首页 > 最新文献

Journal of speech and hearing research最新文献

英文 中文
Learnability constraints on deaf learners' acquisition of English wh-questions. 聋人英语wh-疑问句习得的可学性制约因素
Pub Date : 1996-06-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3903.625
G P Berent

This article explores deaf college students' knowledge of English wh-question formation in the context of government-binding theory and an associated learnability theory. The parameters of universal grammar (UG) that are relevant to wh-question formation are identified, and predictions are made regarding the learning of the English values of these parameters in accordance with the subset principle, which, it has been proposed, guides the acquisition of UG parameter values that define languages ordered as proper subsets. The results of two learnability tasks revealed that, despite years of exposure to English language input, many deaf learners have not internalized the positive evidence required to set the marked values of the wh-question parameters. This finding provides strong empirical support for the subset principle. Theoretical and educational implications are discussed.

本文在政府约束理论和相关可学性理论的背景下,探讨了聋人大学生英语wh-疑问句形成的知识。识别与wh-question形成相关的通用语法(UG)参数,并根据子集原则对这些参数的英语值的学习进行预测,该原则已被提出,指导获取将语言定义为适当子集的UG参数值。两项可学习性任务的结果显示,尽管多年接触英语语言输入,但许多聋人学习者并没有内化设置wh-question参数标记值所需的积极证据。这一发现为子集原理提供了强有力的实证支持。讨论了理论和教育意义。
{"title":"Learnability constraints on deaf learners' acquisition of English wh-questions.","authors":"G P Berent","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3903.625","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3903.625","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This article explores deaf college students' knowledge of English wh-question formation in the context of government-binding theory and an associated learnability theory. The parameters of universal grammar (UG) that are relevant to wh-question formation are identified, and predictions are made regarding the learning of the English values of these parameters in accordance with the subset principle, which, it has been proposed, guides the acquisition of UG parameter values that define languages ordered as proper subsets. The results of two learnability tasks revealed that, despite years of exposure to English language input, many deaf learners have not internalized the positive evidence required to set the marked values of the wh-question parameters. This finding provides strong empirical support for the subset principle. Theoretical and educational implications are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 3","pages":"625-42"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3903.625","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19755153","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 30
From planning to articulation in speech production: what differentiates a person who stutters from a person who does not stutter? 从言语规划到发音:口吃者与非口吃者的区别是什么?
Pub Date : 1996-06-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3903.546
P H van Lieshout, W Hulstijn, H F Peters

The main purpose of the present study was to differentiate between people who stutter and control speakers regarding their ability to assemble motor plans and to prepare (and execute) muscle commands. Adult males who stutter, matched for age, gender, and educational level with a group of control speakers, were tested on naming words and symbols. In addition, their ability to encode and retrieve memory representations of combinations of a symbol and a word, was tested in a recognition task, using manual reaction times and sensitivity scores, as defined in signal detection theory, as performance measures. Group differences in muscle command preparation were assessed from electromyographic recordings of upper lip and lower lip. Results indicated no interaction between group and word size effects in choice reaction times or a group effect in the ability to recognize previously learned symbol-word combinations. However, they were significantly different in the timing of peak amplitudes in the integrated electromyographic signals of upper lip and lower lip (IEMG peak latency). Findings question the claim that people who stutter have problems in creating abstract motor plans for speech. In addition, it is argued that the group differences in IEMG peak latency that were found in the present study might be better understood in terms of motor control strategies than in terms of motor control deficits.

