首页 > 最新文献

Canadian journal of psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Remote memory: recalling autobiographical and public events from across the lifespan. 远程记忆:回忆一生中发生的自传体和公共事件。
Pub Date : 1992-03-01 DOI: 10.1037/h0084311
J L Howes, A N Katz

In this study the recall of autobiographical and public events across the lifespan was examined in a sample of middle-aged and older-aged adults. The tasks were modified versions of one first introduced by Galton (1879), and they required subjects to recall events from specific time periods across their entire lifespan. The four tasks differed in the nature of the episodes requested (autobiographical or public) and whether recall was word-cued or non-word-cued. Verification of public events was assessed archivally, and autobiographical events were verified by a sample of relatives for a subgroup of the subjects and reported events. The results indicated that memory for public events decreases with increased age of the subject, but this effect is not generally found for the recall of autobiographical events. The older-aged subjects were able to recall an equal number of autobiographical episodes from all life segments, whereas recall of news events tended to decrease with remoteness of the episode. These data are contrary to those models that posit general memory loss as a function of age or of remoteness of the events.

在这项研究中,研究人员以中老年成人为样本,考察了他们一生中对自传体和公共事件的回忆。这些任务是高尔顿(1879年)首次提出的任务的修改版本,他们要求受试者回忆他们一生中特定时期的事件。这四项任务的不同之处是所要求的情节的性质(自传体的或公开的),以及回忆是由单词提示还是由非单词提示。对公共事件的验证以档案形式进行评估,自传式事件由受试者的亲属样本和报告事件进行验证。结果表明,对公共事件的记忆随着年龄的增长而下降,但对自传式事件的记忆则不存在这种效应。年龄较大的受试者能够回忆起所有生活片段中相同数量的自传体事件,而对新闻事件的回忆往往随着事件的遥远而减少。这些数据与那些假设一般记忆丧失是年龄或事件遥远程度的函数的模型相反。
{"title":"Remote memory: recalling autobiographical and public events from across the lifespan.","authors":"J L Howes,&nbsp;A N Katz","doi":"10.1037/h0084311","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084311","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this study the recall of autobiographical and public events across the lifespan was examined in a sample of middle-aged and older-aged adults. The tasks were modified versions of one first introduced by Galton (1879), and they required subjects to recall events from specific time periods across their entire lifespan. The four tasks differed in the nature of the episodes requested (autobiographical or public) and whether recall was word-cued or non-word-cued. Verification of public events was assessed archivally, and autobiographical events were verified by a sample of relatives for a subgroup of the subjects and reported events. The results indicated that memory for public events decreases with increased age of the subject, but this effect is not generally found for the recall of autobiographical events. The older-aged subjects were able to recall an equal number of autobiographical episodes from all life segments, whereas recall of news events tended to decrease with remoteness of the episode. These data are contrary to those models that posit general memory loss as a function of age or of remoteness of the events.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"46 1","pages":"92-116"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1992-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084311","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"12758859","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 61
Concurrent language and motor performance in bilinguals: a test of the age of acquisition hypothesis. 双语者并发语言和运动表现:对习得年龄假说的检验。
Pub Date : 1991-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/h0084306
J C Furtado, W G Webster

The objective of the present study was to test the hypothesis that the age at which a second language is acquired influences the pattern of cerebral lateralization associated with that language. Subjects who differed in terms of the age at which they had acquired their second language (English or French) were tested on a concurrent task paradigm involving motor and language performance. Hemispheric processing was inferred from the pattern of lateralized and generalized interference between the tasks. No support was found for the age-of-acquisition hypothesis. Instead, the data indicated a language-specific effect. Regardless of age of acquisition and of whether the first language was English or French, bilingual subjects showed lateralized interference effects consistent with left-hemisphere processing when reading in English and translating from French into English, but no lateralized interference when reading in French and translating from English into French. Whether this effect reflects characteristics of the two languages or the influence of social factors in subject-experimenter interaction is considered.

