Pub Date : 1984-08-01DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90038-2
Margaret A. Neale
Recent literature reports a reliance on third-party intervention procedures in contract negotiation. This study suggests that the relative evaluation of the costs related to arbitration (such as the uncertainty and expense of arbitration), and the costs related to negotiation (such as loss of face or perceived incompetency) provide a partial explanation for the increasing frequency of the negotiator's use of arbitration procedures rather than reaching a negotiated settlement. The relative salience of negotiation-related costs (high and low) and the relative salience of arbitration-related costs (high and low) were manipulated in a 2 × 2 design, involving 147 subjects negotiating a five-issue contract. The results were consistent with the hypothesized relationships. Implications of the results of this study for improving the understanding of and control over negotiator behavior are discussed.
{"title":"The effects of negotiation and arbitration cost salience on bargainer behavior: The role of the arbitrator and constituency on negotiator judgment","authors":"Margaret A. Neale","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90038-2","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90038-2","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Recent literature reports a reliance on third-party intervention procedures in contract negotiation. This study suggests that the relative evaluation of the costs related to arbitration (such as the uncertainty and expense of arbitration), and the costs related to negotiation (such as loss of face or perceived incompetency) provide a partial explanation for the increasing frequency of the negotiator's use of arbitration procedures rather than reaching a negotiated settlement. The relative salience of negotiation-related costs (high and low) and the relative salience of arbitration-related costs (high and low) were manipulated in a 2 × 2 design, involving 147 subjects negotiating a five-issue contract. The results were consistent with the hypothesized relationships. Implications of the results of this study for improving the understanding of and control over negotiator behavior are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"34 1","pages":"Pages 97-111"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90038-2","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53840307","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 1984-06-01DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90025-4
Anson Seers, George B. Graen
The job characteristics model has guided a great deal of research on the relationship between task characteristics and employee satisfaction and performance. Research in this area has indicated the necessity of incorporating nontask factors of jobs for further theory building in organizational behavior. Based upon the framework of organizational role theory, the dual attachment model is proposed for the integration of the job characteristic model and the leader-member exchange model. Questionnaire data from 101 employees in a federal agency is used to analyze the three models with respect to the prediction of job attitudes and performance. The results indicate the complementarity of task and interpersonal factors in the prediction of job outcomes. An additive model, as opposed to an interactive model, is supported for the combination of these factors. Findings from time lagged analysis of these models are generally consistent with those from concurrent analyses, but the findings from dynamic analysis are only partially consistent with those of the concurrent and time lagged analyses. Individual difference variables are found to be of little importance in these models.
{"title":"The dual attachment concept: A longitudinal investigation of the combination of task characteristics and leader—member exchange","authors":"Anson Seers, George B. Graen","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90025-4","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90025-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The job characteristics model has guided a great deal of research on the relationship between task characteristics and employee satisfaction and performance. Research in this area has indicated the necessity of incorporating nontask factors of jobs for further theory building in organizational behavior. Based upon the framework of organizational role theory, the dual attachment model is proposed for the integration of the job characteristic model and the leader-member exchange model. Questionnaire data from 101 employees in a federal agency is used to analyze the three models with respect to the prediction of job attitudes and performance. The results indicate the complementarity of task and interpersonal factors in the prediction of job outcomes. An additive model, as opposed to an interactive model, is supported for the combination of these factors. Findings from time lagged analysis of these models are generally consistent with those from concurrent analyses, but the findings from dynamic analysis are only partially consistent with those of the concurrent and time lagged analyses. Individual difference variables are found to be of little importance in these models.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 3","pages":"Pages 283-306"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90025-4","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53839466","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 1984-06-01DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90026-6
Patrick A. Knight
B. M. Staw and J. Ross (Journal of Applied Psychology, 1980, 65, 249–260) present data which suggest that one barrier to experimenting management is an implicit theory of heroic leadership favoring managers who are consistent in the face of failure, The currant research tests this theory of heroism against an implicit theory of competent management. Subjects read a case study describing a manager who was either experimenting or consistent, and either immediately successful or successful only after initial failure (i.e., ultimately successful). The results supported the implicit theory of competent management with the consistent managers, whose first policies were effective, rated significantly higher than the ultimately successful-experimenting manager, whose first two policies failed. The immediately successful-experimenting manager was unexpectedly rated significantly higher than all other managers. The results show that there is not a general bias against experimentation in favor of consistency, and that reactions to managers are based more upon evidence of competence than upon experimenting and consistency per se.
