首页 > 最新文献

Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science最新文献

英文 中文
iCatcher+: Robust and Automated Annotation of Infants' and Young Children's Gaze Behavior From Videos Collected in Laboratory, Field, and Online Studies. iCatcher+:从实验室、现场和在线研究中收集的视频中对婴幼儿凝视行为进行稳健和自动的注释。
IF 13.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-18 DOI: 10.1177/25152459221147250
Yotam Erel, Katherine Adams Shannon, Junyi Chu, Kim Scott, Melissa Kline Struhl, Peng Cao, Xincheng Tan, Peter Hart, Gal Raz, Sabrina Piccolo, Catherine Mei, Christine Potter, Sagi Jaffe-Dax, Casey Lew-Williams, Joshua Tenenbaum, Katherine Fairchild, Amit Bermano, Shari Liu

Technological advances in psychological research have enabled large-scale studies of human behavior and streamlined pipelines for automatic processing of data. However, studies of infants and children have not fully reaped these benefits because the behaviors of interest, such as gaze duration and direction, still have to be extracted from video through a laborious process of manual annotation, even when these data are collected online. Recent advances in computer vision raise the possibility of automated annotation of these video data. In this article, we built on a system for automatic gaze annotation in young children, iCatcher, by engineering improvements and then training and testing the system (referred to hereafter as iCatcher+) on three data sets with substantial video and participant variability (214 videos collected in U.S. lab and field sites, 143 videos collected in Senegal field sites, and 265 videos collected via webcams in homes; participant age range = 4 months-3.5 years). When trained on each of these data sets, iCatcher+ performed with near human-level accuracy on held-out videos on distinguishing "LEFT" versus "RIGHT" and "ON" versus "OFF" looking behavior across all data sets. This high performance was achieved at the level of individual frames, experimental trials, and study videos; held across participant demographics (e.g., age, race/ethnicity), participant behavior (e.g., movement, head position), and video characteristics (e.g., luminance); and generalized to a fourth, entirely held-out online data set. We close by discussing next steps required to fully automate the life cycle of online infant and child behavioral studies, representing a key step toward enabling robust and high-throughput developmental research.

心理学研究的技术进步使人们能够对人类行为进行大规模研究,并简化了数据自动处理的管道。然而,对婴儿和儿童的研究并没有完全获得这些好处,因为感兴趣的行为,如凝视持续时间和方向,仍然需要通过手动注释的费力过程从视频中提取,即使这些数据是在网上收集的。计算机视觉的最新进展提高了对这些视频数据进行自动注释的可能性。在本文中,我们构建了一个用于幼儿凝视自动注释的系统iCatcher,通过工程改进,然后在具有显著视频和参与者可变性的三个数据集上对系统(以下简称为iCatcher+)进行培训和测试(214个视频在美国实验室和现场采集,143个视频在塞内加尔现场采集,265个视频通过家庭网络摄像头采集;参与者年龄范围=4个月-3.5岁)。当在这些数据集上进行训练时,iCatcher+在所有数据集上以接近人类水平的准确度对手持视频进行了区分“左”与“右”以及“开”与“关”的行为。这种高性能是在单个帧、实验试验和研究视频的水平上实现的;在参与者人口统计数据(例如,年龄、种族/民族)、参与者行为(例如,运动、头部位置)和视频特征(例如,亮度)中进行;并推广到第四个完全公开的在线数据集。最后,我们讨论了完全自动化在线婴儿和儿童行为研究生命周期所需的下一步,这是实现稳健和高通量发展研究的关键一步。
{"title":"iCatcher+: Robust and Automated Annotation of Infants' and Young Children's Gaze Behavior From Videos Collected in Laboratory, Field, and Online Studies.","authors":"Yotam Erel,&nbsp;Katherine Adams Shannon,&nbsp;Junyi Chu,&nbsp;Kim Scott,&nbsp;Melissa Kline Struhl,&nbsp;Peng Cao,&nbsp;Xincheng Tan,&nbsp;Peter Hart,&nbsp;Gal Raz,&nbsp;Sabrina Piccolo,&nbsp;Catherine Mei,&nbsp;Christine Potter,&nbsp;Sagi Jaffe-Dax,&nbsp;Casey Lew-Williams,&nbsp;Joshua Tenenbaum,&nbsp;Katherine Fairchild,&nbsp;Amit Bermano,&nbsp;Shari Liu","doi":"10.1177/25152459221147250","DOIUrl":"10.1177/25152459221147250","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Technological advances in psychological research have enabled large-scale studies of human behavior and streamlined pipelines for automatic processing of data. However, studies of infants and children have not fully reaped these benefits because the behaviors of interest, such as gaze duration and direction, still have to be extracted from video through a laborious process of manual annotation, even when these data are collected online. Recent advances in computer vision raise the possibility of automated annotation of these video data. In this article, we built on a system for automatic gaze annotation in young children, iCatcher, by engineering improvements and then training and testing the system (referred to hereafter as iCatcher+) on three data sets with substantial video and participant variability (214 videos collected in U.S. lab and field sites, 143 videos collected in Senegal field sites, and 265 videos collected via webcams in homes; participant age range = 4 months-3.5 years). When trained on each of these data sets, iCatcher+ performed with near human-level accuracy on held-out videos on distinguishing \"LEFT\" versus \"RIGHT\" and \"ON\" versus \"OFF\" looking behavior across all data sets. This high performance was achieved at the level of individual frames, experimental trials, and study videos; held across participant demographics (e.g., age, race/ethnicity), participant behavior (e.g., movement, head position), and video characteristics (e.g., luminance); and generalized to a fourth, entirely held-out online data set. We close by discussing next steps required to fully automate the life cycle of online infant and child behavioral studies, representing a key step toward enabling robust and high-throughput developmental research.</p>","PeriodicalId":55645,"journal":{"name":"Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science","volume":"6 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":13.6,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/55/6e/nihms-1916587.PMC10471135.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10152159","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beyond the Mean: Can We Improve the Predictive Power of Psychometric Scales? 超越平均值:我们能提高心理量表的预测能力吗?
