首页 > 最新文献

Open psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Looking at HMI Concepts for Highly Automated Vehicles: Permanent vs. Context-Adaptive Information Presentation 高度自动化车辆的HMI概念:永久与上下文自适应信息呈现
Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2022-0124
Cornelia Hollander, Franziska Hartwich, J. Krems
Abstract To facilitate the usage and expected benefits of higher-level automated vehicles, passengers’ distrust and safety concerns should be reduced through increasing system transparency (ST) by providing driving-related information. We therefore examined the effects of ST on passengers’ gaze behavior during driving, trust in automated driving and evaluation of different human-machine interface (HMI) concepts. In a driving simulator, 50 participants experienced three identical highly automated drives under three HMI conditions: no HMI (only conventional speedometer), context-adaptive HMI (all system information only available in more complex situations) or permanent HMI (all system information permanently available). Compared to driving without HMI, the introduction of the two HMIs resulted in significantly higher usage of the center stack display (i.e. gazes towards the HMIs), which was accompanied by significantly higher trust ratings. The considerable differences in information availability provided by the context-adaptive versus permanent HMI did not reflect in similarly considerable differences regarding the passengers’ gaze behavior or accompanied trust ratings. Additionally, user experience evaluations expressed preferences for the context-adaptive HMI. Hence, the permanent HMI did not seem to create benefits over the context-adaptive HMI, supporting the usage of more economical, context-adaptive HMIs in higher-level automated vehicles.
摘要为了促进更高级别自动化车辆的使用和预期效益,应通过提供驾驶相关信息来提高系统透明度(ST),从而减少乘客的不信任和安全问题。因此,我们研究了ST对乘客在驾驶过程中的凝视行为、对自动驾驶的信任以及对不同人机界面(HMI)概念的评估的影响。在驾驶模拟器中,50名参与者在三种HMI条件下体验了三种完全相同的高度自动化驾驶:无HMI(仅传统速度计)、上下文自适应HMI(所有系统信息仅在更复杂的情况下可用)或永久HMI(所有系统信息永久可用)。与没有HMI的驾驶相比,两种HMI的引入显著提高了中央堆栈显示器的使用率(即凝视HMI),同时也带来了显著更高的信任评级。上下文自适应HMI与永久HMI提供的信息可用性的显著差异并没有反映出乘客的凝视行为或伴随的信任评级的类似显著差异。此外,用户体验评估表达了对上下文自适应HMI的偏好。因此,与上下文自适应HMI相比,永久HMI似乎没有带来好处,支持在更高级别的自动化车辆中使用更经济、上下文自适应HMI。
{"title":"Looking at HMI Concepts for Highly Automated Vehicles: Permanent vs. Context-Adaptive Information Presentation","authors":"Cornelia Hollander, Franziska Hartwich, J. Krems","doi":"10.1515/psych-2022-0124","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2022-0124","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract To facilitate the usage and expected benefits of higher-level automated vehicles, passengers’ distrust and safety concerns should be reduced through increasing system transparency (ST) by providing driving-related information. We therefore examined the effects of ST on passengers’ gaze behavior during driving, trust in automated driving and evaluation of different human-machine interface (HMI) concepts. In a driving simulator, 50 participants experienced three identical highly automated drives under three HMI conditions: no HMI (only conventional speedometer), context-adaptive HMI (all system information only available in more complex situations) or permanent HMI (all system information permanently available). Compared to driving without HMI, the introduction of the two HMIs resulted in significantly higher usage of the center stack display (i.e. gazes towards the HMIs), which was accompanied by significantly higher trust ratings. The considerable differences in information availability provided by the context-adaptive versus permanent HMI did not reflect in similarly considerable differences regarding the passengers’ gaze behavior or accompanied trust ratings. Additionally, user experience evaluations expressed preferences for the context-adaptive HMI. Hence, the permanent HMI did not seem to create benefits over the context-adaptive HMI, supporting the usage of more economical, context-adaptive HMIs in higher-level automated vehicles.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48625667","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Acute aerobic exercise and attentional focus influence the self-positivity bias in emotional evaluation. Evidence from an experimental study 急性有氧运动和注意力集中影响情绪评价中的自我积极性偏差。实验研究的证据
Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2022-0010
Friedrich Meixner, C. Herbert
Abstract The influence of aerobic exercise, as a possibly mood-enhancing experience, was investigated for its effect on emotional evaluation of self- vs. other-related emotional (e.g., my/his joy) or neutral (e.g., my/his notes) stimuli. N=30 participants (healthy, female novices, i.e., with no regular, dedicated training schedule) performed four separate sessions of moderate aerobic exercise (22 min of cycling at 60-70% HRmax). Attentional focus was manipulated across exercise bouts (internal focus vs. external focus vs. no instructed attention focus vs. control condition consisting of no exercise and no instructed attention focus). Emotional evaluation of verbal stimuli was assessed 10 minutes after each exercise bout. Exercise increased self-reported positive affect compared to the inactive control condition regardless of attentional focus. Exercising without instructed attentional focus led to faster reaction times in the emotional evaluation task compared to the inactive control condition. Instructed internal or external attentional focus did not affect evaluations (number of valence-congruent responses) or reaction times compared to the no-exercise control condition. Self-reported internal attentional focus during cycling was associated with a subsequently stronger, and in the case of an external focus with a subsequently rather reduced self-positivity bias in the evaluation of self-related, positive vs. other-related positive words.