本研究的主要目的是区分口吃者和控制说话者在装配运动计划和准备(并执行)肌肉指令方面的能力。口吃的成年男性在年龄、性别和教育程度上与一组对照说话者相匹配,他们接受了命名单词和符号的测试。此外,他们编码和检索符号和单词组合的记忆表征的能力在一个识别任务中进行了测试,使用手动反应时间和灵敏度分数,如信号检测理论所定义的,作为性能衡量标准。通过上唇和下唇的肌电图记录来评估肌肉指令准备的组间差异。结果表明,在选择反应时间方面,组效应和单词大小效应之间没有相互作用,在识别先前学习过的符号-单词组合的能力方面,组效应也没有相互作用。然而,他们在上唇和下唇综合肌电信号(IEMG峰潜伏期)的峰值振幅时间上存在显著差异。研究结果质疑了口吃者在为语言制定抽象运动计划方面存在问题的说法。此外,我们认为本研究中发现的IEMG峰潜伏期的组间差异可能更好地理解为运动控制策略,而不是运动控制缺陷。
{"title":"From planning to articulation in speech production: what differentiates a person who stutters from a person who does not stutter?","authors":"P H van Lieshout,&nbsp;W Hulstijn,&nbsp;H F Peters","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3903.546","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3903.546","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The main purpose of the present study was to differentiate between people who stutter and control speakers regarding their ability to assemble motor plans and to prepare (and execute) muscle commands. Adult males who stutter, matched for age, gender, and educational level with a group of control speakers, were tested on naming words and symbols. In addition, their ability to encode and retrieve memory representations of combinations of a symbol and a word, was tested in a recognition task, using manual reaction times and sensitivity scores, as defined in signal detection theory, as performance measures. Group differences in muscle command preparation were assessed from electromyographic recordings of upper lip and lower lip. Results indicated no interaction between group and word size effects in choice reaction times or a group effect in the ability to recognize previously learned symbol-word combinations. However, they were significantly different in the timing of peak amplitudes in the integrated electromyographic signals of upper lip and lower lip (IEMG peak latency). Findings question the claim that people who stutter have problems in creating abstract motor plans for speech. In addition, it is argued that the group differences in IEMG peak latency that were found in the present study might be better understood in terms of motor control strategies than in terms of motor control deficits.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 3","pages":"546-64"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3903.546","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19755246","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 68
Classification of adults for family studies of developmental language disorders. 发育性语言障碍家庭研究的成人分类。
Pub Date : 1996-06-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3903.661
E Plante, K Shenkman, M M Clark

A variety of approaches has been used to classify the status of adult subjects in familial studies of developmental language disorders. In this report, we directly compare the results of four different methods that appear in the research literature. Two of the approaches rely on case history reports, and two are performance-based methods. Subjects included 24 parents (12 mothers, 12 fathers) of children with developmental language disorders and 24 unrelated adult control subjects (12 female, 12 male) who completed case history items and standardized language testing designed for classification purposes. All classification methods identified more parents than control subjects as "affected". However, classification by case history methods resulted in fewer affected adults than classification through standardized testing. This outcome suggests that the variability in classification rates in studies to date may be the result of method rather than subject sample differences.

在发育性语言障碍的家族性研究中,有多种方法被用来对成人受试者的状态进行分类。在本报告中,我们直接比较了研究文献中出现的四种不同方法的结果。其中两种方法依赖于病例历史报告,另外两种是基于绩效的方法。研究对象包括24名发育性语言障碍儿童的父母(12名母亲,12名父亲)和24名不相关的成人对照(12名女性,12名男性),他们完成了病史项目和标准化语言测试设计的分类目的。所有的分类方法都确定了比对照组更多的家长受到“影响”。然而,通过病例史方法进行分类的结果比通过标准化测试进行分类的结果更少。这一结果表明,迄今为止研究中分类率的差异可能是方法而不是受试者样本差异的结果。
{"title":"Classification of adults for family studies of developmental language disorders.","authors":"E Plante,&nbsp;K Shenkman,&nbsp;M M Clark","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3903.661","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3903.661","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A variety of approaches has been used to classify the status of adult subjects in familial studies of developmental language disorders. In this report, we directly compare the results of four different methods that appear in the research literature. Two of the approaches rely on case history reports, and two are performance-based methods. Subjects included 24 parents (12 mothers, 12 fathers) of children with developmental language disorders and 24 unrelated adult control subjects (12 female, 12 male) who completed case history items and standardized language testing designed for classification purposes. All classification methods identified more parents than control subjects as \"affected\". However, classification by case history methods resulted in fewer affected adults than classification through standardized testing. This outcome suggests that the variability in classification rates in studies to date may be the result of method rather than subject sample differences.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 3","pages":"661-7"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19754428","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 38
Voice onset time in speech produced during simultaneous communication. 在同步通信中产生的语音的开始时间。
Pub Date : 1996-06-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3903.565
N Schiavetti, R L Whitehead, D E Metz, B Whitehead, M Mignerey