IF 13.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/25152459231177713
Yngwie Asbjørn Nielsen, Isabel Thielmann, Stefan Pfattheicher
Two participants completing a psychometric scale may leave wildly different responses yet attain the same mean score. Moreover, the mean score often does not represent the bulk of participants’ responses, which may be skewed, kurtotic, or bimodal. Even so, researchers in psychological science often aggregate item scores using an unweighted mean or a sum score, thereby neglecting a substantial amount of information. In the present contribution, we explore whether other summary statistics of a scale (e.g., the standard deviation, the median, or the kurtosis) can capture and leverage some of this neglected information to improve prediction of a broad range of outcome measures: life satisfaction, mental health, self-esteem, counterproductive work behavior, and social value orientation. Overall, across 32 psychometric scales and three data sets (total N = 8,376), we show that the mean is the strongest predictor of all five outcomes considered, with little to no additional variance explained by other summary statistics. These results provide justification for the current practice of relying on the mean score but hopefully inspire future research to explore the predictive power of other summary statistics for relevant outcomes. For this purpose, we provide a tutorial and example code for R.
完成心理测量量表的两个参与者可能会留下截然不同的回答,但得到相同的平均得分。此外,平均得分往往不能代表大部分参与者的反应,这可能是偏斜的,峰度的,或双峰。即便如此,心理科学的研究人员经常使用未加权的平均值或总和得分来汇总项目得分,从而忽略了大量的信息。在目前的贡献中,我们探讨了量表的其他汇总统计(例如,标准差,中位数或峰度)是否可以捕获和利用这些被忽视的信息来改进对广泛结果测量的预测:生活满意度,心理健康,自尊,反生产行为和社会价值取向。总的来说,在32个心理测量量表和3个数据集(总N = 8376)中,我们表明平均值是所有考虑的五种结果的最强预测因子,其他汇总统计数据几乎没有解释额外的方差。这些结果为目前依赖平均分的做法提供了理由,但希望能启发未来的研究,以探索其他汇总统计对相关结果的预测能力。为此,我们提供了R的教程和示例代码。
{"title":"Beyond the Mean: Can We Improve the Predictive Power of Psychometric Scales?","authors":"Yngwie Asbjørn Nielsen, Isabel Thielmann, Stefan Pfattheicher","doi":"10.1177/25152459231177713","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459231177713","url":null,"abstract":"Two participants completing a psychometric scale may leave wildly different responses yet attain the same mean score. Moreover, the mean score often does not represent the bulk of participants’ responses, which may be skewed, kurtotic, or bimodal. Even so, researchers in psychological science often aggregate item scores using an unweighted mean or a sum score, thereby neglecting a substantial amount of information. In the present contribution, we explore whether other summary statistics of a scale (e.g., the standard deviation, the median, or the kurtosis) can capture and leverage some of this neglected information to improve prediction of a broad range of outcome measures: life satisfaction, mental health, self-esteem, counterproductive work behavior, and social value orientation. Overall, across 32 psychometric scales and three data sets (total N = 8,376), we show that the mean is the strongest predictor of all five outcomes considered, with little to no additional variance explained by other summary statistics. These results provide justification for the current practice of relying on the mean score but hopefully inspire future research to explore the predictive power of other summary statistics for relevant outcomes. For this purpose, we provide a tutorial and example code for R.","PeriodicalId":55645,"journal":{"name":"Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":13.6,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46022858","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Primer on Structural Equation Model Diagrams and Directed Acyclic Graphs: When and How to Use Each in Psychological and Epidemiological Research 结构方程模型图和有向无环图的初步研究:何时以及如何在心理和流行病学研究中使用它们
IF 13.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/25152459231156085
Zachary J. Kunicki, Meghan L. Smith, E. Murray
Many psychological researchers use some form of a visual diagram in their research processes. Model diagrams used with structural equation models (SEMs) and causal directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) can guide causal-inference research. SEM diagrams and DAGs share visual similarities, often leading researchers familiar with one to wonder how the other differs. This article is intended to serve as a guide for researchers in the psychological sciences and psychiatric epidemiology on the distinctions between these methods. We offer high-level overviews of SEMs and causal DAGs using a guiding example. We then compare and contrast the two methodologies and describe when each would be used. In brief, SEM diagrams are both a conceptual and statistical tool in which a model is drawn and then tested, whereas causal DAGs are exclusively conceptual tools used to help guide researchers in developing an analytic strategy and interpreting results. Causal DAGs are explicitly tools for causal inference, whereas the results of a SEM are only sometimes interpreted causally. A DAG may be thought of as a “qualitative schematic” for some SEMs, whereas SEMs may be thought of as an “algebraic system” for a causal DAG. As psychology begins to adopt more causal-modeling concepts and psychiatric epidemiology begins to adopt more latent-variable concepts, the ability of researchers to understand and possibly combine both of these tools is valuable. Using an applied example, we provide sample analyses, code, and write-ups for both SEM and causal DAG approaches.