摘要有氧运动作为一种可能增强情绪的体验,对其对自我和其他相关情绪(如我/他的快乐)或中性(如我的笔记)刺激的情绪评估的影响进行了研究。N=30名参与者(健康的女性新手,即没有定期、专门的训练计划)进行了四次单独的中等有氧运动(22分钟的骑行,最高心率为60-70%)。注意力集中在运动中被操纵(内部集中与外部集中与无指示注意力集中与由无运动和无指示注意力聚焦组成的控制条件)。在每次运动后10分钟评估言语刺激的情绪评估。与不活跃的对照条件相比,无论注意力集中如何,运动都会增加自我报告的积极影响。与不活跃的控制条件相比,在没有指导注意力集中的情况下锻炼会导致情绪评估任务的反应时间更快。与无运动控制条件相比,指导的内部或外部注意力集中不会影响评估(效价一致反应的数量)或反应时间。自行车运动中自我报告的内部注意力集中与随后更强的自我相关,在外部注意力集中的情况下,在评估自我相关、积极词与其他相关积极词时,自我积极偏见随后相当减少。
{"title":"Acute aerobic exercise and attentional focus influence the self-positivity bias in emotional evaluation. Evidence from an experimental study","authors":"Friedrich Meixner, C. Herbert","doi":"10.1515/psych-2022-0010","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2022-0010","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The influence of aerobic exercise, as a possibly mood-enhancing experience, was investigated for its effect on emotional evaluation of self- vs. other-related emotional (e.g., my/his joy) or neutral (e.g., my/his notes) stimuli. N=30 participants (healthy, female novices, i.e., with no regular, dedicated training schedule) performed four separate sessions of moderate aerobic exercise (22 min of cycling at 60-70% HRmax). Attentional focus was manipulated across exercise bouts (internal focus vs. external focus vs. no instructed attention focus vs. control condition consisting of no exercise and no instructed attention focus). Emotional evaluation of verbal stimuli was assessed 10 minutes after each exercise bout. Exercise increased self-reported positive affect compared to the inactive control condition regardless of attentional focus. Exercising without instructed attentional focus led to faster reaction times in the emotional evaluation task compared to the inactive control condition. Instructed internal or external attentional focus did not affect evaluations (number of valence-congruent responses) or reaction times compared to the no-exercise control condition. Self-reported internal attentional focus during cycling was associated with a subsequently stronger, and in the case of an external focus with a subsequently rather reduced self-positivity bias in the evaluation of self-related, positive vs. other-related positive words.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45375158","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Equal Quality for Online and Lab Data: A Direct Comparison from Two Dual-Task Paradigms 在线和实验室数据的同等质量:两种双重任务范式的直接比较
Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2022-0003
Marian Sauter, Maximilian Stefani, Wolfgang Mack
Abstract Conducting behavioral experiments online has become more prevalent recently. Still, there is reluctance to embrace the possibilities this technology has to offer. So far, only simple tasks have been replicated in an online setting. In order to investigate whether collecting online also leads to high quality data in demanding tasks, we directly compared data collected in the lab with data collected online from a demanding dual-task paradigm and a psychological refractory period paradigm. In Experiment 1, we recruited from local pools, online and offline; in Experiment 2, we collected lab data from our local pool and online data from a remote commercial participant platform. We found that all relevant effects were replicated in the lab and online settings; effect sizes were similar. Additionally, most response time distributions were even statistically equivalent when comparing online and lab data. Thus, online effect sizes and variances can be comparable to lab-based data. Online studies are time-efficient and recruiting an online sample instead or on top of a laboratory sample should be considered for basic behavioral research. This can serve an important role in the generalizability and replicability of findings in the cognitive and behavioral sciences.