本研究的目的是验证一种假设,即习得第二语言的年龄会影响与该语言相关的大脑侧化模式。学习第二语言(英语或法语)年龄不同的受试者接受了涉及运动和语言表现的并发任务范式测试。从任务之间的横向和广义干扰模式推断出半球加工。没有发现对习得年龄假说的支持。相反,数据显示了一种特定于语言的影响。无论习得年龄和第一语言是英语还是法语,双语受试者在阅读英语和从法语翻译成英语时表现出与左半球加工一致的偏侧干扰效应,但在阅读法语和从英语翻译成法语时没有偏侧干扰。这种效应是反映了两种语言的特点,还是受被试-实验者互动中社会因素的影响。
{"title":"Concurrent language and motor performance in bilinguals: a test of the age of acquisition hypothesis.","authors":"J C Furtado,&nbsp;W G Webster","doi":"10.1037/h0084306","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084306","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The objective of the present study was to test the hypothesis that the age at which a second language is acquired influences the pattern of cerebral lateralization associated with that language. Subjects who differed in terms of the age at which they had acquired their second language (English or French) were tested on a concurrent task paradigm involving motor and language performance. Hemispheric processing was inferred from the pattern of lateralized and generalized interference between the tasks. No support was found for the age-of-acquisition hypothesis. Instead, the data indicated a language-specific effect. Regardless of age of acquisition and of whether the first language was English or French, bilingual subjects showed lateralized interference effects consistent with left-hemisphere processing when reading in English and translating from French into English, but no lateralized interference when reading in French and translating from English into French. Whether this effect reflects characteristics of the two languages or the influence of social factors in subject-experimenter interaction is considered.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 4","pages":"448-61"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084306","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"12941524","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 24
Attentional factors in visual field asymmetries. 视野不对称的注意因素。
Pub Date : 1991-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/h0084305
M P Bryden, T A Mondor

Over the past 30 years, numerous studies have reported left/right asymmetries in visual field performance, with performance generally superior in the right visual field for verbal tasks and in the left visual field for spatial tasks. These asymmetries parallel those found in neurological studies of hemispheric specialization. Consequently, many investigators have concluded that visual hemifield differences are primarily a reflection of the functional differences between the two cerebral hemispheres. However, alternative explanations proposing that visual field effects are dependent on other factors such as inadequate fixation, eye movements during presentation, postexposural scanning, and attentional biases have been offered. The potential impact of each of these factors on visual field differences are reviewed and discussed. Evidence is provided suggesting that attention and hemispheric functional differences interact to produce the magnitude and direction of visual field differences.

在过去的30年里,大量的研究报告了左右视野表现的不对称性,在言语任务中,右视野表现优于左视野,在空间任务中,左视野表现优于右视野。这些不对称与在半球特化的神经学研究中发现的相似。因此,许多研究者得出结论,视觉半球的差异主要反映了两个大脑半球之间的功能差异。然而,另一种解释认为,视野效应取决于其他因素,如注视不足、呈现时的眼球运动、暴露后扫描和注意偏差。这些因素对视野差异的潜在影响进行了回顾和讨论。有证据表明,注意力和半球功能差异相互作用,产生视野差异的大小和方向。
{"title":"Attentional factors in visual field asymmetries.","authors":"M P Bryden,&nbsp;T A Mondor","doi":"10.1037/h0084305","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084305","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Over the past 30 years, numerous studies have reported left/right asymmetries in visual field performance, with performance generally superior in the right visual field for verbal tasks and in the left visual field for spatial tasks. These asymmetries parallel those found in neurological studies of hemispheric specialization. Consequently, many investigators have concluded that visual hemifield differences are primarily a reflection of the functional differences between the two cerebral hemispheres. However, alternative explanations proposing that visual field effects are dependent on other factors such as inadequate fixation, eye movements during presentation, postexposural scanning, and attentional biases have been offered. The potential impact of each of these factors on visual field differences are reviewed and discussed. Evidence is provided suggesting that attention and hemispheric functional differences interact to produce the magnitude and direction of visual field differences.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 4","pages":"427-47"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084305","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"12941523","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 52
Sex differences in human brain size and the general meaning of differences in brain size. 人类大脑大小的性别差异和大脑大小差异的一般含义。
Pub Date : 1991-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/h0084307
M Peters