许多心理学研究人员在研究过程中使用某种形式的视觉图表。与结构方程模型(SEM)和因果有向无环图(DAG)一起使用的模型图可以指导因果推理研究。SEM图和DAG在视觉上有着共同的相似之处,经常让熟悉其中一种的研究人员想知道另一种是如何不同的。本文旨在为心理科学和精神流行病学的研究人员提供关于这些方法之间区别的指南。我们通过一个指导性的例子对SEM和因果DAG进行了高层次的概述。然后,我们比较和对比这两种方法,并描述何时使用每种方法。简言之,SEM图既是一种概念工具,也是一种统计工具,在其中绘制模型并进行测试,而因果DAG只是一种概念性工具,用于帮助指导研究人员制定分析策略和解释结果。因果DAG是因果推断的明确工具,而SEM的结果有时只是因果解释。DAG可以被认为是一些SEM的“定性示意图”,而SEM可以被认为为因果DAG的“代数系统”。随着心理学开始采用更多的因果建模概念,精神流行病学开始采用更多潜在变量概念,研究人员理解并可能结合这两种工具的能力是有价值的。使用一个应用示例,我们提供了SEM和因果DAG方法的样本分析、代码和总结。
{"title":"A Primer on Structural Equation Model Diagrams and Directed Acyclic Graphs: When and How to Use Each in Psychological and Epidemiological Research","authors":"Zachary J. Kunicki, Meghan L. Smith, E. Murray","doi":"10.1177/25152459231156085","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459231156085","url":null,"abstract":"Many psychological researchers use some form of a visual diagram in their research processes. Model diagrams used with structural equation models (SEMs) and causal directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) can guide causal-inference research. SEM diagrams and DAGs share visual similarities, often leading researchers familiar with one to wonder how the other differs. This article is intended to serve as a guide for researchers in the psychological sciences and psychiatric epidemiology on the distinctions between these methods. We offer high-level overviews of SEMs and causal DAGs using a guiding example. We then compare and contrast the two methodologies and describe when each would be used. In brief, SEM diagrams are both a conceptual and statistical tool in which a model is drawn and then tested, whereas causal DAGs are exclusively conceptual tools used to help guide researchers in developing an analytic strategy and interpreting results. Causal DAGs are explicitly tools for causal inference, whereas the results of a SEM are only sometimes interpreted causally. A DAG may be thought of as a “qualitative schematic” for some SEMs, whereas SEMs may be thought of as an “algebraic system” for a causal DAG. As psychology begins to adopt more causal-modeling concepts and psychiatric epidemiology begins to adopt more latent-variable concepts, the ability of researchers to understand and possibly combine both of these tools is valuable. Using an applied example, we provide sample analyses, code, and write-ups for both SEM and causal DAG approaches.","PeriodicalId":55645,"journal":{"name":"Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":13.6,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42921040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Corrigendum: Journal N-Pact Factors From 2011 to 2019: Evaluating the Quality of Social/Personality Journals With Respect to Sample Size and Statistical Power 更正:2011年至2019年的期刊N-Pact因素:根据样本量和统计能力评估社会/个性期刊的质量
IF 13.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/25152459231175075
JRP 330 238 (17) 438 (14) 325 (26) 302 (15) 369 (14) 280 (11) 330 (18) 500 (20) 438 (22) EJP 261 111 (13) 194 (16) 392 (14) 217 (7) 200 (12) 261 (15) 422 (15) 576 (11) 496 (10) JP 251 239 (14) 198 (22) 354 (16) 359 (10) 406 (19) 239 (23) 251 (11) 240 (44) 286 (30) PS:S 200 72 (26) 82 (24) 174 (20) 104 (21) 200 (36) 231 (35) 248 (23) 386 (52) 202 (32) SPPS 186 128 (26) 134 (24) 210 (25) 172 (26) 186 (38) 178 (29) 300 (39) 364 (26) 383 (33) JPSP 179 98 (70) 108 (75) 102 (73) 116 (71) 220 (64) 179 (81) 225 (77) 225 (75) 320 (45) PSPB 139 105 (51) 78 (47) 117 (50) 151 (62) 130 (84) 139 (57) 186 (74) 220 (51) 208 (51) EJSP 131 96 (31) 93 (29) 78 (24) 139 (29) 131 (34) 126 (30) 219 (27) 153 (42) 169 (60) JESP 113 94 (66) 69 (53) 120 (68) 92 (56) 113 (89) 102 (46) 204 (60) 206 (81) 275 (70)
JRP 330 238(17)438(14)325(26)302(15)369(14)280(11)330(18)500(20)438)248(23)386(52)202(32)SPPS 186 128(26)134(24)210(25)172(26)186(38)178(29)300(39)364(26)383(33)JPSP 179 98(70)108(75)102(73)116(71)220(64)179(81)225(77)225(75)320(45)PSPB 139 105(51)78(47)117(50)151(62)130(84)139(57)186(74)220(51)208(51)EJSP 131 96(31)93(29)78(24)139(29)131(34)126(30)219(27)153(42)169(60)JESP 113 94(66)69(53)120(68)92(56)113(89)102(46)204(60)206(81)275(70)
{"title":"Corrigendum: Journal N-Pact Factors From 2011 to 2019: Evaluating the Quality of Social/Personality Journals With Respect to Sample Size and Statistical Power","authors":"","doi":"10.1177/25152459231175075","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459231175075","url":null,"abstract":"JRP 330 238 (17) 438 (14) 325 (26) 302 (15) 369 (14) 280 (11) 330 (18) 500 (20) 438 (22) EJP 261 111 (13) 194 (16) 392 (14) 217 (7) 200 (12) 261 (15) 422 (15) 576 (11) 496 (10) JP 251 239 (14) 198 (22) 354 (16) 359 (10) 406 (19) 239 (23) 251 (11) 240 (44) 286 (30) PS:S 200 72 (26) 82 (24) 174 (20) 104 (21) 200 (36) 231 (35) 248 (23) 386 (52) 202 (32) SPPS 186 128 (26) 134 (24) 210 (25) 172 (26) 186 (38) 178 (29) 300 (39) 364 (26) 383 (33) JPSP 179 98 (70) 108 (75) 102 (73) 116 (71) 220 (64) 179 (81) 225 (77) 225 (75) 320 (45) PSPB 139 105 (51) 78 (47) 117 (50) 151 (62) 130 (84) 139 (57) 186 (74) 220 (51) 208 (51) EJSP 131 96 (31) 93 (29) 78 (24) 139 (29) 131 (34) 126 (30) 219 (27) 153 (42) 169 (60) JESP 113 94 (66) 69 (53) 120 (68) 92 (56) 113 (89) 102 (46) 204 (60) 206 (81) 275 (70)","PeriodicalId":55645,"journal":{"name":"Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":13.6,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42797342","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Selecting the Number and Labels of Topics in Topic Modeling: A Tutorial 在主题建模中选择主题的编号和标签:教程