摘要最近,在网上进行行为实验变得越来越普遍。尽管如此,人们还是不愿接受这项技术所提供的可能性。到目前为止,只有简单的任务在在线环境中被复制。为了研究在线收集是否也能在要求苛刻的任务中获得高质量的数据,我们直接将实验室收集的数据与从要求苛刻的双任务范式和心理不应期范式在线收集的数据进行了比较。在实验1中,我们从当地的人才库中招募,包括在线和离线;在实验2中,我们从本地池中收集了实验室数据,并从远程商业参与者平台中收集了在线数据。我们发现,所有相关效果都在实验室和在线环境中复制;效果大小相似。此外,在比较在线和实验室数据时,大多数响应时间分布甚至在统计上是相等的。因此,在线效应大小和方差可以与基于实验室的数据相比较。在线研究具有时效性,基础行为研究应考虑招募在线样本或在实验室样本的基础上招募在线样本。这可以在认知和行为科学研究结果的可推广性和可复制性方面发挥重要作用。
{"title":"Equal Quality for Online and Lab Data: A Direct Comparison from Two Dual-Task Paradigms","authors":"Marian Sauter, Maximilian Stefani, Wolfgang Mack","doi":"10.1515/psych-2022-0003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2022-0003","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Conducting behavioral experiments online has become more prevalent recently. Still, there is reluctance to embrace the possibilities this technology has to offer. So far, only simple tasks have been replicated in an online setting. In order to investigate whether collecting online also leads to high quality data in demanding tasks, we directly compared data collected in the lab with data collected online from a demanding dual-task paradigm and a psychological refractory period paradigm. In Experiment 1, we recruited from local pools, online and offline; in Experiment 2, we collected lab data from our local pool and online data from a remote commercial participant platform. We found that all relevant effects were replicated in the lab and online settings; effect sizes were similar. Additionally, most response time distributions were even statistically equivalent when comparing online and lab data. Thus, online effect sizes and variances can be comparable to lab-based data. Online studies are time-efficient and recruiting an online sample instead or on top of a laboratory sample should be considered for basic behavioral research. This can serve an important role in the generalizability and replicability of findings in the cognitive and behavioral sciences.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48956135","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
“In-emotional blindness”? Lower detection rates for unexpected stimuli in negative compared to positive emotions “情感失明”?与积极情绪相比,消极情绪中意外刺激的检测率更低
Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2022-0130
C. Bermeitinger, R. Hackländer, Pamela Baess, Cathleen Kappes, Mareike Meinhard
Abstract Determining how emotional experience influences attention is a long standing goal of cognitive psychologists. Emotion is often broken down into two main dimensions, arousal and valence. While many theories focus more on the influence of one dimension than the other, the systematic investigation of the independent influences of the two dimensions of emotion on attention has been slow. In order to examine the relevance of both aspects of emotion, and their interplay on attention simultaneously, in the current experiment we induced low (satisfaction) and high (happiness) arousal positive emotions and low (sadness) and high (anger) arousal negative emotions in subjects before having them complete an inattentional blindness (IB) test. In line with theories that focus on the role of valence, we found that negative emotions led to more IB than positive emotions, and that arousal did not influence attention. Implications of the results for the theoretical contributions of the dimensions of emotion to visual attention are discussed.
