首页 > 最新文献

Open psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Reanalysis of an Allocentric Navigation Strategy Scale based on Item Response Theory 基于项目反应理论的异心导航策略量表再分析
Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2020-0100
Jimmy Y. Zhong
Abstract Focusing on 12 allocentric/survey-based strategy items of the Navigation Strategy Questionnaire (Zhong & Kozhevnikov, 2016), the current study applied item response theory-based analysis to determine whether a bidimensional model could better describe the latent structure of the survey-based strategy. Results from item and model fit diagnostics, categorical response and item information curves showed that an item with the lowest rotated component loading (.27) [SURVEY12], could be considered for exclusion in future studies; and that a bidimensional model with three preference-related items constituting a content factor offered a better representation of the latent structure than a unidimensional model per se. Mean scores from these three items also correlated significantly with a pointing-to-landmarks task to the same relative magnitude as the mean scores from all items, and all items excluding SURVEY12. These findings gave early evidence suggesting that the three preference-related items could constitute a subscale for deriving quick estimates of large-scale allocentric spatial processing in healthy adults in both experimental and clinical settings. Potential cognitive and brain mechanisms were discussed, followed by calls for future studies to gather greater evidence confirming the predictive validity of the full and sub scales, along with the design of new items focusing on environmental familiarity.
摘要本研究以导航策略问卷(Zhong&Kozhevnikov,2016)中的12个非中心/基于调查的策略项目为重点,应用基于项目反应理论的分析来确定二维模型是否能够更好地描述基于调查的战略的潜在结构。项目和模型拟合诊断、分类反应和项目信息曲线的结果表明,在未来的研究中,可以考虑排除具有最低旋转分量负荷(.27)[SURVEY12]的项目;与一维模型本身相比,由三个与偏好相关的项目构成内容因子的二维模型提供了更好的潜在结构表示。这三个项目的平均得分也与指向地标任务显著相关,其相对幅度与所有项目以及除SURVEY12外的所有项目的平均分数相同。这些发现提供了早期证据,表明这三个与偏好相关的项目可以构成一个分量表,用于在实验和临床环境中快速估计健康成年人的大规模非中心空间处理。讨论了潜在的认知和大脑机制,随后呼吁未来的研究收集更多证据,证实全量表和亚量表的预测有效性,并设计新的项目,重点关注环境熟悉度。
{"title":"Reanalysis of an Allocentric Navigation Strategy Scale based on Item Response Theory","authors":"Jimmy Y. Zhong","doi":"10.1515/psych-2020-0100","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2020-0100","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Focusing on 12 allocentric/survey-based strategy items of the Navigation Strategy Questionnaire (Zhong & Kozhevnikov, 2016), the current study applied item response theory-based analysis to determine whether a bidimensional model could better describe the latent structure of the survey-based strategy. Results from item and model fit diagnostics, categorical response and item information curves showed that an item with the lowest rotated component loading (.27) [SURVEY12], could be considered for exclusion in future studies; and that a bidimensional model with three preference-related items constituting a content factor offered a better representation of the latent structure than a unidimensional model per se. Mean scores from these three items also correlated significantly with a pointing-to-landmarks task to the same relative magnitude as the mean scores from all items, and all items excluding SURVEY12. These findings gave early evidence suggesting that the three preference-related items could constitute a subscale for deriving quick estimates of large-scale allocentric spatial processing in healthy adults in both experimental and clinical settings. Potential cognitive and brain mechanisms were discussed, followed by calls for future studies to gather greater evidence confirming the predictive validity of the full and sub scales, along with the design of new items focusing on environmental familiarity.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":"2 1","pages":"90 - 105"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/psych-2020-0100","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45506189","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Is subliminal face processing modulated by attentional task sets? Evidence from masked priming effects in a gender decision task 阈下面部加工是否受到注意任务集的调节?性别决策任务中隐藏启动效应的证据
Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2020-0006
Eun-Jim Sim, Marcel Harpaintner, M. Kiefer
Abstract Unlike classical theories of automaticity, refined theories suggest that unconscious automatic processes depend on cognitive control settings. Cognitive control influences on unconscious word and object processing are well documented, but corresponding findings in the field of face processing are heterogeneous. The present study therefore investigated, whether subliminal face priming in a gender categorization task is susceptible to feature-specific attention. Participants performed a gender decision task by orthogonally varying gender congruency (prime-target: same vs. different gender) and emotion congruency (prime-target: same vs. different emotional facial expression) using a masked priming paradigm. Perceptual vs. emotional induction tasks, performed prior to prime presentation, served to activate corresponding attentional task sets. Subliminal gender priming (faster reactions to gender-congruent primes) differed as a function of induction task and emotional congruency. Following perceptual induction, gender priming was only obtained in the emotionally congruent condition, whereas following emotional induction gender priming was observed independently of emotional congruency. In line with the classical notion of automaticity, subliminal gender priming did not depend on a specific attentional focus. However, attention to shape facilitated subliminal processing of task-irrelevant emotional facial expressions. Most likely, mutual facilitation of emotionally congruent prime and target representations enhanced gender priming compared with emotionally incongruent pairings.