IF 13.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/25152459231160105
S. Weston, Ian Shryock, Ryan Light, Phillip A. Fisher
Topic modeling is a type of text analysis that identifies clusters of co-occurring words, or latent topics. A challenging step of topic modeling is determining the number of topics to extract. This tutorial describes tools researchers can use to identify the number and labels of topics in topic modeling. First, we outline the procedure for narrowing down a large range of models to a select number of candidate models. This procedure involves comparing the large set on fit metrics, including exclusivity, residuals, variational lower bound, and semantic coherence. Next, we describe the comparison of a small number of models using project goals as a guide and information about topic representative and solution congruence. Finally, we describe tools for labeling topics, including frequent and exclusive words, key examples, and correlations among topics.
主题建模是一种文本分析,用于识别共现单词或潜在主题的聚类。主题建模的一个具有挑战性的步骤是确定要提取的主题的数量。本教程介绍了研究人员可以用来识别主题建模中主题的数量和标签的工具。首先,我们概述了将大量模型缩小到选定数量的候选模型的过程。该过程涉及比较拟合度量的大集合,包括排他性、残差、变分下界和语义一致性。接下来,我们描述了以项目目标为指导的少数模型的比较,以及关于主题代表性和解决方案一致性的信息。最后,我们描述了标记主题的工具,包括常用词和专有词、关键示例以及主题之间的相关性。
{"title":"Selecting the Number and Labels of Topics in Topic Modeling: A Tutorial","authors":"S. Weston, Ian Shryock, Ryan Light, Phillip A. Fisher","doi":"10.1177/25152459231160105","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459231160105","url":null,"abstract":"Topic modeling is a type of text analysis that identifies clusters of co-occurring words, or latent topics. A challenging step of topic modeling is determining the number of topics to extract. This tutorial describes tools researchers can use to identify the number and labels of topics in topic modeling. First, we outline the procedure for narrowing down a large range of models to a select number of candidate models. This procedure involves comparing the large set on fit metrics, including exclusivity, residuals, variational lower bound, and semantic coherence. Next, we describe the comparison of a small number of models using project goals as a guide and information about topic representative and solution congruence. Finally, we describe tools for labeling topics, including frequent and exclusive words, key examples, and correlations among topics.","PeriodicalId":55645,"journal":{"name":"Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":13.6,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42107656","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Using Market-Research Panels for Behavioral Science: An Overview and Tutorial 将市场研究小组用于行为科学:概述和教程
IF 13.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/25152459221140388
Aaron J. Moss, David J. Hauser, Cheskie Rosenzweig, Shalom N Jaffe, Jonathan Robinson, L. Litman
Behavioral scientists looking to run online studies are confronted with a bevy of options. Where to recruit participants? Which tools to use for survey creation and study management? How to maintain data quality? In this tutorial, we highlight the unique capabilities of market-research panels and demonstrate how researchers can effectively sample from such panels. Unlike the microtask platforms most academics are familiar with (e.g., MTurk and Prolific), market-research panels have access to more than 100 million potential participants worldwide, provide more representative samples, and excel at demographic targeting. However, efficiently gathering data from online panels requires integration between the panel and a researcher’s survey in ways that are uncommon on microtask sites. For example, panels allow researchers to target participants according to preprofiled demographics (“Level 1” targeting, e.g., parents) and demographics that are not preprofiled but are screened for within the survey (“Level 2” targeting, e.g., parents of autistic children). In this article, we demonstrate how to sample hard-to-reach groups using market-research panels. We also describe several best practices for conducting research using online panels, including setting in-survey quotas to control sample composition and managing data quality. Our aim is to provide researchers with enough information to determine whether market-research panels are right for their research and to outline the necessary considerations for using such panels.