摘要确定情绪体验如何影响注意力是认知心理学家长期以来的目标。情绪通常分为两个主要维度,唤醒和效价。虽然许多理论更多地关注一个维度的影响而不是另一个维度,但对情绪的两个维度对注意力的独立影响的系统研究进展缓慢。为了同时检验情绪的两个方面的相关性,以及它们对注意力的相互作用,在当前的实验中,我们在受试者完成注意力不集中盲(IB)测试之前,诱导他们产生低(满意度)和高(幸福感)唤醒的积极情绪,以及低(悲伤)和高)唤醒的消极情绪。与关注效价作用的理论一致,我们发现消极情绪比积极情绪导致更多的IB,唤醒不会影响注意力。讨论了这些结果对情感维度对视觉注意力的理论贡献的启示。
{"title":"“In-emotional blindness”? Lower detection rates for unexpected stimuli in negative compared to positive emotions","authors":"C. Bermeitinger, R. Hackländer, Pamela Baess, Cathleen Kappes, Mareike Meinhard","doi":"10.1515/psych-2022-0130","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2022-0130","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Determining how emotional experience influences attention is a long standing goal of cognitive psychologists. Emotion is often broken down into two main dimensions, arousal and valence. While many theories focus more on the influence of one dimension than the other, the systematic investigation of the independent influences of the two dimensions of emotion on attention has been slow. In order to examine the relevance of both aspects of emotion, and their interplay on attention simultaneously, in the current experiment we induced low (satisfaction) and high (happiness) arousal positive emotions and low (sadness) and high (anger) arousal negative emotions in subjects before having them complete an inattentional blindness (IB) test. In line with theories that focus on the role of valence, we found that negative emotions led to more IB than positive emotions, and that arousal did not influence attention. Implications of the results for the theoretical contributions of the dimensions of emotion to visual attention are discussed.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49465673","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When does “inhibition of return” occur in spatial cueing tasks? Temporally disentangling multiple cue-triggered effects using response history and conditional accuracy analyses 空间提示任务中什么时候会出现“抑制返回”?使用反应历史和条件准确性分析暂时解开多个线索触发效应
Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2022-0005
Sven Panis, T. Schmidt
Abstract Research on spatial cueing has shown that uninformative cues often facilitate mean response time (RT) performance in valid- compared to invalid-cueing conditions at short cue-target stimulus-onset-asynchronies (SOAs), and robustly generate a reversed or inhibitory cueing effect at longer SOAs that is widely known as inhibition-of-return (IOR). To study the within-trial time course of the IOR and facilitation effects we employ discrete-time hazard and conditional accuracy analyses to analyze the shapes of the RT and accuracy distributions measured in two experimental tasks. Our distributional analyses show that (a) IOR is present only from ~160 ms to ~280 ms after target onset for cue-target SOAs above ~200 ms, (b) facilitation does not precede IOR, but co-occurs with it, (c) the cue-triggered motor response activation is selectively and actively inhibited before target onset, (d) the IOR effect consists of a facilitatory and an inhibitory component when compared to central cueing, (e) the addition of an extra central cue causes a temporary negative cueing effect in the conditional accuracy functions, and (f) the within-trial time course of IOR is not affected much by the task employed (detection or localization). We conclude that the traditional mean performance measures conceal crucial information on behavioral dynamics in spatial cueing paradigms.
空间线索的研究表明,在短线索-目标刺激-启动-异步(soa)条件下,与无效线索相比,非信息性线索往往能促进有效线索条件下的平均反应时间(RT)表现,而在长线索-目标刺激-启动-异步(soa)条件下,非信息性线索会产生强烈的逆转或抑制线索效应,即众所周知的返回抑制效应(IOR)。为了研究IOR的试验时间过程和促进效应,我们采用离散时间风险和条件准确度分析来分析两个实验任务中测量的RT和准确度分布的形状。我们的分布分析表明:(a)对于超过~200 ms的线索-目标SOAs, IOR仅在目标开始后~160 ms至~280 ms存在;(b)促进性并不先于IOR,而是与IOR共同发生;(c)线索触发的运动反应激活在目标开始前被选择性地主动抑制;(d)与中央线索相比,IOR效应包括促进性和抑制性成分。(e)额外中心线索的添加在条件准确度函数中引起暂时的负线索效应,(f) IOR的试验时间过程不受所采用的任务(检测或定位)的影响。我们的结论是,传统的平均表现度量隐藏了空间线索范式中行为动力学的关键信息。
{"title":"When does “inhibition of return” occur in spatial cueing tasks? Temporally disentangling multiple cue-triggered effects using response history and conditional accuracy analyses","authors":"Sven Panis, T. Schmidt","doi":"10.1515/psych-2022-0005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2022-0005","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Research on spatial cueing has shown that uninformative cues often facilitate mean response time (RT) performance in valid- compared to invalid-cueing conditions at short cue-target stimulus-onset-asynchronies (SOAs), and robustly generate a reversed or inhibitory cueing effect at longer SOAs that is widely known as inhibition-of-return (IOR). To study the within-trial time course of the IOR and facilitation effects we employ discrete-time hazard and conditional accuracy analyses to analyze the shapes of the RT and accuracy distributions measured in two experimental tasks. Our distributional analyses show that (a) IOR is present only from ~160 ms to ~280 ms after target onset for cue-target SOAs above ~200 ms, (b) facilitation does not precede IOR, but co-occurs with it, (c) the cue-triggered motor response activation is selectively and actively inhibited before target onset, (d) the IOR effect consists of a facilitatory and an inhibitory component when compared to central cueing, (e) the addition of an extra central cue causes a temporary negative cueing effect in the conditional accuracy functions, and (f) the within-trial time course of IOR is not affected much by the task employed (detection or localization). We conclude that the traditional mean performance measures conceal crucial information on behavioral dynamics in spatial cueing paradigms.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45253358","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Feeling Capable in an Ubuntu Way: Kenyan Comprehensions of Control Beliefs Compared with the German Perspective 以乌班图的方式感受能力:肯尼亚人对控制信念的理解与德国人的观点比较
Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2022-0004
Michaela Heinecke-Müller, C. Quaiser-Pohl, P. W. Kariuki, J. Arasa
Abstract In Western personnel psychology, control beliefs are a valued predictor for work-related outcomes. Yet, little is known about the culture-specific functioning of control in East Africa. Kenya, as an Ubuntu culture, is examined regarding control beliefs and contrasted with a German sample considered to represent an individualistic or Western culture. Responses to N=143 quantitative personality tests were attended with qualitative interviews on control beliefs (self-concept of ability, internality, powerful others, and chance). Content validity and factor structure of control beliefs were analyzed, followed by a Procrustean target rotation. Linear regression analyses were conducted to assess the predictability of job performance, achievement motivation, and well-being. Item comprehension, as well as factor structure of the four control aspects, differ between the two samples. In particular, the ‘powerful others’ control aspect diverges the most between the cultures. Linear regression analyses showed comparable, but not fully congruent predictability. Results indicate that an uncritical transfer of the control beliefs measure from one culture to another is inappropriate. Results fit in the picture of African Ubuntu philosophy, emphasizing social-relational aspects shaping control beliefs. More emic-etic based research is demanded concerning intra- and intercultural variability of control beliefs to depict a transcultural applicable and invariant model.
在西方人事心理学中,控制信念是工作相关结果的重要预测因子。然而,人们对东非特定文化的控制功能知之甚少。肯尼亚作为一种乌班图文化,在控制信念方面进行了研究,并与被认为代表个人主义或西方文化的德国样本进行了对比。对N=143个定量人格测试的回答进行了控制信念(能力自我概念、内在性、强大的他人和机会)的定性访谈。对控制信念的内容效度和因素结构进行分析,并进行普罗斯特目标旋转。采用线性回归分析评估工作绩效、成就动机和幸福感的可预测性。两个样本的项目理解以及四个控制方面的因素结构存在差异。特别是,“强大的他人”控制方面在文化之间差异最大。线性回归分析显示了可预测性,但不是完全一致的。结果表明,不加批判地将控制信念测量从一种文化转移到另一种文化是不合适的。结果符合非洲乌班图哲学,强调社会关系方面塑造控制信念。需要更多基于符号学的研究来研究控制信念的内部和跨文化变异性,以描绘一个跨文化适用和不变的模型。
{"title":"Feeling Capable in an Ubuntu Way: Kenyan Comprehensions of Control Beliefs Compared with the German Perspective","authors":"Michaela Heinecke-Müller, C. Quaiser-Pohl, P. W. Kariuki, J. Arasa","doi":"10.1515/psych-2022-0004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2022-0004","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract In Western personnel psychology, control beliefs are a valued predictor for work-related outcomes. Yet, little is known about the culture-specific functioning of control in East Africa. Kenya, as an Ubuntu culture, is examined regarding control beliefs and contrasted with a German sample considered to represent an individualistic or Western culture. Responses to N=143 quantitative personality tests were attended with qualitative interviews on control beliefs (self-concept of ability, internality, powerful others, and chance). Content validity and factor structure of control beliefs were analyzed, followed by a Procrustean target rotation. Linear regression analyses were conducted to assess the predictability of job performance, achievement motivation, and well-being. Item comprehension, as well as factor structure of the four control aspects, differ between the two samples. In particular, the ‘powerful others’ control aspect diverges the most between the cultures. Linear regression analyses showed comparable, but not fully congruent predictability. Results indicate that an uncritical transfer of the control beliefs measure from one culture to another is inappropriate. Results fit in the picture of African Ubuntu philosophy, emphasizing social-relational aspects shaping control beliefs. More emic-etic based research is demanded concerning intra- and intercultural variability of control beliefs to depict a transcultural applicable and invariant model.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48514028","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Response inhibition in the Negative Compatibility Effect in the absence of inhibitory stimulus features 负相容效应中反应抑制在没有抑制刺激时的特征
Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2022-0012
T. Schmidt, Sven Panis, Maximilian P. Wolkersdorfer, D. Vorberg
Abstract The Negative Compatibility Effect (NCE) is a reversal in priming effects that can occur when a masked arrow prime is followed by an arrow target at a long stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA). To test the explanation that the NCE is actually a positive priming effect elicited by mask features associated with the prime-opposed response, we devise masks that always point in the same direction as the prime, eliminating all antiprime features. We find large positive priming effects for arrow primes without masks and for arrow masks without primes. When a neutral mask is introduced, priming effects turn negative at long SOAs. In the critical case where the mask is an arrow in the same direction as the prime, the prime does not add to the positive priming effect from the mask shape, but instead strongly diminishes it and induces response errors even though all stimuli point in the same direction. No such feature-free inhibition is seen when arrows are replaced by color stimuli. We conclude that even though response activation by stimulus features plays a role in the NCE, there is a strong inhibitory component (though perhaps not in all feature domains) that is not based on visual features.