This study investigated speaking rate and voice onset time (VOT) in speech produced during simultaneous communication (SC) by speakers with normal hearing. Stimulus words initiated with voiced and voiceless plosives were embedded in a sentence that was spoken and produced with SC. VOT measures were calculated from acoustic recordings and results indicated significant differences between speech-only and SC conditions, with speech produced during SC demonstrating both slower speaking rate and increased VOT of voiceless consonants. VOTs produced during both SC and speech-only conditions followed English voicing rules and varied appropriately with place of articulation. The somewhat enlarged voicing contrast during SC was consistent with previous findings regarding the influence of rate changes on the temporal fine structure of speech (Miller, 1987) and was similar to the voicing contrast results reported for clear speech by Picheny, Durlach, and Braida (1986).

本研究对听力正常的说话人在同步交流中产生的言语的语速和语音起始时间进行了研究。用发声爆破和不发声爆破启动的刺激词被嵌入在用SC说话和产生的句子中。VOT测量从录音中计算出来,结果表明语音和SC条件之间存在显著差异,在SC过程中产生的语音显示出说话速度变慢和不发声辅音的VOT增加。在SC和纯语音条件下产生的vot都遵循英语发音规则,并随着发音位置的不同而适当变化。SC过程中略微放大的发声对比与先前关于语率变化对言语时间精细结构影响的研究结果一致(Miller, 1987),与Picheny, Durlach, and Braida(1986)报道的清晰言语的发声对比结果相似。
{"title":"Voice onset time in speech produced during simultaneous communication.","authors":"N Schiavetti,&nbsp;R L Whitehead,&nbsp;D E Metz,&nbsp;B Whitehead,&nbsp;M Mignerey","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3903.565","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3903.565","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study investigated speaking rate and voice onset time (VOT) in speech produced during simultaneous communication (SC) by speakers with normal hearing. Stimulus words initiated with voiced and voiceless plosives were embedded in a sentence that was spoken and produced with SC. VOT measures were calculated from acoustic recordings and results indicated significant differences between speech-only and SC conditions, with speech produced during SC demonstrating both slower speaking rate and increased VOT of voiceless consonants. VOTs produced during both SC and speech-only conditions followed English voicing rules and varied appropriately with place of articulation. The somewhat enlarged voicing contrast during SC was consistent with previous findings regarding the influence of rate changes on the temporal fine structure of speech (Miller, 1987) and was similar to the voicing contrast results reported for clear speech by Picheny, Durlach, and Braida (1986).</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 3","pages":"565-72"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3903.565","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19755147","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 30
Acoustic correlates of breathy vocal quality: dysphonic voices and continuous speech. 呼吸性音质的声学相关性:不发音的声音和连续的讲话。
Pub Date : 1996-04-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3902.311
J Hillenbrand, R A Houde

In an earlier study, we evaluated the effectiveness of several acoustic measures in predicting breathiness ratings for sustained vowels spoken by nonpathological talkers who were asked to produce nonbreathy, moderately breathy, and very breathy phonation (Hillenbrand, Cleveland, & Erickson, 1994). The purpose of the present study was to extend these results to speakers with laryngeal pathologies and to conduct tests using connected speech in addition to sustained vowels. Breathiness ratings were obtained from a sustained vowel and a 12-word sentence spoken by 20 pathological and 5 nonpathological talkers. Acoustic measures were made of (a) signal periodicity, (b) first harmonic amplitude, and (c) spectral tilt. For the sustained vowels, a frequency domain measure of periodicity provided the most accurate predictions of perceived breathiness, accounting for 92% of the variance in breathiness ratings. The relative amplitude of the first harmonic and two measures of spectral tilt correlated moderately with breathiness ratings. For the sentences, both signal periodicity and spectral tilt provided accurate predictions of breathiness ratings, accounting for 70%-85% of the variance.

