{"title":"Field studies of sleep/wake patterns and performance: a laboratory experience.","authors":"R J Broughton","doi":"10.1037/h0084277","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084277","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 2","pages":"240-53"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084277","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13034252","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
M D Herman, S L Denlinger, R Patarca, L Katz, J A Hobson
The evolution of sleep patterns in developing kittens was studied using time-lapse video technology and direct observation. The duration, frequency, and onset of the behavioural states and interactions of the cats were analyzed and then organized into phases that represent major changes in developmental structure during the first 6 weeks of kitten life. We have demonstrated that the kittens began exhibiting adult bi-cyclic sleep patterns on approximately Day 30 of development. During the 10-day period that preceded this consolidation of sleep pattern, REM sleep decreased by half, with a reciprocal increase in NREM sleep. These changes were coincident with an increase in kitten patterned motor behaviour and an increase in stimulation of the kittens by the mother during her bi-cyclic active periods.
{"title":"Developmental phases of sleep and motor behaviour in a cat mother-infant system: a time-lapse video approach.","authors":"M D Herman, S L Denlinger, R Patarca, L Katz, J A Hobson","doi":"10.1037/h0084278","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084278","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The evolution of sleep patterns in developing kittens was studied using time-lapse video technology and direct observation. The duration, frequency, and onset of the behavioural states and interactions of the cats were analyzed and then organized into phases that represent major changes in developmental structure during the first 6 weeks of kitten life. We have demonstrated that the kittens began exhibiting adult bi-cyclic sleep patterns on approximately Day 30 of development. During the 10-day period that preceded this consolidation of sleep pattern, REM sleep decreased by half, with a reciprocal increase in NREM sleep. These changes were coincident with an increase in kitten patterned motor behaviour and an increase in stimulation of the kittens by the mother during her bi-cyclic active periods.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 2","pages":"101-14"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084278","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13035659","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
The purpose of this study was to test the potential relationship between REM sleep and information processing with inversion of the visual field. In the first experiment, four male subjects slept in the laboratory for two sessions of 6 consecutive nights: 2 adaptation nights, 2 nights of polysomnography, and 2 nights of dream collection. During the days preceding Nights 3, 4, 5, and 6 of each session, the subjects wore glasses which, during the second session, completely inverted (rotation of 180 degrees) their visual field. In a second experiment with four other male subjects, the order of conditions was reversed, and the experimental condition (visual inversion) was introduced a second time. When the data of the two experiments were combined, there was a significant (p less than .01) increase in the percentage of REM sleep from Nights 3 and 4 of the control condition to Nights 3 and 4 of the visual inversion condition, but there was no significant change in any of the other sleep stages. There was a significant decrease in horizontal (p less than .04) and vertical (p less than .005) REM density and in the density of vertical REM bursts (p less than .02). The increase in REM sleep supports the hypothesis that REM sleep contributes to information processing while the decrease in REM density suggests that this component of REM sleep may be involved in a homeostatic process of sensory input.