Contrary to commonly held convictions, there is no clear association between brain size and body parameters in humans. Within sexes, once age and health status are controlled for, there is no significant association between brain size and body height for females. For males, body height accounts for no more than .04% of the variance in brain size. The relation between brain weight and body weight is even less clearly defined. Nevertheless, there are large and significant differences in brain size between the sexes. If no adequate body parameters can be found that scale to brain size within the sexes, the marked dimorphism between males and females makes it even more difficult to find a common set of parameters that allow evaluation of brain size differences between sexes. Within and across sexes, there is no convincing link between a limited measure of behavioural capacity (IQ) and brain size. This leads to the more general question: Why would one expect such a link, and, if it is not found, what does this mean in the context of general theories of cortical function?

与人们普遍持有的观点相反,人类的大脑大小和身体参数之间没有明确的联系。在性别范围内,一旦控制了年龄和健康状况,女性的脑容量和身高之间就没有显著的关联。对于男性来说,身高对大脑大小差异的影响不超过0.04%。大脑重量和体重之间的关系就更不清楚了。然而,两性之间的大脑大小存在着巨大而显著的差异。如果找不到足够的身体参数来衡量两性之间的大脑大小,那么男性和女性之间明显的二态性就更难找到一套共同的参数来评估两性之间的大脑大小差异。在性别内部和性别之间,有限的行为能力(智商)和大脑大小之间没有令人信服的联系。这就引出了一个更普遍的问题:为什么人们会期待这样的联系,如果没有发现,这在皮层功能的一般理论背景下意味着什么?
{"title":"Sex differences in human brain size and the general meaning of differences in brain size.","authors":"M Peters","doi":"10.1037/h0084307","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084307","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Contrary to commonly held convictions, there is no clear association between brain size and body parameters in humans. Within sexes, once age and health status are controlled for, there is no significant association between brain size and body height for females. For males, body height accounts for no more than .04% of the variance in brain size. The relation between brain weight and body weight is even less clearly defined. Nevertheless, there are large and significant differences in brain size between the sexes. If no adequate body parameters can be found that scale to brain size within the sexes, the marked dimorphism between males and females makes it even more difficult to find a common set of parameters that allow evaluation of brain size differences between sexes. Within and across sexes, there is no convincing link between a limited measure of behavioural capacity (IQ) and brain size. This leads to the more general question: Why would one expect such a link, and, if it is not found, what does this mean in the context of general theories of cortical function?</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 4","pages":"507-22"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084307","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"12941525","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 66
Occupation and handedness: an examination of architects and mail survey biases. 职业和惯用手:对建筑师和邮件调查偏见的考察。
Pub Date : 1991-09-01 DOI: 10.1037/h0084292
C J Wood, J P Aggleton

Handedness among architects was determined by mail survey. Contrary to previous reports, no evidence was found of an excess of left-handers among a sample of 236 fully qualified male architects and 78 male architectural students. A second study examined whether the use of mail surveys systematically biases the returns of handedness questionnaires. For this, questionnaires were sent to 1,017 university students. No evidence was found for a bias amongst those who did and did not reply to the initial questionnaire. These results strengthen the findings of the first study.