与经典的自动性理论不同,改进的理论认为无意识的自动过程依赖于认知控制设置。认知控制对无意识文字和物体加工的影响已有文献记载,但在面部加工领域的相应研究结果却不一致。因此,本研究调查了性别分类任务中的阈下面部启动是否容易受到特征特异性注意的影响。被试使用一个隐藏启动范式,通过正交改变性别一致性(启动-目标:相同与不同性别)和情绪一致性(启动-目标:相同与不同的情绪面部表情)来完成性别决策任务。知觉和情绪诱导任务,在启动呈现之前进行,有助于激活相应的注意任务集。阈下性别启动(对性别一致启动的快速反应)在诱导任务和情绪一致性的作用下存在差异。在知觉诱导后,性别启动仅在情绪一致条件下产生,而在情绪诱导后,性别启动不受情绪一致条件的影响。与经典的自动性概念一致,阈下性别启动并不依赖于特定的注意焦点。然而,对形状的注意促进了与任务无关的情绪面部表情的阈下加工。最有可能的是,与情绪不一致的配对相比,情绪一致的启动和目标表征的相互促进增强了性别启动。
{"title":"Is subliminal face processing modulated by attentional task sets? Evidence from masked priming effects in a gender decision task","authors":"Eun-Jim Sim, Marcel Harpaintner, M. Kiefer","doi":"10.1515/psych-2020-0006","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2020-0006","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Unlike classical theories of automaticity, refined theories suggest that unconscious automatic processes depend on cognitive control settings. Cognitive control influences on unconscious word and object processing are well documented, but corresponding findings in the field of face processing are heterogeneous. The present study therefore investigated, whether subliminal face priming in a gender categorization task is susceptible to feature-specific attention. Participants performed a gender decision task by orthogonally varying gender congruency (prime-target: same vs. different gender) and emotion congruency (prime-target: same vs. different emotional facial expression) using a masked priming paradigm. Perceptual vs. emotional induction tasks, performed prior to prime presentation, served to activate corresponding attentional task sets. Subliminal gender priming (faster reactions to gender-congruent primes) differed as a function of induction task and emotional congruency. Following perceptual induction, gender priming was only obtained in the emotionally congruent condition, whereas following emotional induction gender priming was observed independently of emotional congruency. In line with the classical notion of automaticity, subliminal gender priming did not depend on a specific attentional focus. However, attention to shape facilitated subliminal processing of task-irrelevant emotional facial expressions. Most likely, mutual facilitation of emotionally congruent prime and target representations enhanced gender priming compared with emotionally incongruent pairings.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":"2 1","pages":"76 - 89"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/psych-2020-0006","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42469078","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Varieties of abstract concepts and their grounding in perception or action 各种抽象概念及其在感知或行动中的基础
Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2020-0104
M. Kiefer, Marcel Harpaintner
Abstract For a very long time, theorizing in the cognitive sciences was dominated by the assumption that abstract concepts, which lack a perceivable referent, can only be handled by amodal or verbal linguistic representations. In the last years, however, refined grounded cognition theories emphasizing the importance of emotional and introspective information for abstract concepts, in addition to verbal associations and sensorimotor information, have received increasing support. Here, we review theoretical accounts of the structure and neural basis of conceptual memory and evaluate them in light of recent empirical evidence with regard to the processing of concrete and abstract concepts. Based on this literature review, we argue that abstract concepts should not be treated as a homogenous conceptual category, whose meaning is established by one single specific type of representation. Instead, depending on the feature composition, there are different subgroups of abstract concepts, including those with strong relations to vision or action, which are represented in the visual and motor brain systems similar to concrete concepts. The reviewed findings with regard to concrete and abstract concepts can be accommodated best by hybrid theories of conceptual representation assuming an interaction between modality-specific, multimodal and amodal hub areas.