在早期的一项研究中,我们评估了几种声学测量在预测非病态说话者所讲的持续元音呼吸评级方面的有效性,这些说话者被要求发出无呼吸、中度呼吸和非常呼吸的发音(Hillenbrand, Cleveland, & Erickson, 1994)。本研究的目的是将这些结果扩展到有喉部病变的说话者,并使用连音和持续元音进行测试。研究人员通过20名病理患者和5名非病理患者说出的一个持续的元音和一个12个单词的句子来获得呼吸评分。声学测量包括(a)信号周期,(b)一谐波幅度,(c)频谱倾斜。对于持续元音,周期性的频域测量提供了感知呼吸的最准确预测,占呼吸评级差异的92%。第一谐波的相对振幅和光谱倾斜的两个测量值与呼吸评级适度相关。对于句子,信号周期性和频谱倾斜都提供了呼吸评级的准确预测,占方差的70%-85%。
{"title":"Acoustic correlates of breathy vocal quality: dysphonic voices and continuous speech.","authors":"J Hillenbrand,&nbsp;R A Houde","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3902.311","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3902.311","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In an earlier study, we evaluated the effectiveness of several acoustic measures in predicting breathiness ratings for sustained vowels spoken by nonpathological talkers who were asked to produce nonbreathy, moderately breathy, and very breathy phonation (Hillenbrand, Cleveland, & Erickson, 1994). The purpose of the present study was to extend these results to speakers with laryngeal pathologies and to conduct tests using connected speech in addition to sustained vowels. Breathiness ratings were obtained from a sustained vowel and a 12-word sentence spoken by 20 pathological and 5 nonpathological talkers. Acoustic measures were made of (a) signal periodicity, (b) first harmonic amplitude, and (c) spectral tilt. For the sustained vowels, a frequency domain measure of periodicity provided the most accurate predictions of perceived breathiness, accounting for 92% of the variance in breathiness ratings. The relative amplitude of the first harmonic and two measures of spectral tilt correlated moderately with breathiness ratings. For the sentences, both signal periodicity and spectral tilt provided accurate predictions of breathiness ratings, accounting for 70%-85% of the variance.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 2","pages":"311-21"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3902.311","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19703749","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 503
Noise exposure associated with hearing aid use in industry. 工业中助听器使用的噪音暴露。
Pub Date : 1996-04-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3902.251
T G Dolan, J F Maurer

Although noise may be innocuous in many vocational environments, there is a growing concern in industry that it can reach hazardous levels when amplified by hearing aids. This study examined the daily noise exposures associated with hearing aid use in industry. This was done by both laboratory and site measurements in which hearing aids were coupled to the microphone of an integrating sound level meter or dosimeter. The former method involved the use of recorded railroad and manufacturing noise and a Bruel and Kjaer 4128 Head and Torso simulator. In the latter procedure, a worker wore one of three hearing aids coupled to a dosimeter during 8-hour shifts in a manufacturing plant. Both methods demonstrated that even when amplified by mild-gain hearing aids, noise exposures rose from time-weighted averages near 80 dBA to well above the OSHA maximum of 90 dBA. The OSHA maximum was also exceeded when moderate and high gain instruments were worn in non-occupational listening environments. The results suggest that current OSHA regulations that limit noise exposure in sound field are inappropriate for hearing aid users.