建筑师的惯用手是通过邮件调查确定的。与之前的报告相反,在236名完全合格的男性建筑师和78名男性建筑专业学生的样本中,没有发现左撇子过多的证据。第二项研究考察了邮件调查是否会系统性地影响惯用手问卷的结果。为此,研究人员向1017名大学生发放了调查问卷。没有证据表明在回答和没有回答最初问卷的人之间存在偏见。这些结果加强了第一项研究的发现。
{"title":"Occupation and handedness: an examination of architects and mail survey biases.","authors":"C J Wood,&nbsp;J P Aggleton","doi":"10.1037/h0084292","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084292","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Handedness among architects was determined by mail survey. Contrary to previous reports, no evidence was found of an excess of left-handers among a sample of 236 fully qualified male architects and 78 male architectural students. A second study examined whether the use of mail surveys systematically biases the returns of handedness questionnaires. For this, questionnaires were sent to 1,017 university students. No evidence was found for a bias amongst those who did and did not reply to the initial questionnaire. These results strengthen the findings of the first study.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 3","pages":"395-404"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084292","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13093358","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14
Concurrent task interference in stutterers: dissociating hemispheric specialization and activation. 口吃者并发任务干扰:分离半球特化和激活。
Pub Date : 1991-09-01 DOI: 10.1037/h0084300
D C Forster, W G Webster

Using all four combinations of hand and foot, adult stutterers and nonstutterers performed a unimanual sequential finger tapping task together with a stimulus-contingent foot responding task. The two groups were similar in that both demonstrated more dual-task interference when using the ipsilateral than contralateral limb pairs. Contrary to a prediction based on a hypothesis that attributes stuttering to neural interference due to an ungated or unregulated activity flow between the hemispheres, stutterers did not show more interference than nonstutterers when using the contralateral limb pairs. The groups did differ, however, in terms of finger tapping interference associated with right- versus left-foot concurrent task responding. Nonstutterers showed more interference when using the left than right foot. This was interpreted in terms of the attentional demands that the foot responding task placed on a system with an inherent left-hemisphere attentional bias. The lack of a reliable difference in stutterers with respect to interference by the two feet is consistent with indications from other research that stutterers have a relatively labile system of hemispheric attention or activation. The results of the study are placed into the context of a general model of the brain mechanisms associated with stuttering.

使用所有四种手和脚的组合,成年口吃者和非口吃者一起执行一个单手顺序手指敲击任务和一个刺激-偶然的足反应任务。两组的相似之处在于,当使用同侧肢体时,两组都表现出比对侧肢体更多的双任务干扰。与基于假设的预测相反,当使用对侧肢体对时,口吃者并不比非口吃者表现出更多的干扰,该假设将口吃归因于两脑半球之间不受限制或不受调节的活动流造成的神经干扰。然而,在手指敲击与右脚和左脚并发任务反应相关的干扰方面,两组确实有所不同。非口吃者在使用左脚时比使用右脚时表现出更多的干扰。这被解释为足部反应任务对具有固有左半球注意偏向的系统的注意要求。口吃者在两脚干扰方面缺乏可靠的差异,这与其他研究的迹象一致,即口吃者有一个相对不稳定的半球注意或激活系统。这项研究的结果被置于与口吃有关的大脑机制的一般模型的背景下。
{"title":"Concurrent task interference in stutterers: dissociating hemispheric specialization and activation.","authors":"D C Forster,&nbsp;W G Webster","doi":"10.1037/h0084300","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084300","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Using all four combinations of hand and foot, adult stutterers and nonstutterers performed a unimanual sequential finger tapping task together with a stimulus-contingent foot responding task. The two groups were similar in that both demonstrated more dual-task interference when using the ipsilateral than contralateral limb pairs. Contrary to a prediction based on a hypothesis that attributes stuttering to neural interference due to an ungated or unregulated activity flow between the hemispheres, stutterers did not show more interference than nonstutterers when using the contralateral limb pairs. The groups did differ, however, in terms of finger tapping interference associated with right- versus left-foot concurrent task responding. Nonstutterers showed more interference when using the left than right foot. This was interpreted in terms of the attentional demands that the foot responding task placed on a system with an inherent left-hemisphere attentional bias. The lack of a reliable difference in stutterers with respect to interference by the two feet is consistent with indications from other research that stutterers have a relatively labile system of hemispheric attention or activation. The results of the study are placed into the context of a general model of the brain mechanisms associated with stuttering.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 3","pages":"321-35"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084300","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13092709","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 15
Sex, handedness, mathematical ability, and biological causation. 性别,利手性,数学能力和生物学原因。
Pub Date : 1991-09-01 DOI: 10.1037/h0084296
M Peters