摘要在很长一段时间里,认知科学的理论化一直被这样一种假设所主导,即抽象概念缺乏可感知的指称,只能通过语言表征来处理。然而,在过去的几年里,除了言语联想和感觉运动信息外,强调情感和内省信息对抽象概念的重要性的精炼的基础认知理论得到了越来越多的支持。在这里,我们回顾了概念记忆的结构和神经基础的理论描述,并根据最近关于具体和抽象概念处理的经验证据对其进行了评估。基于这篇文献综述,我们认为抽象概念不应被视为一个同质的概念类别,其含义是由一种特定的表示类型建立的。相反,根据特征组成,抽象概念有不同的亚组,包括那些与视觉或动作有强烈关系的概念,它们在视觉和运动脑系统中表现得与具体概念相似。关于具体和抽象概念的综述结果可以通过概念表征的混合理论来最好地适应,假设特定模态、多模态和非模态中枢区域之间的相互作用。
{"title":"Varieties of abstract concepts and their grounding in perception or action","authors":"M. Kiefer, Marcel Harpaintner","doi":"10.1515/psych-2020-0104","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2020-0104","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract For a very long time, theorizing in the cognitive sciences was dominated by the assumption that abstract concepts, which lack a perceivable referent, can only be handled by amodal or verbal linguistic representations. In the last years, however, refined grounded cognition theories emphasizing the importance of emotional and introspective information for abstract concepts, in addition to verbal associations and sensorimotor information, have received increasing support. Here, we review theoretical accounts of the structure and neural basis of conceptual memory and evaluate them in light of recent empirical evidence with regard to the processing of concrete and abstract concepts. Based on this literature review, we argue that abstract concepts should not be treated as a homogenous conceptual category, whose meaning is established by one single specific type of representation. Instead, depending on the feature composition, there are different subgroups of abstract concepts, including those with strong relations to vision or action, which are represented in the visual and motor brain systems similar to concrete concepts. The reviewed findings with regard to concrete and abstract concepts can be accommodated best by hybrid theories of conceptual representation assuming an interaction between modality-specific, multimodal and amodal hub areas.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":"2 1","pages":"119 - 137"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/psych-2020-0104","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45186693","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 38
Evidence Against Combined Effects of Stress and Brain Stimulation on Working Memory 反对压力和大脑刺激对工作记忆的联合影响的证据
Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2020-0004
M. A. Friehs, C. Frings
Abstract The effect of stress on working memory has been traced back to a modulation of the prefrontal cortex (PFC). We investigated the effects of neuromodulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (lDLPFC) after exposure to psychosocial stress through the Socially Evaluated Cold Pressure Test (SECPT). The hypothesis was that neuromodulation interacts with the stress intervention, to either boost performance even under stressed conditions or compensate negative stress effects. Fifty-nine participants were randomly divided into two groups. One group received active, anodal, offline transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the lDLPFC while the other group received sham stimulation. Participants performed a lexical n-back task, before and after the SECPT and tDCS intervention. The first n-back task was used as a baseline measurement and the second n-back task was performed during recovery from stress when cortisol levels are at their peak, but still under the influence of tDCS aftereffects. Additionally, after the psychosocial stress phase participants were post-hoc divided into cortisol responders and nonresponders. Results showed that generally stress increased lexical n-back task performance as indicated by faster correct reaction times and higher accuracy but that this was not modulated by tDCS. Crucially, using Bayes analysis we obtained evidence against the influence of anodal tDCS on stressed individual’s working memory performance.
摘要压力对工作记忆的影响可以追溯到前额叶皮层(PFC)的调节。我们通过社会评估冷压力测试(SECPT)研究了暴露于心理社会压力后左背外侧前额叶皮质(lDLPFC)的神经调控的影响。该假说认为神经调控和压力干预相互作用,即使在压力条件下也能提高表现,或者补偿负面的压力影响。59名参与者被随机分为两组。一组通过lDLPFC接受主动、阳极、离线经颅直流电刺激(tDCS),而另一组接受假刺激。参与者在SECPT和tDCS干预前后进行了一项词汇n-back任务。第一项n-back任务被用作基线测量,第二项n-back工作是在皮质醇水平达到峰值但仍受tDCS后遗症影响的压力恢复期间进行的。此外,在心理社会应激阶段后,参与者被事后分为皮质醇反应者和无反应者。结果表明,一般来说,压力会提高词汇n-back任务的表现,表现为更快的正确反应时间和更高的准确性,但这不受tDCS的调节。至关重要的是,使用贝叶斯分析,我们获得了反对阳极tDCS对压力个体工作记忆表现影响的证据。
{"title":"Evidence Against Combined Effects of Stress and Brain Stimulation on Working Memory","authors":"M. A. Friehs, C. Frings","doi":"10.1515/psych-2020-0004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2020-0004","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The effect of stress on working memory has been traced back to a modulation of the prefrontal cortex (PFC). We investigated the effects of neuromodulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (lDLPFC) after exposure to psychosocial stress through the Socially Evaluated Cold Pressure Test (SECPT). The hypothesis was that neuromodulation interacts with the stress intervention, to either boost performance even under stressed conditions or compensate negative stress effects. Fifty-nine participants were randomly divided into two groups. One group received active, anodal, offline transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the lDLPFC while the other group received sham stimulation. Participants performed a lexical n-back task, before and after the SECPT and tDCS intervention. The first n-back task was used as a baseline measurement and the second n-back task was performed during recovery from stress when cortisol levels are at their peak, but still under the influence of tDCS aftereffects. Additionally, after the psychosocial stress phase participants were post-hoc divided into cortisol responders and nonresponders. Results showed that generally stress increased lexical n-back task performance as indicated by faster correct reaction times and higher accuracy but that this was not modulated by tDCS. Crucially, using Bayes analysis we obtained evidence against the influence of anodal tDCS on stressed individual’s working memory performance.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":"2 1","pages":"40 - 56"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/psych-2020-0004","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45947299","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