{"title":"[Paradoxical sleep and information processing: exploration by inversion of the visual field].","authors":"J. De Koninck, F. Prévost","doi":"10.1037/H0084290","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0084290","url":null,"abstract":"The purpose of this study was to test the potential relationship between REM sleep and information processing with inversion of the visual field. In the first experiment, four male subjects slept in the laboratory for two sessions of 6 consecutive nights: 2 adaptation nights, 2 nights of polysomnography, and 2 nights of dream collection. During the days preceding Nights 3, 4, 5, and 6 of each session, the subjects wore glasses which, during the second session, completely inverted (rotation of 180 degrees) their visual field. In a second experiment with four other male subjects, the order of conditions was reversed, and the experimental condition (visual inversion) was introduced a second time. When the data of the two experiments were combined, there was a significant (p less than .01) increase in the percentage of REM sleep from Nights 3 and 4 of the control condition to Nights 3 and 4 of the visual inversion condition, but there was no significant change in any of the other sleep stages. There was a significant decrease in horizontal (p less than .04) and vertical (p less than .005) REM density and in the density of vertical REM bursts (p less than .02). The increase in REM sleep supports the hypothesis that REM sleep contributes to information processing while the decrease in REM density suggests that this component of REM sleep may be involved in a homeostatic process of sensory input.","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"116 1","pages":"125-39"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89258423","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
During most of the first half of this century psychologists knew what they wanted to do but had no idea how to do it, and during the second half they have, for the most part, been so preoccupied with how to do it that they have forgotten what they wanted to do. When J. Olds and Milner (1954) announced that rats would stimulate themselves in the septal area, it appeared to open the door to understanding motivation and reinforcement in terms of the underlying physiology. In the ensuing 36 years some progress has been made in that direction, though far outstripped by the progress in methodology. In this review I trace the efforts that have been made to locate the structures involved in self-stimulation by lesions, drugs, determinations of their neurophysiological characteristics, and other more sophisticated methods. I then review experiments, none very recent, comparing brain-stimulation reward to natural rewards and finally indicate how the information so far collected may be incorporated into theories of learning and motivation.
{"title":"Brain-stimulation reward: a review.","authors":"P M Milner","doi":"10.1037/h0084275","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084275","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>During most of the first half of this century psychologists knew what they wanted to do but had no idea how to do it, and during the second half they have, for the most part, been so preoccupied with how to do it that they have forgotten what they wanted to do. When J. Olds and Milner (1954) announced that rats would stimulate themselves in the septal area, it appeared to open the door to understanding motivation and reinforcement in terms of the underlying physiology. In the ensuing 36 years some progress has been made in that direction, though far outstripped by the progress in methodology. In this review I trace the efforts that have been made to locate the structures involved in self-stimulation by lesions, drugs, determinations of their neurophysiological characteristics, and other more sophisticated methods. I then review experiments, none very recent, comparing brain-stimulation reward to natural rewards and finally indicate how the information so far collected may be incorporated into theories of learning and motivation.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 1","pages":"1-36"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084275","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13200039","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
This paper describes some aspects of the food caching behaviour of four captive coyotes. Detailed observations of the actions used by coyotes to cache food revealed them to be strikingly similar to those previously described for timber wolves. The similarities included the identity of the movements used, their temporal sequencing, and their susceptibility to interruption. This suggests that there exists a stereotypy across canids in the action sequences used in caching. Second, an examination of the distribution of cache sites revealed that each coyote scattered cache sites widely within a wooded region of their enclosure and preferentially in terrains close to exposed tree roots.
{"title":"Food caching in captive coyotes: stereotypy of action sequence and spatial distribution of cache sites.","authors":"D P Phillips, J Ryon, W Danilchuk, J C Fentress","doi":"10.1037/h0084274","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084274","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This paper describes some aspects of the food caching behaviour of four captive coyotes. Detailed observations of the actions used by coyotes to cache food revealed them to be strikingly similar to those previously described for timber wolves. The similarities included the identity of the movements used, their temporal sequencing, and their susceptibility to interruption. This suggests that there exists a stereotypy across canids in the action sequences used in caching. Second, an examination of the distribution of cache sites revealed that each coyote scattered cache sites widely within a wooded region of their enclosure and preferentially in terrains close to exposed tree roots.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 1","pages":"83-91"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084274","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13200043","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
This paper provides a critical evaluation of the idea that, relative to the identity of the parts (local aspect), the configuration of the parts (global aspect) is of special importance during perceptual analysis. The controversy surrounding the global precedence phenomenon (Navon, 1977) is reviewed. In particular, I evaluate and reject the hypothesis that global precedence is simply the product of the greater discriminability of the global aspect. Instead, I propose that one general rule characterizing preattentive perceptual processing is a broad categorization of global information. I present the empirical evidence supporting this model and suggest some direction for future studies.