想要进行在线研究的行为科学家面临着许多选择。在哪里招募参与者?哪些工具可用于调查创建和研究管理?如何保持数据质量?在本教程中,我们强调了市场研究小组的独特功能,并展示了研究人员如何有效地从这些小组中进行采样。与大多数学者熟悉的微任务平台(如MTurk和Prolific)不同,市场研究小组可以接触到全球超过1亿的潜在参与者,提供更具代表性的样本,并擅长人口定位。然而,有效地从在线小组中收集数据需要小组和研究人员的调查之间的集成,这种集成在微任务网站上是不常见的。例如,小组允许研究人员根据预先编制的人口统计数据(“1级”目标,例如父母)和未预先编制但在调查中筛选的人口统计信息(“2级”目标(例如自闭症儿童的父母)来确定参与者。在这篇文章中,我们展示了如何使用市场研究小组对难以接触的群体进行抽样。我们还描述了使用在线小组进行研究的几种最佳实践,包括设置调查配额以控制样本组成和管理数据质量。我们的目标是为研究人员提供足够的信息,以确定市场研究小组是否适合他们的研究,并概述使用此类小组的必要考虑因素。
{"title":"Using Market-Research Panels for Behavioral Science: An Overview and Tutorial","authors":"Aaron J. Moss, David J. Hauser, Cheskie Rosenzweig, Shalom N Jaffe, Jonathan Robinson, L. Litman","doi":"10.1177/25152459221140388","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459221140388","url":null,"abstract":"Behavioral scientists looking to run online studies are confronted with a bevy of options. Where to recruit participants? Which tools to use for survey creation and study management? How to maintain data quality? In this tutorial, we highlight the unique capabilities of market-research panels and demonstrate how researchers can effectively sample from such panels. Unlike the microtask platforms most academics are familiar with (e.g., MTurk and Prolific), market-research panels have access to more than 100 million potential participants worldwide, provide more representative samples, and excel at demographic targeting. However, efficiently gathering data from online panels requires integration between the panel and a researcher’s survey in ways that are uncommon on microtask sites. For example, panels allow researchers to target participants according to preprofiled demographics (“Level 1” targeting, e.g., parents) and demographics that are not preprofiled but are screened for within the survey (“Level 2” targeting, e.g., parents of autistic children). In this article, we demonstrate how to sample hard-to-reach groups using market-research panels. We also describe several best practices for conducting research using online panels, including setting in-survey quotas to control sample composition and managing data quality. Our aim is to provide researchers with enough information to determine whether market-research panels are right for their research and to outline the necessary considerations for using such panels.","PeriodicalId":55645,"journal":{"name":"Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science","volume":"6 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":13.6,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41396512","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
PsyCalibrator: An Open-Source Package for Display Gamma Calibration and Luminance and Color Measurement PsyCalibrator:一个用于显示器伽马校准、亮度和颜色测量的开源软件包
IF 13.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/25152459221151151
Zhicheng Lin, Qimin Ma, Yang Zhang
Studies in vision, psychology, and neuroscience often present visual stimuli on digital screens. Crucially, the appearance of visual stimuli depends on properties such as luminance and color, making it critical to measure them. Yet conventional luminance-measuring equipment is not only expensive but also onerous to operate (particularly for novices). Building on previous work, here we present an open-source integrated software package—PsyCalibrator (https://github.com/yangzhangpsy/PsyCalibrator)—that takes advantage of consumer hardware (SpyderX, Spyder5) and makes luminance/color measurement and gamma calibration accessible and flexible. Gamma calibration based on visual methods (without photometers) is also implemented. PsyCalibrator requires MATLAB (or its free alternative, GNU Octave) and works in Windows, macOS, and Linux. We first validated measurements from SpyderX and Spyder5 by comparing them with professional, high-cost photometers (ColorCAL MKII Colorimeter and Photo Research PR-670 SpectraScan). Validation results show (a) excellent accuracy in linear correction and luminance/color measurement and (b) for practical purposes, low measurement variances. We offer a detailed tutorial on using PsyCalibrator to measure luminance/color and calibrate displays. Finally, we recommend reporting templates to describe simple (e.g., computer-generated shapes) and complex (e.g., naturalistic images and videos) visual stimuli.