Spatial thinking from a different view: Disentangling top-down and bottom-up processes using eye tracking 不同视角的空间思维:用眼动追踪解开自上而下和自下而上的过程
Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2020-0105
B. Fehringer
Abstract The goal of the present study was to investigate the potential of gaze fixation patterns to reflect cognitive processing steps during test performance. Gaze movements, however, can reflect top-down and bottom-up processes. Top-down processes are the cognitive processing steps that are necessary to solve a certain test item. In contrast, bottom-up processes may be provoked by varying visual features that are not related to the item solution. To disentangle top-down and bottom-up processes in the context of spatial thinking, a new test (R-Cube-Vis Test) was developed and validated explicitly for the usage of eye tracking in three studies as long and short version. The R-Cube-Vis Test measures visualization and is conform to the linear logistic test model with six difficulty levels. All items of one level demand the same transformation steps to solve an item. The R-Cube-Vis Test was then utilized to investigate different gaze-fixation-based indicators to identify top-down and bottom-up processes. Some of the indicators were also able to predict the correctness of the answer of a single item. Gaze-related measures have a high potential to reveal cognitive processing steps during solving an item of a given difficulty level, if top-down and bottom-up processes can be segregated.
摘要本研究的目的是探讨注视模式在测试过程中反映认知加工步骤的潜力。然而,凝视运动可以反映自上而下和自下而上的过程。自顶向下过程是解决某一测试项目所必需的认知加工步骤。相反,自底向上过程可能由与项目解决方案无关的不同视觉特征引起。为了理清空间思维背景下自上而下和自下而上的过程,我们开发了一个新的测试(R-Cube-Vis测试),并在三个研究中明确验证了眼动追踪的使用,分为长版本和短版本。R-Cube-Vis测试测量可视化,符合线性逻辑测试模型,具有六个难度等级。同一级别的所有项目都需要相同的转换步骤来解决一个项目。然后利用R-Cube-Vis测试来研究基于注视的不同指标,以确定自上而下和自下而上的过程。其中一些指标还能预测单个问题答案的正确性。如果可以将自上而下和自下而上的过程分离开来,那么与凝视相关的测量方法很有可能揭示在解决给定难度级别的项目时的认知处理步骤。
{"title":"Spatial thinking from a different view: Disentangling top-down and bottom-up processes using eye tracking","authors":"B. Fehringer","doi":"10.1515/psych-2020-0105","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2020-0105","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The goal of the present study was to investigate the potential of gaze fixation patterns to reflect cognitive processing steps during test performance. Gaze movements, however, can reflect top-down and bottom-up processes. Top-down processes are the cognitive processing steps that are necessary to solve a certain test item. In contrast, bottom-up processes may be provoked by varying visual features that are not related to the item solution. To disentangle top-down and bottom-up processes in the context of spatial thinking, a new test (R-Cube-Vis Test) was developed and validated explicitly for the usage of eye tracking in three studies as long and short version. The R-Cube-Vis Test measures visualization and is conform to the linear logistic test model with six difficulty levels. All items of one level demand the same transformation steps to solve an item. The R-Cube-Vis Test was then utilized to investigate different gaze-fixation-based indicators to identify top-down and bottom-up processes. Some of the indicators were also able to predict the correctness of the answer of a single item. Gaze-related measures have a high potential to reveal cognitive processing steps during solving an item of a given difficulty level, if top-down and bottom-up processes can be segregated.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":"2 1","pages":"138 - 212"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/psych-2020-0105","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45604745","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Sounds in the classroom: Auditory context-dependent memory in school-aged children 课堂上的声音:学龄儿童的听觉情境依赖记忆
Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2020-0101
Anna L. Ostendorf, Helge Schlüter, R. Hackländer
Abstract Context-dependent memory (CDM) is the effect whereby information is retrieved more accurately in the presence of the contextual information that was present during encoding than in the absence of that contextual information. Most previous CDM experiments have focused on spatial location, but contexts such as sights, smells, and sounds have also been shown to be effective mnemonic cues, although the research is more limited. In relation to auditory contexts, much of the previous research has focused on music and on adults. We were interested in determining whether auditory CDM effects could be found in a classroom setting in school-aged children using background noises. Across two experiments we found that the reinstatement of the auditory context improved memory performance for 2nd, 3rd, and 5th grade students. Sounds, not just musical pieces, are stored in memory and can be effective contextual mnemonic cues. Further, (auditory) CDM effects can be found in young children. Teachers should be aware of the influence of contextual auditory cues in the classroom setting, and how this information is stored along with the focus of the teaching lesson.
