efficacy of L2 phonetic training and examined various moderating variables, which indicated the heterogeneity and limitations of research on this topic. The results highlighted the need for further investigation of the potential factors of L2 phonetic training and the relationship between perception and production.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"1784-1802"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143665325","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bilingual Heterogeneity in Speech: "Typical" Trends and "Atypical" Cases in Disfluency.
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-04-08 Epub Date: 2025-03-20 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00415
Sveta Fichman

Purpose: Bilingual children's speech often contains high percentages of disfluencies in both their languages; however, the distribution of disfluency types and the difference across bilinguals' two languages have received insufficient and inconsistent empirical support. The present research aims to profile "typical" bilingual disfluency phenomena while comparing across the two languages and examining the impact of language exposure as well as proficiency.

Method: The current research analyzed disfluency rates and types among 32 bilingual children aged 4-9 years speaking a heritage language (HL; English or Russian) and a societal language (SL; Hebrew). Children's language proficiency was examined using a receptive vocabulary task and a sentence repetition task in both HL and SL. Analyses of three speech samples (spontaneous speech, narrative telling, and narrative retelling) were conducted examining rates and types of stuttering-like disfluencies (SLDs) and other disfluencies (ODs).

Results: The percentage of SLDs was higher in SL than in HL, which was related to the amount of exposure, whereas the percentage of ODs was associated with the proficiency vocabulary score and age of onset of bilingualism. Analysis of individual profiles revealed that nine children had high SLDs in at least one language. This could lead to stuttering overdiagnosis. Children whose SLD rates were high in both languages showed low performance (1 SD below the group level) on proficiency tasks, whereas children with a high percentage of SLDs only in HL had intact proficiency.

Conclusions: The results reveal high individual variation in disfluencies and support the importance of SLD-OD distinction in research on bilingual disfluency. Clinical implications of the research suggest that assessment of bilingual speech should be conducted applying unique criteria, different from the existing monolingual norms.

{"title":"Bilingual Heterogeneity in Speech: \"Typical\" Trends and \"Atypical\" Cases in Disfluency.","authors":"Sveta Fichman","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00415","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00415","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Bilingual children's speech often contains high percentages of disfluencies in both their languages; however, the distribution of disfluency types and the difference across bilinguals' two languages have received insufficient and inconsistent empirical support. The present research aims to profile \"typical\" bilingual disfluency phenomena while comparing across the two languages and examining the impact of language exposure as well as proficiency.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>The current research analyzed disfluency rates and types among 32 bilingual children aged 4-9 years speaking a heritage language (HL; English or Russian) and a societal language (SL; Hebrew). Children's language proficiency was examined using a receptive vocabulary task and a sentence repetition task in both HL and SL. Analyses of three speech samples (spontaneous speech, narrative telling, and narrative retelling) were conducted examining rates and types of stuttering-like disfluencies (SLDs) and other disfluencies (ODs).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The percentage of SLDs was higher in SL than in HL, which was related to the amount of exposure, whereas the percentage of ODs was associated with the proficiency vocabulary score and age of onset of bilingualism. Analysis of individual profiles revealed that nine children had high SLDs in at least one language. This could lead to stuttering overdiagnosis. Children whose SLD rates were high in both languages showed low performance (1 <i>SD</i> below the group level) on proficiency tasks, whereas children with a high percentage of SLDs only in HL had intact proficiency.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The results reveal high individual variation in disfluencies and support the importance of SLD-OD distinction in research on bilingual disfluency. Clinical implications of the research suggest that assessment of bilingual speech should be conducted applying unique criteria, different from the existing monolingual norms.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"1691-1710"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143671647","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Insights Into the Effect of General Attentional State, Coarticulation, and Primed Speech Rate in Phoneme Production Time.
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-04-08 Epub Date: 2025-03-22 DOI: 10.1044/2025_JSLHR-24-00595
Montse Soberanes, Carlos A Pérez-Ramírez, M Florencia Assaneo

Purpose: This study aimed to identify how a set of predefined factors modulates phoneme articulation time within a speaker.