虽然噪音在许多职业环境中可能是无害的,但在工业中,越来越多的人担心,如果通过助听器放大,噪音可能会达到危险的程度。本研究调查了与工业助听器使用相关的日常噪音暴露。这是通过实验室和现场测量完成的,其中助听器与集成声级计或剂量计的麦克风耦合。前一种方法涉及使用记录铁路和制造噪声和Bruel和Kjaer 4128头部和躯干模拟器。在后一种程序中,一名工人在一家制造工厂的8小时轮班期间戴着三种助听器中的一种与剂量计相连。两种方法都表明,即使使用轻度增益助听器放大,噪声暴露也从近80 dBA的时间加权平均值上升到远高于OSHA最大值90 dBA。在非职业听力环境中佩戴中、高增益仪器也超过OSHA最大值。结果表明,目前的OSHA规定,限制噪声暴露在声场是不适合助听器使用者。
{"title":"Noise exposure associated with hearing aid use in industry.","authors":"T G Dolan,&nbsp;J F Maurer","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3902.251","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3902.251","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although noise may be innocuous in many vocational environments, there is a growing concern in industry that it can reach hazardous levels when amplified by hearing aids. This study examined the daily noise exposures associated with hearing aid use in industry. This was done by both laboratory and site measurements in which hearing aids were coupled to the microphone of an integrating sound level meter or dosimeter. The former method involved the use of recorded railroad and manufacturing noise and a Bruel and Kjaer 4128 Head and Torso simulator. In the latter procedure, a worker wore one of three hearing aids coupled to a dosimeter during 8-hour shifts in a manufacturing plant. Both methods demonstrated that even when amplified by mild-gain hearing aids, noise exposures rose from time-weighted averages near 80 dBA to well above the OSHA maximum of 90 dBA. The OSHA maximum was also exceeded when moderate and high gain instruments were worn in non-occupational listening environments. The results suggest that current OSHA regulations that limit noise exposure in sound field are inappropriate for hearing aid users.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 2","pages":"251-60"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3902.251","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19702548","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 24
Activity of intrinsic laryngeal muscles in fluent and disfluent speech. 流利和不流利言语中喉内肌的活动。
Pub Date : 1996-04-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3902.329
A Smith, M Denny, L A Shaffer, E M Kelly, M Hirano

The goal of the present experiment was to determine if stuttering is associated with unusually high levels of activity in laryngeal muscles. Qualitative and quantitative analyses of thyroarytenoid and cricothyroid recordings from 4 stuttering and 3 nonstuttering adults revealed the following: Compared to periods of fluent speech, intervals of disfluent speech are not typically characterized by higher levels of activity in these muscles; and when EMG levels during conversational speech are compared to maximal activation levels for these muscles (e.g., those observed during singing and the Valsalva maneuver), normally fluent adults show robust and sometimes near maximal recruitment during conversational speech. The adults who stutter had a lower operating range for these muscles during conversational speech, and their disfluencies did not produce relatively high activation levels. In summary, the present data require us to reject the claim that adults with a history of chronic stuttering routinely produce excessive levels of intrinsic laryngeal muscle activity. These results suggest that the use of botulinum toxin injections into the vocal folds to treat stuttering should be questioned.

本实验的目的是确定口吃是否与喉部肌肉异常高水平的活动有关。对4名口吃和3名非口吃成人的类甲状腺肌和环甲肌的定性和定量分析显示:与语言流利期相比,语言不流利期这些肌肉的活动水平并不高;当将会话演讲期间的肌电图水平与这些肌肉的最大激活水平(例如,唱歌和Valsalva动作时观察到的肌肉)进行比较时,通常流利的成年人在会话演讲期间表现出强劲的,有时接近最大的恢复。口吃的成年人在谈话时,这些肌肉的活动范围较低,他们的不流利并没有产生相对较高的激活水平。总之,目前的数据要求我们拒绝有慢性口吃史的成年人通常会产生过高水平的喉部固有肌活动的说法。这些结果表明,使用肉毒杆菌毒素注射声带治疗口吃应该受到质疑。
{"title":"Activity of intrinsic laryngeal muscles in fluent and disfluent speech.","authors":"A Smith,&nbsp;M Denny,&nbsp;L A Shaffer,&nbsp;E M Kelly,&nbsp;M Hirano","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3902.329","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3902.329","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The goal of the present experiment was to determine if stuttering is associated with unusually high levels of activity in laryngeal muscles. Qualitative and quantitative analyses of thyroarytenoid and cricothyroid recordings from 4 stuttering and 3 nonstuttering adults revealed the following: Compared to periods of fluent speech, intervals of disfluent speech are not typically characterized by higher levels of activity in these muscles; and when EMG levels during conversational speech are compared to maximal activation levels for these muscles (e.g., those observed during singing and the Valsalva maneuver), normally fluent adults show robust and sometimes near maximal recruitment during conversational speech. The adults who stutter had a lower operating range for these muscles during conversational speech, and their disfluencies did not produce relatively high activation levels. In summary, the present data require us to reject the claim that adults with a history of chronic stuttering routinely produce excessive levels of intrinsic laryngeal muscle activity. These results suggest that the use of botulinum toxin injections into the vocal folds to treat stuttering should be questioned.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 2","pages":"329-48"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3902.329","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19703031","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 38
A within-subject comparison of adult patients using the Nucleus F0F1F2 and F0F1F2B3B4B5 speech processing strategies. 成年患者使用F0F1F2核和F0F1F2B3B4B5语音处理策略的受试者内比较。
Pub Date : 1996-04-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3902.261
A J Parkinson, R S Tyler, G G Woodworth, M W Lowder, B J Gantz

