首页 > 最新文献

Morphology最新文献

英文 中文
Morphological Reanalysis of Word-Formation Elements 词形要素的形态学再分析
IF 1.4 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-11-30 DOI: 10.51157/kmor.2023.25.2.139
Meng-xiao Wang
Recently, it has been frequently observed in the Korean language that a portion of a word actively participates in the process of word formation in a fixed form. However, there is a lack of clear discussions regarding the morphological status of this “fixed element” whether it has a morphological status, what kind of morphological status it possesses if it does, and how it is formed. Therefore, this study aims to argue that such “fixed elements” can have a morphological status. By focusing on the phenomenon where elements that were previously used as part of a word appear productively in the process of word formation and acquire new function, the study aims to typologize how they are formed and what they become based on morphological reanalysis. These results suggest that fixed elements that were not morphemes in the process of neologism formation become “combining form”.
最近,在韩语中经常可以观察到词的一部分以固定的形式积极参与构词过程。然而,关于这种 "固定语素 "的形态地位,它是否具有形态地位,如果具有形态地位,它具有什么样的形态地位,以及它是如何形成的,都缺乏明确的讨论。因此,本研究旨在论证这种 "固定元素 "可以具有形态地位。本研究通过关注以前作为词的一部分的语素在组词过程中出现并获得新功能的现象,旨在根据形态学再分析,对这些语素的形成过程及其功能进行类型化。这些结果表明,在新词形成过程中,原本不是语素的固定语素变成了 "组合形式"。
{"title":"Morphological Reanalysis of Word-Formation Elements","authors":"Meng-xiao Wang","doi":"10.51157/kmor.2023.25.2.139","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.51157/kmor.2023.25.2.139","url":null,"abstract":"Recently, it has been frequently observed in the Korean language that a portion of a word actively participates in the process of word formation in a fixed form. However, there is a lack of clear discussions regarding the morphological status of this “fixed element” whether it has a morphological status, what kind of morphological status it possesses if it does, and how it is formed. Therefore, this study aims to argue that such “fixed elements” can have a morphological status. By focusing on the phenomenon where elements that were previously used as part of a word appear productively in the process of word formation and acquire new function, the study aims to typologize how they are formed and what they become based on morphological reanalysis. These results suggest that fixed elements that were not morphemes in the process of neologism formation become “combining form”.","PeriodicalId":51849,"journal":{"name":"Morphology","volume":"134 ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139205209","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A study on the non-epistemic modal meaning of ‘-l pephata’ and ‘-l manhata’ 关于"-l pephata "和"-l manhata "的非表义情态意义的研究
IF 1.4 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-11-30 DOI: 10.51157/kmor.2023.25.2.167
Jonghyeok Lee
The purpose of this paper is to identify semantic characteristics of non-epistemic modality realized by ‘-l pephata’ and ‘-l manhata’. Modality can be divided into epistemic modality and root modality, which is non-epistemic modality. Epistemic modality expresses a speaker's judgment on the degree of factuality of the proposition, and root modality expresses the speaker's judgment on the likelihood of the state-of-affair. Some previous studies have seen that ‘-l pephata’ and ‘-l manhata’ express the judgment that it is possible for a situation to occur, which can be seen as pointing out that ‘-l pephata’ and ‘-l manhata’ represent the root modality. Root possibility meaning of ‘-l pephata’ and ‘-l manhata’ is analyzed by three criteria: ‘scope of the modal meaning’, ‘source of possibility’, and ‘potential barrier’. Root possibility expressed by ‘-l pephata’ corresponds to the [general situation possibility], which is wide-scope, has an external source, and has no potential barrier. In addition to the [general situation possibility], ‘-l manhata’ can express [Ability] which is narrow-scope, has an internal source. ‘-l pephata’ and ‘-l manhata’ which express [general situation possibility] differs in that ‘-l pephata’ expresses a judgement based on assimilated knowledge, whereas ‘-l manhata’ expresses a judgement based on unassimilated knowledge.