Method: We used a custom in-lab system that records lip muscle activity through electromyography signals, aligned with the produced speech, to measure phoneme articulation time. Twenty Spanish-speaking participants (12 females) were evaluated while producing sequences of a consonant-vowel syllable, with each sequence consisting of repeated articulations of either /pa/ or /pu/. Before starting the sequences, participants underwent a priming step with either a fast or slow speech rate. Additionally, the general attentional state level was assessed at the beginning, middle, and end of the protocol. To analyze the variability in the duration of /p/ and vowel articulation, we fitted individual linear mixed-models considering three factors: general attentional state level, priming rate, and coarticulation effects (for /p/, i.e., followed by /a/ or /u/) or phoneme identity (for vowels, i.e., being /a/ or /u/).

Results: We found that the level of general attentional state positively correlated with production time for both the consonant /p/ and the vowels. Additionally, /p/ production was influenced by the nature of the following vowel (i.e., coarticulation effects), while vowel production time was affected by the primed speech rate.

Conclusions: Phoneme duration appears to be influenced by both stable, speaker-specific characteristics (idiosyncratic traits) and internal, state-dependent factors related to the speaker's condition at the time of speech production. While some factors affect both consonants and vowels, others specifically modify only one of these types.

Supplemental material: https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.28608428.

{"title":"Insights Into the Effect of General Attentional State, Coarticulation, and Primed Speech Rate in Phoneme Production Time.","authors":"Montse Soberanes, Carlos A Pérez-Ramírez, M Florencia Assaneo","doi":"10.1044/2025_JSLHR-24-00595","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2025_JSLHR-24-00595","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>This study aimed to identify how a set of predefined factors modulates phoneme articulation time within a speaker.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We used a custom in-lab system that records lip muscle activity through electromyography signals, aligned with the produced speech, to measure phoneme articulation time. Twenty Spanish-speaking participants (12 females) were evaluated while producing sequences of a consonant-vowel syllable, with each sequence consisting of repeated articulations of either /pa/ or /pu/. Before starting the sequences, participants underwent a priming step with either a fast or slow speech rate. Additionally, the general attentional state level was assessed at the beginning, middle, and end of the protocol. To analyze the variability in the duration of /p/ and vowel articulation, we fitted individual linear mixed-models considering three factors: general attentional state level, priming rate, and coarticulation effects (for /p/, i.e., followed by /a/ or /u/) or phoneme identity (for vowels, i.e., being /a/ or /u/).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>We found that the level of general attentional state positively correlated with production time for both the consonant /p/ and the vowels. Additionally, /p/ production was influenced by the nature of the following vowel (i.e., coarticulation effects), while vowel production time was affected by the primed speech rate.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Phoneme duration appears to be influenced by both stable, speaker-specific characteristics (idiosyncratic traits) and internal, state-dependent factors related to the speaker's condition at the time of speech production. While some factors affect both consonants and vowels, others specifically modify only one of these types.</p><p><strong>Supplemental material: </strong>https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.28608428.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"1773-1783"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143694334","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Construct Validation of the Verb Naming Test for Aphasia.
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-04-08 Epub Date: 2025-03-31 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00236
Marianne Casilio, Gerasimos Fergadiotis, Sun-Joo Cho, Stacey Steel, Mikala Fleegle, Michael Walsh Dickey, William Hula

Purpose: Although there is widespread agreement pertaining to the cognitive processes underlying spoken word production, more generally in aphasia, multiple competing accounts exist regarding the processes involved for verb production, specifically. Some have speculated that suboptimal control of certain item properties (e.g., imageability) may be partially responsible for conflicting reports in the literature, yet there remains a dearth of research on the psychometric validation of verb production tests for aphasia. The purpose of the present study was to investigate the cognitive constructs underlying the Verb Naming Test (VNT), a relatively commonly used verb production test, by expanding upon an item response theory (IRT) modeling framework we previously described.

Method: Using an archival data set of 107 individuals with aphasia, we specified a series of IRT models to investigate whether item covariates (argument structure, imageability), person covariates (aphasia subtype, severity), and their interactions were predictive of VNT item response patterns.