This study compares the Nucleus F0F1F2 and F0F1F2B3B4B5 (also known as "Multipeak") of "Mpeak") processing schemes in 17 patients wearing the Mini Speech Processor. All patients had at least 18 months implant experience using the F0F1F2 processing strategy. For this study, they were switched to the F0F1F2B3B4B5 processing strategy for 3 months. They then returned to using the F0F1F2 strategy for 3 months, then used the F0F1F2B3B4B5 strategy again for 3 months, and lastly used the F0F1F2 strategy for 3 months. Performance' was evaluated with both schemes after each interval, using speech recognition tests and subjective ratings. Overall, differences between the results for the two processing schemes were not large. Average performance was somewhat better for the F0F1F2B3B4B5 strategy for word and sentence identification, but not for any of the other speech measures. Superior performance was observed in 8 patients with the F0F1F2B3B4B5 strategy. However, 6 of the 8 individuals were significantly better on only one of the six speech measures in the test battery. The other 2 patients performed better on two of the speech measures. Superior performance was also observed in 3 patients with F0F1F2 strategy for consonant recognition. For the remaining patients, there was little difference in their performance with the two strategies. Information transmission analyses indicated that the F0F1F2B3B4B5 strategy transmitted consonant duration and frication cues more efficiently than F0F1F2. Experience with one strategy appeared to benefit performance with the other strategy.

本研究比较了17例佩戴Mini语音处理器患者的F0F1F2和F0F1F2B3B4B5(也称为Mpeak的“Multipeak”)处理方案。所有患者使用F0F1F2处理策略至少有18个月的种植经验。在这项研究中,他们被切换到F0F1F2B3B4B5处理策略3个月。然后他们再次使用F0F1F2策略3个月,然后再次使用F0F1F2B3B4B5策略3个月,最后使用F0F1F2策略3个月。在每个间隔后,使用语音识别测试和主观评分对两种方案的性能进行评估。总体而言,两种处理方案的结果差异不大。在单词和句子识别方面,F0F1F2B3B4B5策略的平均表现稍好一些,但在其他任何语音测量方面都没有。采用F0F1F2B3B4B5策略治疗的8例患者疗效显著。然而,8个人中有6个人在测试组的6项言语测试中只有一项明显更好。另外两名患者在两项言语测试中表现更好。采用F0F1F2策略的3例患者在辅音识别方面也有较好的表现。对于其余的患者,两种策略在他们的表现上几乎没有区别。信息传递分析表明,F0F1F2B3B4B5策略比F0F1F2策略更有效地传递辅音时长和摩擦线索。使用一种策略的经验似乎有利于使用另一种策略的表现。
{"title":"A within-subject comparison of adult patients using the Nucleus F0F1F2 and F0F1F2B3B4B5 speech processing strategies.","authors":"A J Parkinson,&nbsp;R S Tyler,&nbsp;G G Woodworth,&nbsp;M W Lowder,&nbsp;B J Gantz","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3902.261","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3902.261","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study compares the Nucleus F0F1F2 and F0F1F2B3B4B5 (also known as \"Multipeak\") of \"Mpeak\") processing schemes in 17 patients wearing the Mini Speech Processor. All patients had at least 18 months implant experience using the F0F1F2 processing strategy. For this study, they were switched to the F0F1F2B3B4B5 processing strategy for 3 months. They then returned to using the F0F1F2 strategy for 3 months, then used the F0F1F2B3B4B5 strategy again for 3 months, and lastly used the F0F1F2 strategy for 3 months. Performance' was evaluated with both schemes after each interval, using speech recognition tests and subjective ratings. Overall, differences between the results for the two processing schemes were not large. Average performance was somewhat better for the F0F1F2B3B4B5 strategy for word and sentence identification, but not for any of the other speech measures. Superior performance was observed in 8 patients with the F0F1F2B3B4B5 strategy. However, 6 of the 8 individuals were significantly better on only one of the six speech measures in the test battery. The other 2 patients performed better on two of the speech measures. Superior performance was also observed in 3 patients with F0F1F2 strategy for consonant recognition. For the remaining patients, there was little difference in their performance with the two strategies. Information transmission analyses indicated that the F0F1F2B3B4B5 strategy transmitted consonant duration and frication cues more efficiently than F0F1F2. Experience with one strategy appeared to benefit performance with the other strategy.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 2","pages":"261-77"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19703744","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Consonant-vowel interdependencies in babbling and early words: preliminary examination of a locus equation approach. 咿呀学语和早期词汇中的辅音-元音相互依赖关系:轨迹方程方法的初步检验。
Pub Date : 1996-04-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3902.424
H M Sussman, F D Minifie, E H Buder, C Stoel-Gammon, J Smith