摘要上下文相关记忆(CDM)是在存在编码期间存在的上下文信息的情况下比在不存在该上下文信息的情形下更准确地检索信息的效果。以前的大多数CDM实验都集中在空间位置上,但视觉、气味和声音等环境也被证明是有效的记忆线索,尽管研究更为有限。关于听觉环境,以前的研究大多集中在音乐和成年人身上。我们感兴趣的是确定在使用背景噪声的学龄儿童的课堂环境中是否可以发现听觉CDM效应。在两个实验中,我们发现恢复听觉环境可以改善二年级、三年级和五年级学生的记忆表现。声音,不仅仅是音乐片段,存储在记忆中,可以成为有效的上下文记忆线索。此外,(听觉)CDM效应可以在幼儿中发现。教师应该意识到语境听觉线索在课堂环境中的影响,以及这些信息是如何与教学重点一起存储的。
{"title":"Sounds in the classroom: Auditory context-dependent memory in school-aged children","authors":"Anna L. Ostendorf, Helge Schlüter, R. Hackländer","doi":"10.1515/psych-2020-0101","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2020-0101","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Context-dependent memory (CDM) is the effect whereby information is retrieved more accurately in the presence of the contextual information that was present during encoding than in the absence of that contextual information. Most previous CDM experiments have focused on spatial location, but contexts such as sights, smells, and sounds have also been shown to be effective mnemonic cues, although the research is more limited. In relation to auditory contexts, much of the previous research has focused on music and on adults. We were interested in determining whether auditory CDM effects could be found in a classroom setting in school-aged children using background noises. Across two experiments we found that the reinstatement of the auditory context improved memory performance for 2nd, 3rd, and 5th grade students. Sounds, not just musical pieces, are stored in memory and can be effective contextual mnemonic cues. Further, (auditory) CDM effects can be found in young children. Teachers should be aware of the influence of contextual auditory cues in the classroom setting, and how this information is stored along with the focus of the teaching lesson.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":"2 1","pages":"106 - 118"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/psych-2020-0101","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47952794","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Perceived and one’s own motion in response priming 反应启动时感知到的和自己的动作
Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2020-0106
C. Bermeitinger, R. Hackländer, Marie Kollek, Matthis Stiegemeyer, Alexandra E. Tränkner
Abstract In response priming, motor pre-activations from a prime to the response to a target can be measured, as a function of whether they require the same (compatible) or different (incompatible) responses. With moving primes and static arrow targets, the results depend on the stimulus onset asynchrony between prime and target: with short SOAs, there were faster responses to compatible than incompatible targets, with longer SOAs, the pattern reverses. However, this reversal was not found with more biological motions. The current study comprised 3 experiments in order to replicate several findings from previous research and add evidence regarding the interplay of one’s own and perceived motions. Subjects performed a response priming task with moving prime stimuli while in motion themselves. With this paradigm, we tested the general influence of motion on responding and compatibility effects in response priming with moving prime stimuli. Furthermore, we assessed specific interactions of features of the perceived stimuli (e.g., moving vs. static; direction of the prime or target) and the own motion (e.g., walking vs. standing; direction of being rotated). We used two different own motions (walking on a treadmill, Exp. 1 & 3; rotating in a human gyroscope, Exp. 2) and two different visual stimulus types (rows-of-dots, Exp. 1 & 2; point light displays, Exp. 3). Compatibility effects were, in general, neither increased nor decreased during motion. Their size depended on the stimulus type, the velocity of one’s own motion, and several interactions of perceived and own motion. We discuss our findings with respect to perception-action interactions and previous findings on response priming with moving prime stimuli.