Results: Across all models, covariates that were most strongly associated with lexical-semantic processing (imageability, aphasia severity) were significant predictors. In contrast, covariates that were most strongly associated with morphosyntactic processing (argument structure, aphasia subtype) were minimally predictive.

Conclusions: VNT item response patterns appear to be primarily explained by covariates representing lexical-semantic processing. In particular, we identified an important role of imageability, a covariate not controlled for in the VNT's item design, which both aligns with a body of prior research and further illustrates the challenge of differentiating morphosyntactic processing from lexical and semantic processes during word production.

Supplemental material: https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.28664669.

{"title":"Construct Validation of the Verb Naming Test for Aphasia.","authors":"Marianne Casilio, Gerasimos Fergadiotis, Sun-Joo Cho, Stacey Steel, Mikala Fleegle, Michael Walsh Dickey, William Hula","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00236","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00236","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Although there is widespread agreement pertaining to the cognitive processes underlying spoken word production, more generally in aphasia, multiple competing accounts exist regarding the processes involved for verb production, specifically. Some have speculated that suboptimal control of certain item properties (e.g., imageability) may be partially responsible for conflicting reports in the literature, yet there remains a dearth of research on the psychometric validation of verb production tests for aphasia. The purpose of the present study was to investigate the cognitive constructs underlying the Verb Naming Test (VNT), a relatively commonly used verb production test, by expanding upon an item response theory (IRT) modeling framework we previously described.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Using an archival data set of 107 individuals with aphasia, we specified a series of IRT models to investigate whether item covariates (argument structure, imageability), person covariates (aphasia subtype, severity), and their interactions were predictive of VNT item response patterns.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Across all models, covariates that were most strongly associated with lexical-semantic processing (imageability, aphasia severity) were significant predictors. In contrast, covariates that were most strongly associated with morphosyntactic processing (argument structure, aphasia subtype) were minimally predictive.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>VNT item response patterns appear to be primarily explained by covariates representing lexical-semantic processing. In particular, we identified an important role of imageability, a covariate not controlled for in the VNT's item design, which both aligns with a body of prior research and further illustrates the challenge of differentiating morphosyntactic processing from lexical and semantic processes during word production.</p><p><strong>Supplemental material: </strong>https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.28664669.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"1932-1949"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143755645","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Multisite Randomized Controlled Trial Comparing the Effectiveness of Two Self-Fit Methods to the Best-Practices Method of Hearing Aid Fitting.
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-04-08 Epub Date: 2025-03-20 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00423
Larry E Humes, Sumitrajit Dhar, Mary Meskan, Anna Pitman, Jasleen Singh