视觉、心理学和神经科学的研究经常在数字屏幕上呈现视觉刺激。至关重要的是,视觉刺激的外观取决于亮度和颜色等特性,因此测量它们至关重要。然而,传统的亮度测量设备不仅昂贵,而且操作起来也很麻烦(尤其是对于新手来说)。在之前工作的基础上,我们提出了一个开源集成软件包——PsyCalibrator(https://github.com/yangzhangpsy/PsyCalibrator)--它利用了消费者硬件(SpyderX、Spyder5),并使亮度/颜色测量和伽马校准变得容易和灵活。还实施了基于视觉方法(无光度计)的伽马校准。PsyCalibrator需要MATLAB(或其免费替代品GNU Octave),可在Windows、macOS和Linux中工作。我们首先通过将SpyderX和Spyder5的测量值与专业的高成本光度计(ColorCAL MKII 色度计和照片研究PR-670 SpectraScan)。验证结果表明(a)线性校正和亮度/颜色测量具有良好的准确性,(b)出于实际目的,测量方差较低。我们提供了关于使用PsyCalibrator测量亮度/颜色和校准显示器的详细教程。最后,我们建议使用报告模板来描述简单(例如,计算机生成的形状)和复杂(例如,自然图像和视频)的视觉刺激。
{"title":"PsyCalibrator: An Open-Source Package for Display Gamma Calibration and Luminance and Color Measurement","authors":"Zhicheng Lin, Qimin Ma, Yang Zhang","doi":"10.1177/25152459221151151","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459221151151","url":null,"abstract":"Studies in vision, psychology, and neuroscience often present visual stimuli on digital screens. Crucially, the appearance of visual stimuli depends on properties such as luminance and color, making it critical to measure them. Yet conventional luminance-measuring equipment is not only expensive but also onerous to operate (particularly for novices). Building on previous work, here we present an open-source integrated software package—PsyCalibrator (https://github.com/yangzhangpsy/PsyCalibrator)—that takes advantage of consumer hardware (SpyderX, Spyder5) and makes luminance/color measurement and gamma calibration accessible and flexible. Gamma calibration based on visual methods (without photometers) is also implemented. PsyCalibrator requires MATLAB (or its free alternative, GNU Octave) and works in Windows, macOS, and Linux. We first validated measurements from SpyderX and Spyder5 by comparing them with professional, high-cost photometers (ColorCAL MKII Colorimeter and Photo Research PR-670 SpectraScan). Validation results show (a) excellent accuracy in linear correction and luminance/color measurement and (b) for practical purposes, low measurement variances. We offer a detailed tutorial on using PsyCalibrator to measure luminance/color and calibrate displays. Finally, we recommend reporting templates to describe simple (e.g., computer-generated shapes) and complex (e.g., naturalistic images and videos) visual stimuli.","PeriodicalId":55645,"journal":{"name":"Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":13.6,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43447182","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Bayesian Repeated-Measures Analysis of Variance: An Updated Methodology Implemented in JASP 方差的贝叶斯重复测度分析:一种在JASP中实现的更新方法
IF 13.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/25152459231168024
D. van den Bergh, E. Wagenmakers, F. Aust
Analysis of variance (ANOVA) is widely used to assess the influence of one or more experimental (or quasi-experimental) manipulations on a continuous outcome. Traditionally, ANOVA is carried out in a frequentist manner using p values, but a Bayesian alternative has been proposed. Assuming that the proposed Bayesian ANOVA is closely modeled after its frequentist counterpart, one may be surprised to find that the two can yield very different conclusions when the design involves multiple repeated-measures factors. We illustrate such a discrepancy with a real data set from a two-factorial within-subject experiment. For this data set, the results of a frequentist and Bayesian ANOVA are in a disagreement about which main effect accounts for the variance in the data. The reason for this disagreement is that frequentist and the proposed Bayesian ANOVA use different model specifications. As currently implemented, the proposed Bayesian ANOVA assumes that there are no individual differences in the magnitude of effects. We suspect that this assumption is neither obvious to nor desired by most analysts because it is untenable in most applications. We argue here that the Bayesian ANOVA should be revised to allow for individual differences. As a default, we suggest the standard frequentist model specification but discuss a recently proposed alternative and provide guidance on how to choose the appropriate model specification. We end by discussing the implications of the revised model specification for previously published results of Bayesian ANOVAs.