本文旨在确定"-l pephata "和"-l manhata "实现的非表义模态的语义特征。模态可分为认识模态和词根模态(即非认识模态)。认识模态表达了说话人对命题事实性程度的判断,而根模态表达了说话人对事态可能性的判断。以往的一些研究认为,"-l pephata "和"-l manhata "表达的是对某一情境有可能发生的判断,这可以看作是指出了"-l pephata "和"-l manhata "代表的是词根模态。通过三个标准来分析"-l pephata "和"-l manhata "的词根可能性意义:模态意义的范围"、"可能性的来源 "和 "潜在障碍"。由'-l pephata'表达的根可能性对应于[一般情况可能性],它范围广,有外部来源,没有潜在障碍。除了[一般情况可能性],'-l manhata'还可以表达[能力],它范围窄,有内部来源。表达[一般情况可能性]的'-l pephata'和'-l manhata'的不同之处在于,'-l pephata'表达的是基于同化知识的判断,而'-l manhata'表达的是基于非同化知识的判断。
{"title":"A study on the non-epistemic modal meaning of ‘-l pephata’ and ‘-l manhata’","authors":"Jonghyeok Lee","doi":"10.51157/kmor.2023.25.2.167","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.51157/kmor.2023.25.2.167","url":null,"abstract":"The purpose of this paper is to identify semantic characteristics of non-epistemic modality realized by ‘-l pephata’ and ‘-l manhata’. Modality can be divided into epistemic modality and root modality, which is non-epistemic modality. Epistemic modality expresses a speaker's judgment on the degree of factuality of the proposition, and root modality expresses the speaker's judgment on the likelihood of the state-of-affair. Some previous studies have seen that ‘-l pephata’ and ‘-l manhata’ express the judgment that it is possible for a situation to occur, which can be seen as pointing out that ‘-l pephata’ and ‘-l manhata’ represent the root modality. Root possibility meaning of ‘-l pephata’ and ‘-l manhata’ is analyzed by three criteria: ‘scope of the modal meaning’, ‘source of possibility’, and ‘potential barrier’. Root possibility expressed by ‘-l pephata’ corresponds to the [general situation possibility], which is wide-scope, has an external source, and has no potential barrier. In addition to the [general situation possibility], ‘-l manhata’ can express [Ability] which is narrow-scope, has an internal source. ‘-l pephata’ and ‘-l manhata’ which express [general situation possibility] differs in that ‘-l pephata’ expresses a judgement based on assimilated knowledge, whereas ‘-l manhata’ expresses a judgement based on unassimilated knowledge.","PeriodicalId":51849,"journal":{"name":"Morphology","volume":"17 5 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139198076","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Suffixal taeng-i derivatives denoting pejoration in Korean 韩语中表示同居的后缀 taeng-i 衍生物
IF 1.4 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-11-30 DOI: 10.51157/kmor.2023.25.2.117
Yu-seok Kwak
The purpose of this paper is to explain the semantic and formal characteristics of ‘taeng-i'. ‘taeng-i’ is widely used as a suffix in ‘pejoration’. However, aside from the meaning ‘pejoration’, it is also used in ‘emphasis’, even for ‘affection’ and ‘intimacy’, presenting positive connotations. Like so, the meaning of ‘taeng-i’ can include from ‘pejoration’ and ‘emphasis’ to ‘affection’ and ‘intimacy’ because of their commonality in which the speaker uses such words to evaluate a subject. On the other hand, the most reasonable explanation of how the term ‘taeng-i’ was created is that it derived from diminutive. This is to say, from a cross-linguistic perspective, diminutives also include meaning from ‘pejoration’ and ‘emphasis’ to ‘affection’ and ‘intimacy’. Moreover, there is an important factor to consider that ‘taeng-i’ shares the base with other similar suffixes in its form and meaning such as ‘ttaeng-i, pang-i, tung-i, ‘jab-i, jaeng-i, tae-gi, du-la-ji, dung-a-li, and dung-i’ to form synonyms.