<p><strong>Trial design: </strong>This is a randomized multisite noninferiority comparative-effectiveness clinical trial with three parallel branches comparing a best-practices audiologist-fit method to two experimental self-fit (person-fit) methods. Outcomes were measured at 6 weeks and 6 months post-fit.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Participants: Five hundred eighty-four participants met the following inclusion criteria and enrolled: (a) age between 50 and 79 years; (b) never used or tried hearing aids previously; (c) can speak, read, and understand English well; (d) willing to purchase the study hearing aids for $650/pair; (e) no diagnosis of a memory or cognitive impairment; (f) 25-item Hearing Handicap Inventory for the Elderly (HHIE) score > 4; (g) Montreal Cognitive Assessment score ≥ 23; and (h) not excluded due to specific audiometric criteria. The audiometric exclusion criteria, based on air-conduction pure-tone thresholds, were as follows: (a) thresholds at all frequencies (250-8000 Hz) < 20 dB HL, for both ears (no hearing loss); (b) pure-tone average for 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz (PTA4) in the better ear > 50 dB HL (greater than moderate hearing loss); and (c) interaural difference > 20 dB at three or more frequencies or ≥ 40 dB at 500 or 1000 Hz. Interventions: Three groups received the same hearing aids fitted either by an audiologist using best practices (Group AB) or by themselves using one of two efficacious self-fit methods (Groups CD and EF). Objectives: The effectiveness of each of the self-fit methods, CD and EF, was hypothesized to be noninferior to the professional-fit method, AB. Outcomes: The primary outcome measure was the global score from the Profile of Hearing Aid Benefit (PHAB), and the secondary outcome measure was the benefit score (unaided - aided) for the 25-item HHIE. Randomization: Participants were stratified into one of three hearing loss categories based on better-ear PTA4: normal (≤ 20 dB HL), mild (20.1-35 dB HL), or moderate (35.1-50 dB HL). The random assignment of sequential enrollees within each hearing loss category to the three treatment groups made use of a site-specific pregenerated randomization list produced from a random-numbers table. Blinding: It was not possible to blind the participants as to the fitting method used, but the research personnel assessing the outcomes were blinded to the treatment group.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Numbers randomized: The 584 enrollees were randomized to one of the three treatment groups: AB (<i>n</i> = 190), CD (<i>n</i> = 193), and EF (<i>n</i> = 201).</p><p><p>Trial status: The trial began on November 1, 2020, and 6-month outcomes were obtained from the last participant on March 29, 2024. Numbers analyzed: At the 6-week measurement interval, outcome measures were completed for 182 AB, 172 CD, and 178 EF participants, representing 91.1% of the 584 individuals who enrolled. At the 6-month interval, completed outcomes were available for 166 AB, 148 C
试验设计:这是一项随机多地点非劣效性比较临床试验,有三个平行分支,比较听力学家验配的最佳实践方法和两种实验性自我验配(个人验配)方法。结果在验配后 6 周和 6 个月进行测量:参与者:五百八十四名参与者符合以下纳入标准并注册:(a) 年龄在 50 至 79 岁之间;(b) 以前从未使用或试用过助听器;(c) 能说英语、阅读英语和理解英语;(d) 愿意以 650 美元/对的价格购买助听器;(e) 没有记忆或认知障碍诊断;(f) 25 项老年人听力障碍量表 (HHIE) 评分大于 4 分;(g) 蒙特利尔认知评估评分≥ 23 分;(h) 没有因特定听力标准而被排除在外。基于气导纯音阈值的听力排除标准如下:(a) 双耳所有频率(250-8000 Hz)的阈值均小于 20 dB HL(无听力损失);(b) 较好耳 500、1000、2000 和 4000 Hz 的纯音平均值(PTA4)> 50 dB HL(中度以上听力损失);(c) 三个或三个以上频率的耳间差> 20 dB 或 500 或 1000 Hz 的耳间差≥ 40 dB。干预措施:三组接受相同的助听器,由听力学家采用最佳方法(AB 组)或自己采用两种有效的自我验配方法之一进行验配(CD 和 EF 组)。目标:假设 CD 和 EF 两种自我验配方法的效果均不逊于专业验配方法 AB。结果:主要结果指标是助听器益处档案(PHAB)的总体得分,次要结果指标是 25 项 HHIE 的益处得分(无助听-有助听)。随机分组:根据较好耳部 PTA4 将参与者分为三个听力损失类别:正常(≤ 20 dB HL)、轻度(20.1-35 dB HL)或中度(35.1-50 dB HL)。使用随机数字表中预先生成的特定地点随机列表,将每个听力损失类别中的连续参试者随机分配到三个治疗组。盲法:无法对参与者使用的验配方法进行盲测,但评估结果的研究人员对治疗组进行了盲测:随机数字:584 名参加者被随机分配到三个治疗组中的一组:AB组(n = 190)、CD组(n = 193)和EF组(n = 201):试验于 2020 年 11 月 1 日开始,2024 年 3 月 29 日从最后一名参与者处获得 6 个月的结果。分析人数在 6 周测量间隔期,182 名 AB 型、172 名 CD 型和 178 名 EF 型参与者完成了结果测量,占 584 名注册者的 91.1%。在 6 个月的测量间隔中,166 名 AB 型参与者、148 名 CD 型参与者和 151 名 EF 型参与者完成了结果测量,占 584 名原始注册者的 79.6%。结果:6周时,AB、CD和EF治疗组在主要结果测量(PHAB总分)上的引导平均值(N = 1,000)和95%置信区间(CIs)分别为16.4(95% CI [14.3,18.7])、16.8(95% CI [14.9,18.6])和15.2(95% CI [13.5,17.1])。6 个月时,AB、CD 和 EF 治疗组的主要结果得分分别为 15.5(95% CI [13.4,17.8])、16.3(95% CI [14.4,18.2])和 15.3(95% CI [13.5,17.3])。在两次测量间隔中,治疗组对主要或次要结果测量均无显著影响(P > .05)。危害:未观察到明显的不良事件或副作用:在配戴后 6 周和 6 个月的时间间隔内,两种自我配戴方法(CD、EF)所产生的结果(主要结果和次要结果)均不劣于专业配戴的最佳实践(AB)配戴方法。此外,无论采用哪种拟合方法,平均都能获得有临床意义的益处。补充材料:https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.28585145。