摘要在反应启动中,可以测量从启动到对目标的反应的运动预激活,作为它们需要相同(兼容)还是不同(不兼容)反应的函数。对于移动素数和静态箭头目标,结果取决于素数和目标之间的刺激开始异步性:对于短SOA,对兼容目标的响应比不兼容目标更快,对于长SOA,模式相反。然而,这种逆转并没有在更多的生物运动中发现。目前的研究包括3个实验,目的是复制之前研究的一些发现,并增加关于自己和感知运动相互作用的证据。受试者在自己运动的同时,用移动的主要刺激进行反应启动任务。利用这一范式,我们测试了运动对反应的一般影响,以及反应启动与运动启动刺激的相容性效应。此外,我们评估了感知刺激特征(例如,移动与静止;素数或目标的方向)和自身运动(例如,行走与站立;旋转的方向)的具体相互作用。我们使用了两种不同的自身运动(在跑步机上行走,实验1&3;在人类陀螺仪中旋转,实验2)和两种不同类型的视觉刺激(点行,实验1&2;点光源显示,实验3)。一般来说,在运动过程中,相容性效应既没有增加也没有减少。它们的大小取决于刺激类型、自身运动的速度以及感知和自身运动的几种相互作用。我们讨论了我们关于感知-动作相互作用的发现,以及之前关于运动主刺激的反应启动的发现。
{"title":"Perceived and one’s own motion in response priming","authors":"C. Bermeitinger, R. Hackländer, Marie Kollek, Matthis Stiegemeyer, Alexandra E. Tränkner","doi":"10.1515/psych-2020-0106","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2020-0106","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract In response priming, motor pre-activations from a prime to the response to a target can be measured, as a function of whether they require the same (compatible) or different (incompatible) responses. With moving primes and static arrow targets, the results depend on the stimulus onset asynchrony between prime and target: with short SOAs, there were faster responses to compatible than incompatible targets, with longer SOAs, the pattern reverses. However, this reversal was not found with more biological motions. The current study comprised 3 experiments in order to replicate several findings from previous research and add evidence regarding the interplay of one’s own and perceived motions. Subjects performed a response priming task with moving prime stimuli while in motion themselves. With this paradigm, we tested the general influence of motion on responding and compatibility effects in response priming with moving prime stimuli. Furthermore, we assessed specific interactions of features of the perceived stimuli (e.g., moving vs. static; direction of the prime or target) and the own motion (e.g., walking vs. standing; direction of being rotated). We used two different own motions (walking on a treadmill, Exp. 1 & 3; rotating in a human gyroscope, Exp. 2) and two different visual stimulus types (rows-of-dots, Exp. 1 & 2; point light displays, Exp. 3). Compatibility effects were, in general, neither increased nor decreased during motion. Their size depended on the stimulus type, the velocity of one’s own motion, and several interactions of perceived and own motion. We discuss our findings with respect to perception-action interactions and previous findings on response priming with moving prime stimuli.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":"2 1","pages":"213 - 237"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/psych-2020-0106","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45843561","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lessons Learned from an ACT-Based Physician-Delivered Weight Loss Intervention: A Pilot RCT Demonstrates Limits to Feasibility 基于act的医生减肥干预的经验教训:一项试点随机对照试验证明了可行性的局限性
Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2020-0003
Mallory Frayn, K. Carrière, B. Knäuper
Abstract Background: Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) interventions have shown to be effective in facilitating weight loss for emotional eaters, however, the lack of accessibility of these interventions limits their impact. The present study aimed to increase the accessibility of an ACT intervention for emotional eaters through delivery by physicians. Methods: This two-arm pilot randomized controlled trial tested the effectiveness and feasibility of a brief ACT intervention for emotional eaters compared with standard care at a weight loss clinic in Toronto, Canada. Primary outcomes were changes in weight and emotional eating. Treatment satisfaction was also assessed. Results: Participants in neither condition lost weight. Both conditions displayed decreases in emotional eating, but no condition interaction was found. Both patients and physicians reported high treatment satisfaction with the ACT intervention. However, there were high attrition rates and variability in intervention completion times. Conclusion: The ACT intervention led to reductions in emotional eating and was well received by patients and physicians alike. However, the present study identified high attrition as a limitation to the feasibility of this mode of intervention delivery. Future interventions may be more effectively delivered in primary care settings by encouraging further brevity and exploring delivery by other health professionals. Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT03611829. Registered 26 July 2018. Retrospectively registered.