摘要负相容性效应(NCE)是一种启动效应的逆转,当在长刺激起始异步(SOA)下,一个掩蔽的箭头素数后面跟着一个箭头目标时,可能会发生这种效应。为了验证NCE实际上是由与素数相对反应相关的掩码特征引发的正启动效应的解释,我们设计了始终指向与素数相同方向的掩码,消除了所有反素数特征。我们发现,对于没有掩码的箭头素数和没有素数的箭头掩码,有很大的正启动效应。当引入中性掩模时,在长SOA时引发效应变为负。在掩模是与素数方向相同的箭头的关键情况下,素数不会增加掩模形状的正启动效应,而是强烈削弱它,并导致响应误差,即使所有刺激都指向同一方向。当箭头被颜色刺激取代时,没有看到这种无特征的抑制。我们得出的结论是,尽管刺激特征的反应激活在NCE中起着一定作用,但仍有一种强烈的抑制成分(尽管可能不是在所有特征域中)不是基于视觉特征的。
{"title":"Response inhibition in the Negative Compatibility Effect in the absence of inhibitory stimulus features","authors":"T. Schmidt, Sven Panis, Maximilian P. Wolkersdorfer, D. Vorberg","doi":"10.1515/psych-2022-0012","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2022-0012","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The Negative Compatibility Effect (NCE) is a reversal in priming effects that can occur when a masked arrow prime is followed by an arrow target at a long stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA). To test the explanation that the NCE is actually a positive priming effect elicited by mask features associated with the prime-opposed response, we devise masks that always point in the same direction as the prime, eliminating all antiprime features. We find large positive priming effects for arrow primes without masks and for arrow masks without primes. When a neutral mask is introduced, priming effects turn negative at long SOAs. In the critical case where the mask is an arrow in the same direction as the prime, the prime does not add to the positive priming effect from the mask shape, but instead strongly diminishes it and induces response errors even though all stimuli point in the same direction. No such feature-free inhibition is seen when arrows are replaced by color stimuli. We conclude that even though response activation by stimulus features plays a role in the NCE, there is a strong inhibitory component (though perhaps not in all feature domains) that is not based on visual features.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45701389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Teachers’ ratings of social exclusion among students: The role of situational information and the ethnic origin of the excluded student based on the example of Syrian refugees 教师对学生社会排斥的评价:情境信息的作用和被排斥学生的种族出身——以叙利亚难民为例
Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2022-0007
Lino Szekely, Meike Bonefeld, Hanna Beißert
Abstract In the current study, we examined teachers’ ratings of social exclusion among students. 120 teachers (Mage=24.00, SD=3.71, 88% female) evaluated a hypothetical exclusion scenario in which the excluded student’s origin (German vs. Syrian refugee) was varied as well as whether participants received additional situational information about prior norm-violating behavior of the excluded student or not. Additionally, participants rated how likely they would intervene in the situation. For the evaluation of exclusion and the likelihood of intervention, there was a main effect of additional situational information, revealing that participants evaluated exclusion as less acceptable and were more likely to intervene if there was no additional situational information. Regarding the evaluation of exclusion, there was an interaction of additional situational information and the origin of the excluded student as the effect of additional situational information was bigger if the excluded student was German. For the likelihood of intervention, this interaction was not significant; but descriptively a similar pattern emerged. Results indicate that information about prior norm-violating behavior is more relevant for teachers’ reactions to social exclusion than the origin of the excluded student. However, in situations with an understandable reason for exclusion, teachers do include the origin of the excluded student in their considerations.