{"title":"[Global predominance in object recognition: artefact or treatment rule?].","authors":"L. Paquet","doi":"10.1037/H0084380","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0084380","url":null,"abstract":"This paper provides a critical evaluation of the idea that, relative to the identity of the parts (local aspect), the configuration of the parts (global aspect) is of special importance during perceptual analysis. The controversy surrounding the global precedence phenomenon (Navon, 1977) is reviewed. In particular, I evaluate and reject the hypothesis that global precedence is simply the product of the greater discriminability of the global aspect. Instead, I propose that one general rule characterizing preattentive perceptual processing is a broad categorization of global information. I present the empirical evidence supporting this model and suggest some direction for future studies.","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"21 1","pages":"37-53"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83172067","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
This paper provides a critical evaluation of the idea that, relative to the identity of the parts (local aspect), the configuration of the parts (global aspect) is of special importance during perceptual analysis. The controversy surrounding the global precedence phenomenon (Navon, 1977) is reviewed. In particular, I evaluate and reject the hypothesis that global precedence is simply the product of the greater discriminability of the global aspect. Instead, I propose that one general rule characterizing preattentive perceptual processing is a broad categorization of global information. I present the empirical evidence supporting this model and suggest some direction for future studies.
{"title":"[Global predominance in object recognition: artefact or treatment rule?].","authors":"L Paquet","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This paper provides a critical evaluation of the idea that, relative to the identity of the parts (local aspect), the configuration of the parts (global aspect) is of special importance during perceptual analysis. The controversy surrounding the global precedence phenomenon (Navon, 1977) is reviewed. In particular, I evaluate and reject the hypothesis that global precedence is simply the product of the greater discriminability of the global aspect. Instead, I propose that one general rule characterizing preattentive perceptual processing is a broad categorization of global information. I present the empirical evidence supporting this model and suggest some direction for future studies.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 1","pages":"37-53"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13200040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Partial report performance is conceived of as consisting of two independent sources of information: visible persistence and schematic persistence. Following the work of Di Lollo and Dixon (1988), these two sources of information were separated experimentally by varying interstimulus interval and stimulus onset asynchrony independently. In the present experiment we asked whether visible persistence and schematic persistence would be affected by display luminance, stimulus meaningfulness, or probe duration. The results were fit accurately by an extension of the Di Lollo and Dixon independent-decay model in which display luminance affected visible persistence but stimulus meaningfulness and probe duration affected neither form of persistence directly.
{"title":"Effects of display luminance, stimulus meaningfulness, and probe duration on visible and schematic persistence.","authors":"P Dixon, V Di Lollo","doi":"10.1037/h0084271","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084271","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Partial report performance is conceived of as consisting of two independent sources of information: visible persistence and schematic persistence. Following the work of Di Lollo and Dixon (1988), these two sources of information were separated experimentally by varying interstimulus interval and stimulus onset asynchrony independently. In the present experiment we asked whether visible persistence and schematic persistence would be affected by display luminance, stimulus meaningfulness, or probe duration. The results were fit accurately by an extension of the Di Lollo and Dixon independent-decay model in which display luminance affected visible persistence but stimulus meaningfulness and probe duration affected neither form of persistence directly.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 1","pages":"54-74"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084271","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13200041","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
We compared the relative effectiveness of rotating or translating the head, either horizontally or vertically, on the perception of depth resulting from motion parallax. Using Rogers and Graham's (1979) paradigm, we yoked the movement of random dots on a screen to movements of the head, simulating a corrugated surface. In two experiments, subjects nulled the apparent depth or motion seen in the display. Horizontal head movements yielded the most precise depth judgements, irrespective of whether the head translated or rotated. Motion thresholds were higher than those for depth and were independent of direction of head movement. In a third experiment, suprathreshold stimuli that simulated differing amounts of depth were used, and the subjects' perception of depth was virtually the same for all types and directions of head movement. In our stimulus situation, rotating or translating the head either vertically or horizontally produced motion parallax cues for depth that were equally effective. Our results also showed that, within a range, retinal image motion from head movement is converted into a depth signal and that above that range location constancy breaks down and motion is seen.