An important part of Benbow's (1988) assertion that sex differences in mathematical ability are primarily due to biological factors is the link between a trait that is assumed to reflect differences in brain organization (left-handedness) and mathematical giftedness. It is shown that the link between mathematical giftedness and an increased prevalence of left-handedness is not convincing. However, Benbow's (1986) data do show a convincing link between strong right-handedness and the lack of mathematical giftedness, in agreement with Annett and Manning's (1990a, 1990b) recent work.

Benbow(1988)断言数学能力的性别差异主要是由于生物因素,其中一个重要部分是反映大脑组织差异的特征(左撇子)与数学天赋之间的联系。研究表明,数学天赋和左撇子增加之间的联系并不令人信服。然而,Benbow(1986)的数据确实显示了强右撇子和缺乏数学天赋之间的令人信服的联系,这与Annett和Manning (1990a, 1990b)最近的研究结果一致。
{"title":"Sex, handedness, mathematical ability, and biological causation.","authors":"M Peters","doi":"10.1037/h0084296","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084296","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An important part of Benbow's (1988) assertion that sex differences in mathematical ability are primarily due to biological factors is the link between a trait that is assumed to reflect differences in brain organization (left-handedness) and mathematical giftedness. It is shown that the link between mathematical giftedness and an increased prevalence of left-handedness is not convincing. However, Benbow's (1986) data do show a convincing link between strong right-handedness and the lack of mathematical giftedness, in agreement with Annett and Manning's (1990a, 1990b) recent work.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 3","pages":"415-9"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084296","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13093360","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 21
Learning the invariants of a perceptual motor skill. 学习知觉运动技能的不变量。
Pub Date : 1991-09-01 DOI: 10.1037/h0084294
I M Franks, M L Stanley

The concept of invariant relative timing has typically been associated with the concept of a generalized motor programme. The present study approaches the phenomenon of invariant relative timing from the perspective of learning. The underlying question of concern for this study was, "What is learned." The specific question was whether relative timing is one of the essential properties of movement that is learned during skill acquisition. In the present experiment, subjects were given extensive practice in learning to track and reproduce a criterion waveform using a joystick control for their response. In order to test whether subjects learn the relative timing of a movement, they were transferred to tracking waveforms that were identical to the criterion in terms of relative timing, but different in terms of absolute timing. Measurements were taken on all waveforms in two conditions: (a) in a pursuit tracking condition where subjects were temporally constrained by the stimulus, and (b) in a reproduction condition where subjects' timing was not constrained. The outcome from both conditions gives support to the idea that humans learn invariant relative timing during the acquisition of a motor skill.