背景:接受和承诺治疗(ACT)干预措施已被证明在促进情绪进食者减肥方面是有效的,然而,缺乏这些干预措施的可及性限制了它们的影响。本研究旨在通过医生的交付,增加对情绪化进食者的ACT干预的可及性。方法:在加拿大多伦多的一家减肥诊所进行的一项两组随机对照试验,测试了对情绪化进食者进行简短ACT干预的有效性和可行性,并与标准治疗进行了比较。主要结果是体重和情绪化饮食的变化。治疗满意度也进行了评估。结果:两种情况下的参与者体重都有所下降。两种情况下情绪性进食都有所减少,但没有发现相互作用。患者和医生对ACT干预的治疗满意度都很高。然而,在干预完成时间方面存在较高的损耗率和可变性。结论:ACT干预导致情绪性进食减少,受到患者和医生的一致好评。然而,目前的研究发现,高损耗是这种干预方式可行性的一个限制。未来的干预措施可以通过鼓励进一步简化和探索由其他卫生专业人员提供,从而在初级保健环境中更有效地提供。试验注册:ClinicalTrials.gov NCT03611829。2018年7月26日注册。回顾注册。
{"title":"Lessons Learned from an ACT-Based Physician-Delivered Weight Loss Intervention: A Pilot RCT Demonstrates Limits to Feasibility","authors":"Mallory Frayn, K. Carrière, B. Knäuper","doi":"10.1515/psych-2020-0003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2020-0003","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Background: Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) interventions have shown to be effective in facilitating weight loss for emotional eaters, however, the lack of accessibility of these interventions limits their impact. The present study aimed to increase the accessibility of an ACT intervention for emotional eaters through delivery by physicians. Methods: This two-arm pilot randomized controlled trial tested the effectiveness and feasibility of a brief ACT intervention for emotional eaters compared with standard care at a weight loss clinic in Toronto, Canada. Primary outcomes were changes in weight and emotional eating. Treatment satisfaction was also assessed. Results: Participants in neither condition lost weight. Both conditions displayed decreases in emotional eating, but no condition interaction was found. Both patients and physicians reported high treatment satisfaction with the ACT intervention. However, there were high attrition rates and variability in intervention completion times. Conclusion: The ACT intervention led to reductions in emotional eating and was well received by patients and physicians alike. However, the present study identified high attrition as a limitation to the feasibility of this mode of intervention delivery. Future interventions may be more effectively delivered in primary care settings by encouraging further brevity and exploring delivery by other health professionals. Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT03611829. Registered 26 July 2018. Retrospectively registered.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":"2 1","pages":"22 - 39"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/psych-2020-0003","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46933372","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
How can we learn what attention is? Response gating via multiple direct routes kept in check by inhibitory control processes 我们怎么知道什么是注意力?反应门控通过多种直接途径保持在抑制控制过程的检查
Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2020-0107
Sven Panis
Abstract To explore the time course of space- and object-based attentional selection processes I analysed the shapes of the response time (RT) and accuracy distributions of left/right arrow identification responses in the two-rectangle paradigm. After cueing one of the four ends of two horizontally or vertically oriented rectangles the arrow typically appears at the cued location (valid), or sometimes at an uncued location in the same (invalid-same) or other rectangle (invalid-different). The data point to a multiple-route model in which (a) an informative cue generates response channel activation before arrow signals emerge, (b) the task-irrelevant arrow location is represented in multiple egocentric and allocentric reference frames around 150 ms after target onset, with the former including a reference frame centered on the currently attended location, (c) the task-irrelevant spatial codes activate premature response tendencies that are actively inhibited to allow gating of arrow direction signals, (d) after an invalid cue the onset of the arrow triggers an “attention shift” – acting between 150 and 240 ms after target onset – that strongly interferes with task performance in certain conditions (invalid-same cueing with horizontal rectangles, and invalid-different cueing with vertical rectangles), and (e) participants differ in which task-irrelevant codes they preferentially inhibit. These results pave the way for future confirmatory studies to temporally characterize and disentangle the contributions of different types of response channel activation processes, from those of reactive cognitive control processes including active and selective response suppression.
摘要:为了探究空间和物体为基础的注意选择过程的时间过程,本文分析了双矩形范式下左右箭头识别反应的反应时间(RT)形状和准确性分布。在提示两个水平或垂直方向矩形的四个端点之一之后,箭头通常出现在提示位置(有效),或者有时出现在同一(无效-相同)或其他矩形(无效-不同)中的未提示位置。数据指向一个多路径模型,其中(a)信息提示在箭头信号出现之前产生反应通道激活,(b)与任务无关的箭头位置在目标开始后150毫秒左右以多个以自我为中心和以非中心为中心的参考帧中表示,其中前者包括一个以当前参与位置为中心的参考帧。(c)与任务无关的空间编码激活了过早反应倾向,这种倾向被积极抑制,从而允许箭头方向信号的门控;(d)在无效提示后,箭头的出现引发了“注意力转移”——在目标出现后150至240毫秒之间发生——在某些条件下(与水平矩形相同的无效提示,以及与垂直矩形不同的无效提示)强烈干扰任务表现;(e)参与者在优先抑制哪些与任务无关的代码方面存在差异。这些结果为未来的验证性研究铺平了道路,以从反应性认知控制过程(包括主动和选择性反应抑制)中暂时表征和解开不同类型的反应通道激活过程的贡献。