方差分析(ANOVA)被广泛用于评估一个或多个实验(或准实验)操作对连续结果的影响。传统上,方差分析是使用p值以频率分析的方式进行的,但已经提出了贝叶斯替代方案。假设所提出的贝叶斯方差分析是以其频繁度对应物为模型的,人们可能会惊讶地发现,当设计涉及多个重复测量因素时,两者可以得出非常不同的结论。我们用受试者内两因子实验的真实数据集来说明这种差异。对于该数据集,频率分析和贝叶斯方差分析的结果在数据方差的主要影响因素方面存在分歧。这种分歧的原因是,频繁度分析和所提出的贝叶斯方差分析使用了不同的模型规范。正如目前所实施的那样,所提出的贝叶斯方差分析假设影响的大小没有个体差异。我们怀疑,这种假设对大多数分析师来说既不明显,也不可取,因为它在大多数应用程序中都是站不住脚的。我们在这里认为,贝叶斯方差分析应该进行修正,以考虑到个体差异。默认情况下,我们建议使用标准的常客模型规范,但讨论了最近提出的替代方案,并就如何选择合适的模型规范提供了指导。最后,我们讨论了修订后的模型规范对先前发表的贝叶斯方差分析结果的影响。
{"title":"Bayesian Repeated-Measures Analysis of Variance: An Updated Methodology Implemented in JASP","authors":"D. van den Bergh, E. Wagenmakers, F. Aust","doi":"10.1177/25152459231168024","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459231168024","url":null,"abstract":"Analysis of variance (ANOVA) is widely used to assess the influence of one or more experimental (or quasi-experimental) manipulations on a continuous outcome. Traditionally, ANOVA is carried out in a frequentist manner using p values, but a Bayesian alternative has been proposed. Assuming that the proposed Bayesian ANOVA is closely modeled after its frequentist counterpart, one may be surprised to find that the two can yield very different conclusions when the design involves multiple repeated-measures factors. We illustrate such a discrepancy with a real data set from a two-factorial within-subject experiment. For this data set, the results of a frequentist and Bayesian ANOVA are in a disagreement about which main effect accounts for the variance in the data. The reason for this disagreement is that frequentist and the proposed Bayesian ANOVA use different model specifications. As currently implemented, the proposed Bayesian ANOVA assumes that there are no individual differences in the magnitude of effects. We suspect that this assumption is neither obvious to nor desired by most analysts because it is untenable in most applications. We argue here that the Bayesian ANOVA should be revised to allow for individual differences. As a default, we suggest the standard frequentist model specification but discuss a recently proposed alternative and provide guidance on how to choose the appropriate model specification. We end by discussing the implications of the revised model specification for previously published results of Bayesian ANOVAs.","PeriodicalId":55645,"journal":{"name":"Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":13.6,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41864814","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
When to Use Different Inferential Methods for Power Analysis and Data Analysis for Between-Subjects Mediation 在主体间中介的功效分析和数据分析中,何时使用不同的推理方法
IF 13.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/25152459231156606
J. Fossum, A. Montoya
Several options exist for conducting inference on indirect effects in mediation analysis. Although methods that use bootstrapping are the preferred inferential approach for testing mediation, they are time-consuming when the test must be performed many times for a power analysis. Alternatives that are more computationally efficient are not as robust, meaning accuracy of the inferences from these methods is more affected by nonnormal and heteroskedastic data. Previous research has shown that different sample sizes are needed to achieve the same amount of statistical power for different inferential approaches with data that meet all the statistical assumptions of linear regression. By contrast, we explore how similar power estimates are at the same sample size, including when assumptions are violated. We compare the power estimates from six inferential methods for between-subjects mediation using a Monte Carlo simulation study. We varied the path coefficients, inferential methods for the indirect effect, and degree to which assumptions are met. We found that when the assumptions of linear regression are met, three inferential methods consistently perform similarly: the joint significance test, the Monte Carlo confidence interval, and the percentile bootstrap confidence interval. When the assumptions were violated, the nonbootstrapping methods tended to have vastly different power estimates compared with the bootstrapping methods. On the basis of these results, we recommend using the more computationally efficient joint significance test for power analysis only when no assumption violations are hypothesized a priori. We also recommend the joint significance test to pick an optimal starting sample size value for power analysis using the percentile bootstrap confidence interval when assumption violations are suspected.
在中介分析中存在几种对间接效应进行推断的方法。尽管使用自引导的方法是测试中介的首选推理方法,但是当为了功率分析必须多次执行测试时,它们会很耗时。计算效率更高的替代方法不那么健壮,这意味着从这些方法推断的准确性更容易受到非正常和异方差数据的影响。以往的研究表明,对于满足线性回归所有统计假设的数据,不同的推理方法需要不同的样本量来获得相同的统计力。相比之下,我们探讨了在相同的样本量下,包括假设被违反时,相似的功率估计是如何产生的。我们使用蒙特卡洛模拟研究比较了受试者间中介的六种推断方法的功率估计。我们改变了路径系数,间接影响的推理方法,以及满足假设的程度。我们发现,当满足线性回归的假设时,三种推理方法的表现一致相似:联合显著性检验、蒙特卡洛置信区间和百分位bootstrap置信区间。当这些假设被违背时,非自举方法与自举方法的功率估计往往相差很大。在这些结果的基础上,我们建议只有在没有先验假设违反的情况下,才使用计算效率更高的联合显著性检验进行功率分析。我们还建议联合显著性检验,以选择一个最优的起始样本量值功率分析时,使用百分位数bootstrap置信区间假设违规怀疑。
{"title":"When to Use Different Inferential Methods for Power Analysis and Data Analysis for Between-Subjects Mediation","authors":"J. Fossum, A. Montoya","doi":"10.1177/25152459231156606","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459231156606","url":null,"abstract":"Several options exist for conducting inference on indirect effects in mediation analysis. Although methods that use bootstrapping are the preferred inferential approach for testing mediation, they are time-consuming when the test must be performed many times for a power analysis. Alternatives that are more computationally efficient are not as robust, meaning accuracy of the inferences from these methods is more affected by nonnormal and heteroskedastic data. Previous research has shown that different sample sizes are needed to achieve the same amount of statistical power for different inferential approaches with data that meet all the statistical assumptions of linear regression. By contrast, we explore how similar power estimates are at