{"title":"Motion parallax judgements of depth as a function of the direction and type of head movement.","authors":"M J Steinbach, H Ono, M E Wolf","doi":"10.1037/h0084272","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084272","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We compared the relative effectiveness of rotating or translating the head, either horizontally or vertically, on the perception of depth resulting from motion parallax. Using Rogers and Graham's (1979) paradigm, we yoked the movement of random dots on a screen to movements of the head, simulating a corrugated surface. In two experiments, subjects nulled the apparent depth or motion seen in the display. Horizontal head movements yielded the most precise depth judgements, irrespective of whether the head translated or rotated. Motion thresholds were higher than those for depth and were independent of direction of head movement. In a third experiment, suprathreshold stimuli that simulated differing amounts of depth were used, and the subjects' perception of depth was virtually the same for all types and directions of head movement. In our stimulus situation, rotating or translating the head either vertically or horizontally produced motion parallax cues for depth that were equally effective. Our results also showed that, within a range, retinal image motion from head movement is converted into a depth signal and that above that range location constancy breaks down and motion is seen.</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 1","pages":"92-8"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084272","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13199944","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
In Mewhort and Campbell's (1981) model of visual word recognition, a scan-parse mechanism transfer information from a character buffer to a short-term store and is largely responsible for the familiarity effects observed in tachistoscopic free recall. Although Mewhort and Campbell do not elaborate on the role of articulation in this task, they do suggest that the scan-parse mechanism acts prior to any involvement of articulatory processes (see Mewhort & Campbell, 1980). However, proponents of working memory (see Baddeley, 1986) are clear that articulation is necessary for the transfer of visual information to a phonological short-term store. In order to clarify the role of articulation in the parsing and retention of letter string, we asked subjects to undertake a tachistoscopic free-recall task, involving first- and fourth-order letter strings, with and without articulatory suppression and/or unattended speech. Unattended speech was observed to interfere with recall, but only when subjects were free to articulate the strings. This implies that subjects were retrieving information from the phonological store and that articulation is necessary for the registration of visual information in this store. The lack of interaction between order-of-approximation and suppression supports the view that the scan-parse mechanism acts independently of articulation. It appears, therefore, that letter strings are parsed at a stage preceding their transfer of the pharmacological store. Finally, the results indicate that subjects are able to draw directly on a representation of the parsed components of a letter string and may do so when phonological storage is difficult.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)
{"title":"The role of articulation in the parsing and retention of letter strings in tachistoscopic free recall.","authors":"H Whiteley, P Walker","doi":"10.1037/h0084273","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084273","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In Mewhort and Campbell's (1981) model of visual word recognition, a scan-parse mechanism transfer information from a character buffer to a short-term store and is largely responsible for the familiarity effects observed in tachistoscopic free recall. Although Mewhort and Campbell do not elaborate on the role of articulation in this task, they do suggest that the scan-parse mechanism acts prior to any involvement of articulatory processes (see Mewhort & Campbell, 1980). However, proponents of working memory (see Baddeley, 1986) are clear that articulation is necessary for the transfer of visual information to a phonological short-term store. In order to clarify the role of articulation in the parsing and retention of letter string, we asked subjects to undertake a tachistoscopic free-recall task, involving first- and fourth-order letter strings, with and without articulatory suppression and/or unattended speech. Unattended speech was observed to interfere with recall, but only when subjects were free to articulate the strings. This implies that subjects were retrieving information from the phonological store and that articulation is necessary for the registration of visual information in this store. The lack of interaction between order-of-approximation and suppression supports the view that the scan-parse mechanism acts independently of articulation. It appears, therefore, that letter strings are parsed at a stage preceding their transfer of the pharmacological store. Finally, the results indicate that subjects are able to draw directly on a representation of the parsed components of a letter string and may do so when phonological storage is difficult.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)</p>","PeriodicalId":75671,"journal":{"name":"Canadian journal of psychology","volume":"45 1","pages":"75-82"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1991-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/h0084273","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"13200042","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}