恒定相对定时的概念通常与广义运动程序的概念联系在一起。本研究从学习的角度探讨相对时间不变现象。这项研究关注的潜在问题是,“学到了什么?”具体的问题是相对时间是否是在技能习得过程中习得的运动的基本属性之一。在本实验中,受试者被给予广泛的练习,学习使用操纵杆控制他们的反应来跟踪和重现标准波形。为了测试被试是否学会了一个动作的相对时间,他们被转移到在相对时间方面与标准相同但在绝对时间方面不同的跟踪波形中。在两种情况下对所有波形进行测量:(a)在受试者受到刺激的时间限制的追踪条件下,以及(b)在受试者的时间不受限制的再现条件下。这两种情况的结果支持了人类在获得运动技能的过程中学习不变的相对时间的观点。
{"title":"Learning the invariants of a perceptual motor skill.","authors":"I M Franks,&nbsp;M L Stanley","doi":"10.1037/h0084294","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084294","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The concept of invariant relative timing has typically been associated with the concept of a generalized motor programme. The present study approaches the phenomenon of invariant relative timing from the perspective of learning. The underlying question of concern for this study was, \"What is learned.\" The specific question was whether relative timing is one of the essential properties of movement that is learned during skill acquisition. In the present experiment, subjects were given extensive practice in learning to track and reproduce a criterion waveform using a joystick control for their response. In order to test whether subjects learn the relative timing of a movement, they were transferred to tracking waveforms that were identical to the criterion in terms of relative timing, but different in terms of absolute timing. Measurements were taken on all waveforms in two conditions: (a) in a pursuit tracking condition where subjects were temporally constrained by the stimulus, and (b) in a reproduction condition where subjects' timing was not constrained. The outcome from both conditions gives support to the idea that humans learn invariant relative timing during the acquisition of a motor skill.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 3","pages":"303-20"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084294","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13092708","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
Binocular unmasking with vertical disparity. 用垂直视差双目揭开面具。
Pub Date : 1991-09-01 DOI: 10.1037/h0084293
G Moraglia, B Schneider

We recently found (Schneider, Moraglia, & Jepson, 1989) that the contrast threshold for the detection of a visual signal in a noisy background can be considerably lower when binocular cues are available then when monocular cues only are present. Here, we investigated the occurrence of binocular unmasking with vertical interocular disparities. Subjects reported about the presence of Gabor signals in fields of two-dimensional broadband Gaussian noise surrounded by a frame of uniform noise. They saw these stimuli through a stereoscope; in all cases, the right-eye noise field was vertically displaced relative to the left one in either an upward or a downward direction, by up to 67.6'. In one condition, the right-eye signal was displaced by an amount equal to that of the noise, so that no opportunities for binocular unmasking existed; in the other, it appeared in exactly corresponding locations in the two fields--here, binocular disparities could be used to unmask the signal. Enhanced signal detectability, by up to 12.7 dB, was observed in the latter case for both directions of displacement, but only for displacements of 13.52' and only when the signal's orientation was horizontal. We argue that these effects result from the summation of monocular inputs carried out by linear binocular mechanisms.

我们最近发现(Schneider, Moraglia, & Jepson, 1989),在噪声背景下,当双眼线索可用时,检测视觉信号的对比度阈值比单目线索存在时要低得多。在这里,我们研究了双眼揭膜与垂直眼间差异的发生。受试者报告了在均匀噪声框架包围的二维宽带高斯噪声场中Gabor信号的存在。他们通过立体镜看到这些刺激;在所有情况下,右眼噪声场相对于左眼在垂直方向上向上或向下移动,最多67.6'。在一种情况下,右眼信号的偏移量与噪声的偏移量相等,这样就没有机会发现双眼的伪装;在另一种情况下,它出现在两个视场中完全对应的位置——在这里,双眼差异可以用来揭开信号的面纱。在后一种情况下,对位移的两个方向都观察到增强的信号可探测性,最高可达12.7 dB,但仅适用于位移为13.52'且仅当信号方向为水平时。我们认为,这些影响是由线性双目机制进行的单眼输入的总和造成的。
{"title":"Binocular unmasking with vertical disparity.","authors":"G Moraglia,&nbsp;B Schneider","doi":"10.1037/h0084293","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084293","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We recently found (Schneider, Moraglia, & Jepson, 1989) that the contrast threshold for the detection of a visual signal in a noisy background can be considerably lower when binocular cues are available then when monocular cues only are present. Here, we investigated the occurrence of binocular unmasking with vertical interocular disparities. Subjects reported about the presence of Gabor signals in fields of two-dimensional broadband Gaussian noise surrounded by a frame of uniform noise. They saw these stimuli through a stereoscope; in all cases, the right-eye noise field was vertically displaced relative to the left one in either an upward or a downward direction, by up to 67.6'. In one condition, the right-eye signal was displaced by an amount equal to that of the noise, so that no opportunities for binocular unmasking existed; in the other, it appeared in exactly corresponding locations in the two fields--here, binocular disparities could be used to unmask the signal. Enhanced signal detectability, by up to 12.7 dB, was observed in the latter case for both directions of displacement, but only for displacements of 13.52' and only when the signal's orientation was horizontal. We argue that these effects result from the summation of monocular inputs carried out by linear binocular mechanisms.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 3","pages":"353-66"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084293","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13093357","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Neither pictures nor propositions: what can we learn from a mental image? 既不是图像也不是命题:我们能从心理图像中学到什么?
Pub Date : 1991-09-01 DOI: 10.1037/h0084297
D Reisberg, D Chambers