Consonant-vowel productions at two distinct stages of language development were studied in a single female child. At 12 months canonical babbling syllables (N = 144) identified by a panel of listeners as comprising [bV], [dV], and [gv] tokens were acoustically analyzed by measuring F2 transition onset and F2 midvowel frequencies and plotting their relationship as locus equations for each stop category. A regression analysis performed on these scatterplots revealed differential slopes and y-intercepts as a function of stop place. The same analysis was performed 9 months later on CV utterances (N = 243) produced as syllable-initial segments of real words by the same child. Whereas labial and velar locus equation parameters moved toward more adult-like values, alveolar slope and y-intercept moved away from adult values and more in the direction of decreased coarticulation between vowel and consonant. There was greater scatter of data points around the regression line for production of words compared to babbling. These results are compared to locus equations obtained from 3-5-year-olds and adults. Locus equations appear to be useful as an empirical developmental probe to document how CV productions gradually approach adult categorical standards.

在语言发展的两个不同阶段的辅音-元音产生研究在一个单一的女孩。在12个月大的时候,一组听众确定了由[bV], [dV]和[gv]组成的典型咿呀学语音节(N = 144),通过测量F2过渡开始和F2中间元音频率,并绘制它们作为每个停止类别的轨迹方程的关系,对它们进行声学分析。对这些散点图进行的回归分析显示,不同的斜率和y截距是停止位置的函数。9个月后,对同一儿童的CV话语(N = 243)作为真实单词的音节首段进行了同样的分析。虽然唇形和舌形轨迹方程参数更接近成人值,但肺泡斜率和y轴截距远离成人值,并且更倾向于减少元音和辅音之间的协同发音。与咿呀学语相比,在单词产生的回归线周围有更大的数据点分散。这些结果与从3-5岁儿童和成人获得的轨迹方程进行了比较。基因座方程似乎是有用的,作为一个经验的发展探针,以记录CV产品如何逐渐接近成人的分类标准。
{"title":"Consonant-vowel interdependencies in babbling and early words: preliminary examination of a locus equation approach.","authors":"H M Sussman,&nbsp;F D Minifie,&nbsp;E H Buder,&nbsp;C Stoel-Gammon,&nbsp;J Smith","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3902.424","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3902.424","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Consonant-vowel productions at two distinct stages of language development were studied in a single female child. At 12 months canonical babbling syllables (N = 144) identified by a panel of listeners as comprising [bV], [dV], and [gv] tokens were acoustically analyzed by measuring F2 transition onset and F2 midvowel frequencies and plotting their relationship as locus equations for each stop category. A regression analysis performed on these scatterplots revealed differential slopes and y-intercepts as a function of stop place. The same analysis was performed 9 months later on CV utterances (N = 243) produced as syllable-initial segments of real words by the same child. Whereas labial and velar locus equation parameters moved toward more adult-like values, alveolar slope and y-intercept moved away from adult values and more in the direction of decreased coarticulation between vowel and consonant. There was greater scatter of data points around the regression line for production of words compared to babbling. These results are compared to locus equations obtained from 3-5-year-olds and adults. Locus equations appear to be useful as an empirical developmental probe to document how CV productions gradually approach adult categorical standards.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 2","pages":"424-33"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3902.424","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19702961","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 37
Spectral distribution of prosodic information. 韵律信息的频谱分布。
Pub Date : 1996-04-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3902.228
K W Grant, B E Walden