摘要在本研究中,我们调查了教师对学生社会排斥的评价。120名教师(Mage=24.00,SD=3.71,88%为女性)评估了一个假设的排斥场景,在该场景中,被排斥学生的出身(德国与叙利亚难民)各不相同,以及参与者是否收到了关于被排斥学生先前违反规范行为的额外情境信息。此外,参与者还评估了他们干预这种情况的可能性。对于排斥和干预可能性的评估,存在额外情境信息的主要影响,表明参与者认为排斥不太可接受,如果没有额外情境信息,他们更有可能进行干预。关于排斥的评估,如果被排斥的学生是德国人,则额外的情境信息的影响更大,因此,额外情境信息与被排斥学生的来源之间存在互动。就干预的可能性而言,这种相互作用并不显著;但可以描述的是,出现了类似的模式。结果表明,与被排斥学生的来源相比,先前违反规范行为的信息与教师对社会排斥的反应更相关。然而,在有可以理解的排斥原因的情况下,教师在考虑被排斥学生的来源时确实会考虑到这一点。
{"title":"Teachers’ ratings of social exclusion among students: The role of situational information and the ethnic origin of the excluded student based on the example of Syrian refugees","authors":"Lino Szekely, Meike Bonefeld, Hanna Beißert","doi":"10.1515/psych-2022-0007","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2022-0007","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract In the current study, we examined teachers’ ratings of social exclusion among students. 120 teachers (Mage=24.00, SD=3.71, 88% female) evaluated a hypothetical exclusion scenario in which the excluded student’s origin (German vs. Syrian refugee) was varied as well as whether participants received additional situational information about prior norm-violating behavior of the excluded student or not. Additionally, participants rated how likely they would intervene in the situation. For the evaluation of exclusion and the likelihood of intervention, there was a main effect of additional situational information, revealing that participants evaluated exclusion as less acceptable and were more likely to intervene if there was no additional situational information. Regarding the evaluation of exclusion, there was an interaction of additional situational information and the origin of the excluded student as the effect of additional situational information was bigger if the excluded student was German. For the likelihood of intervention, this interaction was not significant; but descriptively a similar pattern emerged. Results indicate that information about prior norm-violating behavior is more relevant for teachers’ reactions to social exclusion than the origin of the excluded student. However, in situations with an understandable reason for exclusion, teachers do include the origin of the excluded student in their considerations.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44881907","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interoceptive accuracy does not predict emotion perception in daily life 内感受准确性不能预测日常生活中的情绪知觉
Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2022-0009
A. Pohl, Sebastian Dummel, Mascha Bothur, A. Gerlach
Abstract Peripheral emotion theories suggest a crucial role of interoception for emotion perception, which in turn facilitates emotion regulation. Laboratory studies found positive relations between interoceptive accuracy and perceived emotion intensity and arousal. Studies in natural settings are largely missing, but seem important by virtue of emotional experience and regulation diversity. On hundred seven participants underwent a cardiovascular interoceptive accuracy task. Afterwards, participants provided detailed information on perceived emotions and emotion regulation strategies in an ecological momentary assessment (EMA). Multilevel models were calculated. In consideration of valence, emotion intensity, arousal, intensity of body sensations and, emotion regulation success were modeled as a function of centered interoceptive accuracy. Interoceptive accuracy did not predict any emotion perception criterion. Lower accuracy was related to a slightly stronger decrease of perceived arousal after regulation. Differences in emotion categories, intensity, and sample collection might explain divergences to laboratory studies.