本文旨在解释 "taeng-i "的语义和形式特征。taeng-i "被广泛用作 "pejoration "的后缀。然而,除了 "pejoration "的含义外,它还用于 "强调",甚至用于 "感情 "和 "亲密",呈现出积极的含义。因此,"taeng-i "的意义可以包括从 "pejoration "和 "rephasis "到 "faffection "和 "intemacy",因为它们有共同点,即说话人用这些词来评价一个主题。另一方面,关于 "taeng-i "一词是如何产生的,最合理的解释是它源于 diminutive。这就是说,从跨语言的角度来看,"taeng-i "也包含从 "pejoration "和 "强调 "到 "affection "和 "intemacy "的意思。此外,还有一个重要因素需要考虑,即 "taeng-i "在形式和意义上与其他类似的后缀如 "ttaeng-i、pang-i、tung-i、'jab-i、jaeng-i、tae-gi、du-la-ji、dung-a-li 和 dung-i "共享词基,形成同义词。
{"title":"Suffixal taeng-i derivatives denoting pejoration in Korean","authors":"Yu-seok Kwak","doi":"10.51157/kmor.2023.25.2.117","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.51157/kmor.2023.25.2.117","url":null,"abstract":"The purpose of this paper is to explain the semantic and formal characteristics of ‘taeng-i'. ‘taeng-i’ is widely used as a suffix in ‘pejoration’. However, aside from the meaning ‘pejoration’, it is also used in ‘emphasis’, even for ‘affection’ and ‘intimacy’, presenting positive connotations. Like so, the meaning of ‘taeng-i’ can include from ‘pejoration’ and ‘emphasis’ to ‘affection’ and ‘intimacy’ because of their commonality in which the speaker uses such words to evaluate a subject. On the other hand, the most reasonable explanation of how the term ‘taeng-i’ was created is that it derived from diminutive. This is to say, from a cross-linguistic perspective, diminutives also include meaning from ‘pejoration’ and ‘emphasis’ to ‘affection’ and ‘intimacy’. Moreover, there is an important factor to consider that ‘taeng-i’ shares the base with other similar suffixes in its form and meaning such as ‘ttaeng-i, pang-i, tung-i, ‘jab-i, jaeng-i, tae-gi, du-la-ji, dung-a-li, and dung-i’ to form synonyms.","PeriodicalId":51849,"journal":{"name":"Morphology","volume":"93 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139205536","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Processing of grammatical gender agreement morphemes in Polish: evidence from the Visual World Paradigm 波兰语语法性别一致语素的加工:来自视觉世界范式的证据
0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-10-13 DOI: 10.1007/s11525-023-09418-3
Zuzanna Fuchs
Abstract This paper presents a psycholinguistic study of the processing of grammatical gender agreement morphemes in Polish, which has three gender categories (masculine, feminine, neuter), as well as what language-internal factors impact this processing. Results from an eye-tracking study using the Visual World Paradigm show that, during real-time language comprehension, adult monolingual speakers of Polish use cues from gender agreement on a prenominal adjective to anticipate the upcoming noun. An exploration of language-internal factors affecting this anticipatory processing finds this effect in all three genders, suggesting that encountering the relevant nominative-case agreement morpheme during language comprehension leads to automatic activation of a gender node in the mental lexicon, consistent with the literature on other languages with grammatical gender. These results hold true for the neuter agreement morpheme, despite the fact that this morpheme also instantiates default gender agreement in the language and is syncretic with the nominative plural agreement morpheme in all three genders. Further investigation finds that, while agreement morphemes for each gender prompt anticipatory processing, the reliability of a masculine agreement morpheme as a cue to gender is reduced in the presence of a neuter distractor, and vice versa. This raises questions regarding phonological proximity between the realized suffix and the suffix that would cue the distractor, with implications for the acquisition and processing of gender agreement morphology in Polish.