{"title":"A Multisite Randomized Controlled Trial Comparing the Effectiveness of Two Self-Fit Methods to the Best-Practices Method of Hearing Aid Fitting.","authors":"Larry E Humes, Sumitrajit Dhar, Mary Meskan, Anna Pitman, Jasleen Singh","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00423","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00423","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;&lt;strong&gt;Trial design: &lt;/strong&gt;This is a randomized multisite noninferiority comparative-effectiveness clinical trial with three parallel branches comparing a best-practices audiologist-fit method to two experimental self-fit (person-fit) methods. Outcomes were measured at 6 weeks and 6 months post-fit.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;&lt;strong&gt;Method: &lt;/strong&gt;Participants: Five hundred eighty-four participants met the following inclusion criteria and enrolled: (a) age between 50 and 79 years; (b) never used or tried hearing aids previously; (c) can speak, read, and understand English well; (d) willing to purchase the study hearing aids for $650/pair; (e) no diagnosis of a memory or cognitive impairment; (f) 25-item Hearing Handicap Inventory for the Elderly (HHIE) score &gt; 4; (g) Montreal Cognitive Assessment score ≥ 23; and (h) not excluded due to specific audiometric criteria. The audiometric exclusion criteria, based on air-conduction pure-tone thresholds, were as follows: (a) thresholds at all frequencies (250-8000 Hz) &lt; 20 dB HL, for both ears (no hearing loss); (b) pure-tone average for 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz (PTA4) in the better ear &gt; 50 dB HL (greater than moderate hearing loss); and (c) interaural difference &gt; 20 dB at three or more frequencies or ≥ 40 dB at 500 or 1000 Hz. Interventions: Three groups received the same hearing aids fitted either by an audiologist using best practices (Group AB) or by themselves using one of two efficacious self-fit methods (Groups CD and EF). Objectives: The effectiveness of each of the self-fit methods, CD and EF, was hypothesized to be noninferior to the professional-fit method, AB. Outcomes: The primary outcome measure was the global score from the Profile of Hearing Aid Benefit (PHAB), and the secondary outcome measure was the benefit score (unaided - aided) for the 25-item HHIE. Randomization: Participants were stratified into one of three hearing loss categories based on better-ear PTA4: normal (≤ 20 dB HL), mild (20.1-35 dB HL), or moderate (35.1-50 dB HL). The random assignment of sequential enrollees within each hearing loss category to the three treatment groups made use of a site-specific pregenerated randomization list produced from a random-numbers table. Blinding: It was not possible to blind the participants as to the fitting method used, but the research personnel assessing the outcomes were blinded to the treatment group.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;&lt;strong&gt;Results: &lt;/strong&gt;Numbers randomized: The 584 enrollees were randomized to one of the three treatment groups: AB (&lt;i&gt;n&lt;/i&gt; = 190), CD (&lt;i&gt;n&lt;/i&gt; = 193), and EF (&lt;i&gt;n&lt;/i&gt; = 201).&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Trial status: The trial began on November 1, 2020, and 6-month outcomes were obtained from the last participant on March 29, 2024. Numbers analyzed: At the 6-week measurement interval, outcome measures were completed for 182 AB, 172 CD, and 178 EF participants, representing 91.1% of the 584 individuals who enrolled. At the 6-month interval, completed outcomes were available for 166 AB, 148 C","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"2080-2103"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143671616","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Vocal Fold Kinematics and Convergent-Divergent Oscillatory Glottis: Basic Insights Using Mucosal Wave Modeling and Synthetic Kymograms.
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-04-08 Epub Date: 2025-03-07 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00251
Jan G Švec, S Pravin Kumar, Ondřej Vencálek, Sandhanakrishnan Ravichandran, Sarah Lehoux