外周情绪理论认为内感受在情绪知觉中起着至关重要的作用,而内感受又促进了情绪调节。实验室研究发现,内感受准确性与感知到的情绪强度和觉醒之间存在正相关关系。在自然环境下的研究在很大程度上缺失,但由于情感体验和调节多样性,似乎很重要。107名参与者接受了心血管内感受准确性任务。随后,参与者在生态瞬间评估(EMA)中提供了感知情绪和情绪调节策略的详细信息。计算了多层模型。考虑到效价、情绪强度、唤醒、身体感觉强度和,情绪调节成功被建模为中心内感受准确性的函数。内感受准确度不能预测任何情绪知觉标准。较低的准确性与调节后感知觉醒的轻微减弱有关。情绪类别、强度和样本收集的差异可能解释了实验室研究的分歧。
{"title":"Interoceptive accuracy does not predict emotion perception in daily life","authors":"A. Pohl, Sebastian Dummel, Mascha Bothur, A. Gerlach","doi":"10.1515/psych-2022-0009","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2022-0009","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Peripheral emotion theories suggest a crucial role of interoception for emotion perception, which in turn facilitates emotion regulation. Laboratory studies found positive relations between interoceptive accuracy and perceived emotion intensity and arousal. Studies in natural settings are largely missing, but seem important by virtue of emotional experience and regulation diversity. On hundred seven participants underwent a cardiovascular interoceptive accuracy task. Afterwards, participants provided detailed information on perceived emotions and emotion regulation strategies in an ecological momentary assessment (EMA). Multilevel models were calculated. In consideration of valence, emotion intensity, arousal, intensity of body sensations and, emotion regulation success were modeled as a function of centered interoceptive accuracy. Interoceptive accuracy did not predict any emotion perception criterion. Lower accuracy was related to a slightly stronger decrease of perceived arousal after regulation. Differences in emotion categories, intensity, and sample collection might explain divergences to laboratory studies.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45045020","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does Ego Depletion Elicit Stronger Cues of Deception? 自我耗尽是否会引发更强的欺骗线索?
Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2022-0129
Sarah C Volz, Marc-André Reinhard, P. Müller
Abstract Lying is cognitively demanding and presumably requires self-regulation. According to ego depletion theory, a task that requires self-regulation should therefore impair an individual’s ability to tell a convincing lie in a later task. Consequently, it was hypothesized that a manipulation of ego depletion would enhance behavioral differences between liars and truth-tellers. To manipulate ego depletion, participants worked (vs. did not work) on a task in which they had to suppress dominant responses while copying a text. Subsequently, they talked in a simulated job interview about a job they had previously held (vs. not held). In the sample of 164 participants, there was no evidence to support the hypothesis; the expected Ego Depletion x Veracity interaction was not significant for any of the 15 behavioral cues coded in the videotaped interviews. Although the main effect of ego depletion was significant at the multivariate level for the first of two parts of the interview, none of the univariate main effects reached the significance level corrected for multiple testing. Bayesian analyses rendered moderate to strong evidence in favor of the null hypothesis. Possible implications of the results are discussed, also those related to ego depletion theory.
说谎是一种认知要求,可能需要自我调节。根据自我损耗理论,需要自我调节的任务因此会削弱个体在随后的任务中说出令人信服的谎言的能力。因此,有人假设,操纵自我耗尽会增强说谎者和说真话者之间的行为差异。为了操纵自我耗竭,参与者在完成一项任务时(对照不完成)必须在抄写文本时抑制主导反应。随后,他们在模拟的工作面试中谈论了他们以前做过的工作(与没有做过的工作)。在164名参与者的样本中,没有证据支持这一假设;预期的自我损耗与准确性的相互作用对录像访谈中编码的15个行为线索中的任何一个都不显著。虽然自我耗竭的主效应在访谈的第一部分的多变量水平上是显著的,但没有一个单变量主效应达到多重检验校正的显著性水平。贝叶斯分析提供了中等到强烈的证据支持零假设。讨论了结果的可能含义,以及与自我耗尽理论相关的含义。
{"title":"Does Ego Depletion Elicit Stronger Cues of Deception?","authors":"Sarah C Volz, Marc-André Reinhard, P. Müller","doi":"10.1515/psych-2022-0129","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2022-0129","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Lying is cognitively demanding and presumably requires self-regulation. According to ego depletion theory, a task that requires self-regulation should therefore impair an individual’s ability to tell a convincing lie in a later task. Consequently, it was hypothesized that a manipulation of ego depletion would enhance behavioral differences between liars and truth-tellers. To manipulate ego depletion, participants worked (vs. did not work) on a task in which they had to suppress dominant responses while copying a text. Subsequently, they talked in a simulated job interview about a job they had previously held (vs. not held). In the sample of 164 participants, there was no evidence to support the hypothesis; the expected Ego Depletion x Veracity interaction was not significant for any of the 15 behavioral cues coded in the videotaped interviews. Although the main effect of ego depletion was significant at the multivariate level for the first of two parts of the interview, none of the univariate main effects reached the significance level corrected for multiple testing. Bayesian analyses rendered moderate to strong evidence in favor of the null hypothesis. Possible implications of the results are discussed, also those related to ego depletion theory.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42637626","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Open psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1