Prosodic speech cues for rhythm, stress, and intonation are related primarily to variations in intensity, duration, and fundamental frequency. Because these cues make use of temporal properties of the speech waveform they are likely to be represented broadly across the speech spectrum. In order to determine the relative importance of different frequency regions for the recognition of prosodic cues, identification of four prosodic features, syllable number, syllabic stress, sentence intonation, and phrase boundary location, was evaluated under six filter conditions spanning the range from 200-6100 Hz. Each filter condition had equal articulation index (AI) weights, AI = 0.01; p(C)isolated words approximately equal to 0.40. Results obtained with normally hearing subjects showed that there was an interaction between filter condition and the identification of specific prosodic features. For example, information from high-frequency regions of speech was particularly useful in the identification of syllable number and stress, whereas information from low-frequency regions was helpful in identifying intonation patterns. In spite of these spectral differences, overall listeners performed remarkably well in identifying prosodic patterns, although individual differences were apparent. For some subjects, equivalent levels of performance across the six filter conditions were achieved. These results are discussed in relation to auditory and auditory-visual speech recognition.

节奏、重音和语调的韵律语音线索主要与强度、持续时间和基本频率的变化有关。因为这些线索利用了语音波形的时间特性,它们很可能在整个语音频谱中得到广泛的表示。为了确定不同频率区域对韵律线索识别的相对重要性,在200-6100 Hz范围内的六种过滤条件下,评估了音节数、音节重音、句子语调和短语边界位置四种韵律特征的识别。各滤波条件的articulation index (AI)权值相等,AI = 0.01;p(C)孤立词约等于0.40。听力正常的被试的结果表明,过滤条件与特定韵律特征的识别之间存在交互作用。例如,来自高频区域的信息在识别音节数和重音方面特别有用,而来自低频区域的信息则有助于识别语调模式。尽管存在这些谱上的差异,但总体而言,听众在识别韵律模式方面表现得非常好,尽管个体差异很明显。对于一些受试者,在六个过滤条件下达到了相同的性能水平。这些结果在听觉和听觉-视觉语音识别方面进行了讨论。
{"title":"Spectral distribution of prosodic information.","authors":"K W Grant,&nbsp;B E Walden","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3902.228","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3902.228","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prosodic speech cues for rhythm, stress, and intonation are related primarily to variations in intensity, duration, and fundamental frequency. Because these cues make use of temporal properties of the speech waveform they are likely to be represented broadly across the speech spectrum. In order to determine the relative importance of different frequency regions for the recognition of prosodic cues, identification of four prosodic features, syllable number, syllabic stress, sentence intonation, and phrase boundary location, was evaluated under six filter conditions spanning the range from 200-6100 Hz. Each filter condition had equal articulation index (AI) weights, AI = 0.01; p(C)isolated words approximately equal to 0.40. Results obtained with normally hearing subjects showed that there was an interaction between filter condition and the identification of specific prosodic features. For example, information from high-frequency regions of speech was particularly useful in the identification of syllable number and stress, whereas information from low-frequency regions was helpful in identifying intonation patterns. In spite of these spectral differences, overall listeners performed remarkably well in identifying prosodic patterns, although individual differences were apparent. For some subjects, equivalent levels of performance across the six filter conditions were achieved. These results are discussed in relation to auditory and auditory-visual speech recognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 2","pages":"228-38"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3902.228","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19702544","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 56
期刊
Journal of speech and hearing research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1