摘要本文从心理语言学的角度研究了波兰语中三个性别类别(阳性、阴性、中性)的语法性别一致语素的加工,以及语言内部因素对这种加工的影响。一项使用视觉世界范式的眼动追踪研究结果表明,在实时语言理解过程中,波兰语单语成年人使用对前名形容词的性别认同来预测即将到来的名词。对影响这种预期加工的语言内部因素的探索发现,这一效应在所有三种性别中都存在,这表明在语言理解过程中遇到相关的主格一致语素会导致心理词汇中的性别节点自动激活,这与其他具有语法性别的语言的文献一致。这些结果适用于中性一致语素,尽管该语素也实例化了语言中默认的性别一致,并与所有三种性别的主复数一致语素融合在一起。进一步的研究发现,虽然每种性别的一致语素都能促进预期加工,但在存在中性干扰物的情况下,男性一致语素作为性别线索的可靠性会降低,反之亦然。这就提出了关于已实现后缀和提示干扰物的后缀之间的语音接近性的问题,并对波兰语性别一致形态的习得和加工产生了影响。
{"title":"Processing of grammatical gender agreement morphemes in Polish: evidence from the Visual World Paradigm","authors":"Zuzanna Fuchs","doi":"10.1007/s11525-023-09418-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-023-09418-3","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This paper presents a psycholinguistic study of the processing of grammatical gender agreement morphemes in Polish, which has three gender categories (masculine, feminine, neuter), as well as what language-internal factors impact this processing. Results from an eye-tracking study using the Visual World Paradigm show that, during real-time language comprehension, adult monolingual speakers of Polish use cues from gender agreement on a prenominal adjective to anticipate the upcoming noun. An exploration of language-internal factors affecting this anticipatory processing finds this effect in all three genders, suggesting that encountering the relevant nominative-case agreement morpheme during language comprehension leads to automatic activation of a gender node in the mental lexicon, consistent with the literature on other languages with grammatical gender. These results hold true for the neuter agreement morpheme, despite the fact that this morpheme also instantiates default gender agreement in the language and is syncretic with the nominative plural agreement morpheme in all three genders. Further investigation finds that, while agreement morphemes for each gender prompt anticipatory processing, the reliability of a masculine agreement morpheme as a cue to gender is reduced in the presence of a neuter distractor, and vice versa. This raises questions regarding phonological proximity between the realized suffix and the suffix that would cue the distractor, with implications for the acquisition and processing of gender agreement morphology in Polish.","PeriodicalId":51849,"journal":{"name":"Morphology","volume":"155 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135855402","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are some morphological units more prone to spelling variation than others? A case study using spontaneous handwritten data 是否有些形态单位比其他单位更容易发生拼写变化?一个使用自发手写数据的案例研究
0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-10-09 DOI: 10.1007/s11525-023-09417-4
Kristian Berg, Stefan Hartmann, Daniel Claeser
Abstract The relation between morphology and spelling is an important source of evidence for theories of linguistic processing. In particular, spelling errors can help us assess the role of morphological structure in language users’ mental representations of words in authentic texts. Previous research suggests that some morphological units are more prone to spelling errors than others, partly depending on the degree to which they are perceived as separate units. In this paper, we want to test this hypothesis by exploring graphemic variation in a collection of 1,667 German school-exit exams. Specifically, we code the spelling errors for their morphological structure. We can show that inflectional suffixes show a much higher probability of final letter omissions compared to final stems or derivational suffixes. We also find tentative evidence that case markers are more often affected by omissions than number markers.
词法与拼写的关系是语言加工理论的重要证据来源。特别是,拼写错误可以帮助我们评估词形结构在真实文本中语言使用者对单词的心理表征中的作用。先前的研究表明,一些词形单位比其他单位更容易出现拼写错误,这部分取决于它们被视为独立单位的程度。在本文中,我们想通过探索1667份德国学校毕业考试的文字差异来检验这一假设。具体来说,我们根据拼写错误的形态结构对其进行编码。我们可以证明,与词干或衍生后缀相比,屈折后缀显示出最终字母遗漏的可能性要高得多。我们还发现初步证据表明,大小写标记比数字标记更容易受到省略的影响。
{"title":"Are some morphological units more prone to spelling variation than others? A case study using spontaneous handwritten data","authors":"Kristian Berg, Stefan Hartmann, Daniel Claeser","doi":"10.1007/s11525-023-09417-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-023-09417-4","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The relation between morphology and spelling is an important source of evidence for theories of linguistic processing. In particular, spelling errors can help us assess the role of morphological structure in language users’ mental representations of words in authentic texts. Previous research suggests that some morphological units are more prone to spelling errors than others, partly depending on the degree to which they are perceived as separate units. In this paper, we want to test this hypothesis by exploring graphemic variation in a collection of 1,667 German school-exit exams. Specifically, we code the spelling errors for their morphological structure. We can show that inflectional suffixes show a much higher probability of final letter omissions compared to final stems or derivational suffixes. We also find tentative evidence that case markers are more often affected by omissions than number markers.","PeriodicalId":51849,"journal":{"name":"Morphology","volume":"99 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135044418","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Morphology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1