Purpose: Owing to mucosal waves, the oscillatory glottis is ideally expected to be convergent during opening and divergent during closing. However, this does not necessarily hold for voice disorders. Here, we pave the way for recognizing COnvergent-DIvergent (CODI) waveforms quantitatively and study the kinematic conditions in which they occur.

Method: We simulated 3,125 laryngoscopic glottal waveforms using a kinematic vocal fold (VF) model and synthetic kymograms. We independently varied the oscillatory amplitudes of the upper and lower VF margins, AU and AL (0.1 to 1.1 mm), vertical phase difference (VPD; 0° to 125°), glottal halfwidth HW (-0.05 to 1.2 mm), and prephonatory glottal convergence angle ψCVG (-15° to 35°) to simulate normal and disordered conditions. We introduced the upper and lower margin quotients, QU and QL, quantifying the proportion of time when the upper margin is at the glottal edge during the opening, and when the lower margin is at the glottal edge during the closing, respectively. A CODI waveform was defined as the case when QU = QL = 1.

Results: The likelihood of obtaining the CODI waveform was highest when AU and AL were similar, ψCVG was close to 0, HW was below 0.45 mm, and VPD was larger than 50°. In 88% of the simulated cases, the waveforms did not fulfill the CODI conditions. In these cases, either the lower margin was hidden during some portion of the closing phase or the upper margin was not at the glottal edge during some portion of the opening phase.

Conclusion: The study provides the basis for a better understanding of the variability of glottal waveforms and the appearance of mucosal waves related to VF kinematics.

{"title":"Vocal Fold Kinematics and Convergent-Divergent Oscillatory Glottis: Basic Insights Using Mucosal Wave Modeling and Synthetic Kymograms.","authors":"Jan G Švec, S Pravin Kumar, Ondřej Vencálek, Sandhanakrishnan Ravichandran, Sarah Lehoux","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00251","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00251","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>Owing to mucosal waves, the oscillatory glottis is ideally expected to be convergent during opening and divergent during closing. However, this does not necessarily hold for voice disorders. Here, we pave the way for recognizing COnvergent-DIvergent (CODI) waveforms quantitatively and study the kinematic conditions in which they occur.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We simulated 3,125 laryngoscopic glottal waveforms using a kinematic vocal fold (VF) model and synthetic kymograms. We independently varied the oscillatory amplitudes of the upper and lower VF margins, <i>A</i><sub>U</sub> and <i>A</i><sub>L</sub> (0.1 to 1.1 mm), vertical phase difference (VPD; 0° to 125°), glottal halfwidth <i>H</i><sub>W</sub> (-0.05 to 1.2 mm), and prephonatory glottal convergence angle ψ<sub>CVG</sub> (-15° to 35°) to simulate normal and disordered conditions. We introduced the upper and lower margin quotients, <i>Q</i><sub>U</sub> and <i>Q</i><sub>L</sub>, quantifying the proportion of time when the upper margin is at the glottal edge during the opening, and when the lower margin is at the glottal edge during the closing, respectively. A CODI waveform was defined as the case when <i>Q</i><sub>U</sub> = <i>Q</i><sub>L</sub> = 1.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The likelihood of obtaining the CODI waveform was highest when <i>A</i><sub>U</sub> and <i>A</i><sub>L</sub> were similar, ψ<sub>CVG</sub> was close to 0, <i>H</i><sub>W</sub> was below 0.45 mm, and VPD was larger than 50°. In 88% of the simulated cases, the waveforms did not fulfill the CODI conditions. In these cases, either the lower margin was hidden during some portion of the closing phase or the upper margin was not at the glottal edge during some portion of the opening phase.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The study provides the basis for a better understanding of the variability of glottal waveforms and the appearance of mucosal waves related to VF kinematics.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"1602-1617"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143576014","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Validation of the Language ENvironment Analysis in Swedish Children.
IF 2.2 2区 医学 Q1 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-04-08 Epub Date: 2025-03-18 DOI: 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00275
Sandra Nilsson, Elisabet Östlund, Yvonne Thalén, Ulrika Löfkvist