the same sample size, including when assumptions are violated. We compare the power estimates from six inferential methods for between-subjects mediation using a Monte Carlo simulation study. We varied the path coefficients, inferential methods for the indirect effect, and degree to which assumptions are met. We found that when the assumptions of linear regression are met, three inferential methods consistently perform similarly: the joint significance test, the Monte Carlo confidence interval, and the percentile bootstrap confidence interval. When the assumptions were violated, the nonbootstrapping methods tended to have vastly different power estimates compared with the bootstrapping methods. On the basis of these results, we recommend using the more computationally efficient joint significance test for power analysis only when no assumption violations are hypothesized a priori. We also recommend the joint significance test to pick an optimal starting sample size value for power analysis using the percentile bootstrap confidence interval when assumption violations are suspected.","PeriodicalId":55645,"journal":{"name":"Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":13.6,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48077177","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Chinese Open Science Network (COSN): Building an Open Science Community From Scratch 中国开放科学网:从零开始构建开放科学社区
IF 13.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/25152459221144986
Haiyang Jin, Qing Wang, Yufei Yang, Han Zhang, M. Gao, Shuxian Jin, Yanxiu (Sharon) Chen, Ting Xu, Yuan-Rui Zheng, Ji Chen, Q. Xiao, Jinbiao Yang, Xindi Wang, Haiyang Geng, Jianqiao Ge, Wei-Wei Wang, X. Chen, Lei Zhang, Xianli Zuo, H. Chuan-Peng
Open Science is becoming a mainstream scientific ideology in psychology and related fields. However, researchers, especially early-career researchers (ECRs) in developing countries, are facing significant hurdles in engaging in Open Science and moving it forward. In China, various societal and cultural factors discourage ECRs from participating in Open Science, such as the lack of dedicated communication channels and the norm of modesty. To make the voice of Open Science heard by Chinese-speaking ECRs and scholars at large, the Chinese Open Science Network (COSN) was initiated in 2016. With its core values being grassroots-oriented, diversity, and inclusivity, COSN has grown from a small Open Science interest group to a recognized network both in the Chinese-speaking research community and the international Open Science community. So far, COSN has organized three in-person workshops, 12 tutorials, 48 talks, and 55 journal club sessions and translated 15 Open Science-related articles and blogs from English to Chinese. Currently, the main social media account of COSN (i.e., the WeChat Official Account) has more than 23,000 subscribers, and more than 1,000 researchers/students actively participate in the discussions on Open Science. In this article, we share our experience in building such a network to encourage ECRs in developing countries to start their own Open Science initiatives and engage in the global Open Science movement. We foresee great collaborative efforts of COSN together with all other local and international networks to further accelerate the Open Science movement.
开放科学正在成为心理学及相关领域的主流科学思想。然而,研究人员,特别是发展中国家的早期职业研究人员,在参与开放科学并推动其发展方面面临着重大障碍。在中国,各种社会和文化因素阻碍了ECR参与开放科学,例如缺乏专门的沟通渠道和谦虚的规范。为了让讲中文的ECR和广大学者听到开放科学的声音,中国开放科学网(COSN)于2016年成立。COSN的核心价值观是面向基层、多样性和包容性,它已经从一个小型的开放科学兴趣小组发展成为华语研究界和国际开放科学界公认的网络。到目前为止,COSN已经组织了三次面对面研讨会、12次教程、48次讲座和55次期刊俱乐部会议,并将15篇与开放科学相关的文章和博客从英文翻译成中文。目前,COSN的主要社交媒体账号(即微信公众号)拥有23000多名订阅者,1000多名研究人员/学生积极参与开放科学的讨论。在这篇文章中,我们分享了我们建立这样一个网络的经验,以鼓励发展中国家的ECR启动自己的开放科学倡议,并参与全球开放科学运动。我们预见到COSN将与所有其他本地和国际网络共同努力,进一步加速开放科学运动。
{"title":"The Chinese Open Science Network (COSN): Building an Open Science Community From Scratch","authors":"Haiyang Jin, Qing Wang, Yufei Yang, Han Zhang, M. Gao, Shuxian Jin, Yanxiu (Sharon) Chen, Ting Xu, Yuan-Rui Zheng, Ji Chen, Q. Xiao, Jinbiao Yang, Xindi Wang, Haiyang Geng, Jianqiao Ge, Wei-Wei Wang, X. Chen, Lei Zhang, Xianli Zuo, H. Chuan-Peng","doi":"10.1177/25152459221144986","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459221144986","url":null,"abstract":"Open Science is becoming a mainstream scientific ideology in psychology and related fields. However, researchers, especially early-career researchers (ECRs) in developing countries, are facing significant hurdles in engaging in Open Science and moving it forward. In China, various societal and cultural factors discourage ECRs from participating in Open Science, such as the lack of dedicated communication channels and the norm of modesty. To make the voice of Open Science heard by Chinese-speaking ECRs and scholars at large, the Chinese Open Science Network (COSN) was initiated in 2016. With its core values being grassroots-oriented, diversity, and inclusivity, COSN has grown from a small Open Science interest group to a recognized network both in the Chinese-speaking research community and the international Open Science community. So far, COSN has organized three in-person workshops, 12 tutorials, 48 talks, and 55 journal club sessions and translated 15 Open Science-related articles and blogs from English to Chinese. Currently, the main social media account of COSN (i.e., the WeChat Official Account) has more than 23,000 subscribers, and more than 1,000 researchers/students actively participate in the discussions on Open Science. In this article, we share our experience in building such a network to encourage ECRs in developing countries to start their own Open Science initiatives and engage in the global Open Science movement. We foresee great collaborative efforts of COSN together with all other local and international networks to further accelerate the Open Science movement.","PeriodicalId":55645,"journal":{"name":"Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":13.6,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45736253","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1