In an earlier paper, we reported that subjects have great difficulty in finding alternative construals of their own mental images. In the present paper, we examine how subjects can nonetheless learn from their mental images. We argue that mental images, like percepts, are meaningful depictions. As such, mental images do depict appearance but are also inherently understood in a certain way, and this understanding influences the phenomenal appearance of the represented form. This, in turn, governs what the form will be seen to resemble and what the form is likely to call from memory. We report four experiments in support of this view. In each experiment, subjects are briefly shown outline shapes and asked to form a mental image of each shape. Subjects are then asked what familiar form the imaged shape resembles. Subjects routinely find target shapes in their images when the target is compatible both with the imaged geometry and with how that geometry is organized and understood. When the sought-for target is compatible with image geometry but not with how the image is understood, subjects reliably fail to find the target shape in their images.

在早期的一篇论文中,我们报道了受试者很难找到他们自己的心理图像的替代解释。在本文中,我们研究了受试者如何从他们的心理图像中学习。我们认为,像感知一样,心理图像是有意义的描述。因此,心理图像确实描绘了表象,但也以某种方式被固有地理解,这种理解影响了被表现形式的现象性表象。反过来,这又决定了形体看起来像什么,以及形体可能从记忆中调用什么。我们报告了四个实验来支持这一观点。在每个实验中,研究人员都会向实验对象简要展示轮廓形状,并要求他们在脑海中形成每种形状的图像。然后,研究对象被问及图像形状与熟悉的形状相似。当目标与成像的几何形状以及几何形状的组织和理解方式一致时,受试者通常会在图像中找到目标形状。当寻找的目标与图像的几何形状一致,但与图像的理解方式不一致时,受试者往往无法在图像中找到目标形状。
{"title":"Neither pictures nor propositions: what can we learn from a mental image?","authors":"D Reisberg,&nbsp;D Chambers","doi":"10.1037/h0084297","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084297","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In an earlier paper, we reported that subjects have great difficulty in finding alternative construals of their own mental images. In the present paper, we examine how subjects can nonetheless learn from their mental images. We argue that mental images, like percepts, are meaningful depictions. As such, mental images do depict appearance but are also inherently understood in a certain way, and this understanding influences the phenomenal appearance of the represented form. This, in turn, governs what the form will be seen to resemble and what the form is likely to call from memory. We report four experiments in support of this view. In each experiment, subjects are briefly shown outline shapes and asked to form a mental image of each shape. Subjects are then asked what familiar form the imaged shape resembles. Subjects routinely find target shapes in their images when the target is compatible both with the imaged geometry and with how that geometry is organized and understood. When the sought-for target is compatible with image geometry but not with how the image is understood, subjects reliably fail to find the target shape in their images.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 3","pages":"336-52"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084297","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13092710","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 64
期刊
Canadian journal of psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1