{"title":"How can we learn what attention is? Response gating via multiple direct routes kept in check by inhibitory control processes","authors":"Sven Panis","doi":"10.1515/psych-2020-0107","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2020-0107","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract To explore the time course of space- and object-based attentional selection processes I analysed the shapes of the response time (RT) and accuracy distributions of left/right arrow identification responses in the two-rectangle paradigm. After cueing one of the four ends of two horizontally or vertically oriented rectangles the arrow typically appears at the cued location (valid), or sometimes at an uncued location in the same (invalid-same) or other rectangle (invalid-different). The data point to a multiple-route model in which (a) an informative cue generates response channel activation before arrow signals emerge, (b) the task-irrelevant arrow location is represented in multiple egocentric and allocentric reference frames around 150 ms after target onset, with the former including a reference frame centered on the currently attended location, (c) the task-irrelevant spatial codes activate premature response tendencies that are actively inhibited to allow gating of arrow direction signals, (d) after an invalid cue the onset of the arrow triggers an “attention shift” – acting between 150 and 240 ms after target onset – that strongly interferes with task performance in certain conditions (invalid-same cueing with horizontal rectangles, and invalid-different cueing with vertical rectangles), and (e) participants differ in which task-irrelevant codes they preferentially inhibit. These results pave the way for future confirmatory studies to temporally characterize and disentangle the contributions of different types of response channel activation processes, from those of reactive cognitive control processes including active and selective response suppression.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":"2 1","pages":"238 - 279"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/psych-2020-0107","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44212377","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
German Adaptation of the Subjective Vitality Scales (SVS-G) 主观活力量表(SVS-G)的德语改编
Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/psych-2020-0005
Alex Bertrams, Thomas Dyllick, C. Englert, Ann Krispenz
Abstract Subjective vitality is a positive feeling of aliveness and energy, and it is a crucial aspect of well-being. The Subjective Vitality Scales (SVS) have been developed to measure subjective vitality both at the individual difference level and the state level in English-speaking samples. We translated the SVS into German (the SVS-G) and examined their psychometric properties. In Study 1 (N=260), we found that two correlated factors (Factor 1: individual difference level; Factor 2: state level) with five items each constituted a useful structure for the SVS-G. Moreover, the scores on the individual difference scale were more stable than the scores on the state scale. We also found partial evidence for the measurement invariance over a period of three weeks. Conforming to our expectations, Study 2 (N=296) revealed that the SVS-G scores were related to positive and negative affect but could still be distinguished from the affect variables. In line with previous findings, Study 3 (N=203) showed that SVS-G scores are related to well-being variables (happiness and joviality) and the perceived capacity to actively perform effortful tasks (attentiveness and capacity for self-control). Across all the studies, the SVS-G showed satisfying inner consistency, and the two consideration levels (individual differences vs. state) could be differentiated. The initial evidence suggests that overall, the SVS-G have good psychometric properties.
摘要主体活力是一种积极的活力感,是幸福感的重要方面。主观活力量表(SVS)已被开发用于测量英语样本在个体差异水平和状态水平上的主观活力。我们将SVS翻译成德语(SVS-G),并检查了它们的心理测量特性。在研究1(N=260)中,我们发现两个相关因素(因素1:个体差异水平;因素2:状态水平)各有五个项目,构成了SVS-G的有用结构。此外,个体差异量表的得分比州量表的分数更稳定。我们还发现了在三周内测量不变性的部分证据。符合我们的预期,研究2(N=296)显示,SVS-G评分与积极和消极情绪有关,但仍可以与情绪变量区分开来。与之前的研究结果一致,研究3(N=203)表明,SVS-G评分与幸福感变量(幸福感和愉悦感)和积极执行努力任务的感知能力(注意力和自我控制能力)有关。在所有研究中,SVS-G表现出令人满意的内部一致性,并且两个考虑水平(个体差异与状态)可以区分。初步证据表明,总体而言,SVS-G具有良好的心理测量特性。
{"title":"German Adaptation of the Subjective Vitality Scales (SVS-G)","authors":"Alex Bertrams, Thomas Dyllick, C. Englert, Ann Krispenz","doi":"10.1515/psych-2020-0005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/psych-2020-0005","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Subjective vitality is a positive feeling of aliveness and energy, and it is a crucial aspect of well-being. The Subjective Vitality Scales (SVS) have been developed to measure subjective vitality both at the individual difference level and the state level in English-speaking samples. We translated the SVS into German (the SVS-G) and examined their psychometric properties. In Study 1 (N=260), we found that two correlated factors (Factor 1: individual difference level; Factor 2: state level) with five items each constituted a useful structure for the SVS-G. Moreover, the scores on the individual difference scale were more stable than the scores on the state scale. We also found partial evidence for the measurement invariance over a period of three weeks. Conforming to our expectations, Study 2 (N=296) revealed that the SVS-G scores were related to positive and negative affect but could still be distinguished from the affect variables. In line with previous findings, Study 3 (N=203) showed that SVS-G scores are related to well-being variables (happiness and joviality) and the perceived capacity to actively perform effortful tasks (attentiveness and capacity for self-control). Across all the studies, the SVS-G showed satisfying inner consistency, and the two consideration levels (individual differences vs. state) could be differentiated. The initial evidence suggests that overall, the SVS-G have good psychometric properties.","PeriodicalId":74357,"journal":{"name":"Open psychology","volume":"2 1","pages":"57 - 75"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/psych-2020-0005","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43132077","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 18
期刊
Open psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1