Purpose: The Language ENvironment Analysis (LENA) is a technological tool designed for comprehensive recordings and automated analysis of young children's daily language and auditory environments. LENA recordings play a crucial role in both clinical interventions and research, offering insights into the amount of spoken language children are exposed to in their homes, including adult word count (AWC) and child vocalization count (CVC). Although LENA was initially developed for American English, it has been validated in various other languages. This study aimed to evaluate the reliability of LENA in a Swedish context by comparing its automatic estimates with human transcriptions.

Method: Thirty-six children aged 11-29 months participated in the validation study. A total of 540 min (9 hr) of recordings were transcribed by one experienced speech-language pathologist and two special educators. Interrater agreement over 150 min was notably higher for CVC (intraclass correlation coefficient [ICC] = .97, 95% confidence interval [CI] [.91, .99], r = .96, p < .001) compared to AWC, which yielded an ICC of .82 (95% CI [.15, .96], r = .80, p = .006).

Results: Both estimated CVC and AWC (n = 36) were significantly correlated with human transcriptions (r = .79 and .82, respectively; p < .001).

Conclusions: The findings align with prior validation studies, indicating that LENA is suitable for use in a Swedish context, particularly for families with children aged 11-29 months. Nonetheless, additional validation studies are necessary, particularly focusing on younger infants, to enhance our understanding of the tool's reliability in preverbal children and their caregivers.

{"title":"Validation of the Language ENvironment Analysis in Swedish Children.","authors":"Sandra Nilsson, Elisabet Östlund, Yvonne Thalén, Ulrika Löfkvist","doi":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00275","DOIUrl":"10.1044/2024_JSLHR-24-00275","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>The Language ENvironment Analysis (LENA) is a technological tool designed for comprehensive recordings and automated analysis of young children's daily language and auditory environments. LENA recordings play a crucial role in both clinical interventions and research, offering insights into the amount of spoken language children are exposed to in their homes, including adult word count (AWC) and child vocalization count (CVC). Although LENA was initially developed for American English, it has been validated in various other languages. This study aimed to evaluate the reliability of LENA in a Swedish context by comparing its automatic estimates with human transcriptions.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Thirty-six children aged 11-29 months participated in the validation study. A total of 540 min (9 hr) of recordings were transcribed by one experienced speech-language pathologist and two special educators. Interrater agreement over 150 min was notably higher for CVC (intraclass correlation coefficient [ICC] = .97, 95% confidence interval [CI] [.91, .99], <i>r</i> = .96, <i>p</i> < .001) compared to AWC, which yielded an ICC of .82 (95% CI [.15, .96], <i>r</i> = .80, <i>p</i> = .006).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Both estimated CVC and AWC (<i>n</i> = 36) were significantly correlated with human transcriptions (<i>r</i> = .79 and .82, respectively; <i>p</i> < .001).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The findings align with prior validation studies, indicating that LENA is suitable for use in a Swedish context, particularly for families with children aged 11-29 months. Nonetheless, additional validation studies are necessary, particularly focusing on younger infants, to enhance our understanding of the tool's reliability in preverbal children and their caregivers.</p>","PeriodicalId":51254,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research","volume":" ","pages":"1902-1916"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143659657","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1