B. M. Staw和J. Ross (Journal of Applied Psychology, 1980, 65, 249-260)提供的数据表明,尝试管理的一个障碍是英雄主义领导的内隐理论,这种理论倾向于面对失败始终如一的管理者。目前的研究将这种英雄主义理论与主管管理的内隐理论相比较。受试者们阅读了一个案例研究,该案例描述了一个经理,他要么是不断尝试,要么是始终如一,要么是立即成功,要么是在最初的失败之后才成功(即最终成功)。结果支持胜任管理的内隐理论,一致性管理者的第一项政策是有效的,其评分显著高于最终成功的试验管理者,其前两项政策失败。出乎意料的是,在实验中立即取得成功的经理被评为比其他所有经理都高得多。结果表明,人们并不普遍反对实验,而倾向于一致性,对管理者的反应更多地是基于能力的证据,而不是实验和一致性本身。
{"title":"Heroism versus competence: Competing explanations for the effects of experimenting and consistent management","authors":"Patrick A. Knight","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90026-6","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90026-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>B. M. Staw and J. Ross (<em>Journal of Applied Psychology</em>, 1980</span>, <strong>65,</strong> 249–260) present data which suggest that one barrier to experimenting management is an implicit theory of heroic leadership favoring managers who are consistent in the face of failure, The currant research tests this theory of heroism against an implicit theory of competent management. Subjects read a case study describing a manager who was either experimenting or consistent, and either immediately successful or successful only after initial failure (i.e., ultimately successful). The results supported the implicit theory of competent management with the consistent managers, whose first policies were effective, rated significantly higher than the ultimately successful-experimenting manager, whose first two policies failed. The immediately successful-experimenting manager was unexpectedly rated significantly higher than all other managers. The results show that there is not a general bias against experimentation in favor of consistency, and that reactions to managers are based more upon evidence of competence than upon experimenting and consistency per se.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 3","pages":"Pages 307-322"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90026-6","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53839490","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 1984-06-01DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90031-X
{"title":"Acknowledgment","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90031-X","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90031-X","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 3","pages":"Pages 414-415"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90031-X","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136957942","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 1984-06-01DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90032-1
{"title":"Author index for volume 33","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90032-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90032-1","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 3","pages":"Page 416"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90032-1","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136957941","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 1984-06-01DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90029-1
Angelo S. DeNisi, Thomas P. Cafferty, Bruce M. Meglino
This paper presents a model of performance appraisal which focuses on the cognitive processes employed by a rater attempting to form an evaluation. The model describes the method by which a rater collects, encodes, stores, and later retrieves information from memory, and the method by which he or she weights and combines this information to form an evaluation which is converted to a rating on a scale. The model is based on diverse bodies of literature which share a social-cognitive orientation, and it forms the foundation for a number of testable research propositions.
{"title":"A cognitive view of the performance appraisal process: A model and research propositions","authors":"Angelo S. DeNisi, Thomas P. Cafferty, Bruce M. Meglino","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90029-1","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90029-1","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This paper presents a model of performance appraisal which focuses on the cognitive processes employed by a rater attempting to form an evaluation. The model describes the method by which a rater collects, encodes, stores, and later retrieves information from memory, and the method by which he or she weights and combines this information to form an evaluation which is converted to a rating on a scale. The model is based on diverse bodies of literature which share a social-cognitive orientation, and it forms the foundation for a number of testable research propositions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 3","pages":"Pages 360-396"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90029-1","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53839744","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 1984-06-01DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90027-8
James R. Larson Jr., John H. Lingle, Mark M. Scerbo
Previous research has demonstrated that performance cues can significantly influence raters' responses on leader-behavior rating questionnaires. The purpose of the present study was to explore the cognitive mechanisms that mediate this influence. Subjects watched a videotape of a problem-solving group and later rated the group leader's behavior. Perceptions of the group's performance were manipulated both before (preobservation) and after (postobservation) subjects watched the tape. Both manipulations affected the leader-behavior ratings. Analytic procedures borrowed from signal detection theory suggest that the preobservation performances cue effects were mediated by selective encoding of leader-behavior information in memory, while the postobservation effects were mediated by probabilistic response biases. Selective memory retrieval, on the other hand, did not seem to play a role. The implications of these results for developing strategies to overcome the confounding influence of performance cues on leader-behavior ratings are discussed.
{"title":"The impact of performance cues on leader-behavior ratings: The role of selective information availability and probabilistic response bias","authors":"James R. Larson Jr., John H. Lingle, Mark M. Scerbo","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90027-8","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90027-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research has demonstrated that performance cues can significantly influence raters' responses on leader-behavior rating questionnaires. The purpose of the present study was to explore the cognitive mechanisms that mediate this influence. Subjects watched a videotape of a problem-solving group and later rated the group leader's behavior. Perceptions of the group's performance were manipulated both before (preobservation) and after (postobservation) subjects watched the tape. Both manipulations affected the leader-behavior ratings. Analytic procedures borrowed from signal detection theory suggest that the preobservation performances cue effects were mediated by selective encoding of leader-behavior information in memory, while the postobservation effects were mediated by probabilistic response biases. Selective memory retrieval, on the other hand, did not seem to play a role. The implications of these results for developing strategies to overcome the confounding influence of performance cues on leader-behavior ratings are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 3","pages":"Pages 323-349"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90027-8","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53839548","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 1984-06-01DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90030-8
William S. Waller, Terence R. Mitchell
An important theme in research on behavioral decision making is that decision behavior is largely contingent on characteristics of the task and context. One variation on this theme is the appearance of contingency models that explain variation in decision behavior in terms of the effects that changes in task and context have on the relative net benefit of each available decision strategy. The L. R. Beach and T. R. Mitchell (1978, Academy of Management Review, July, 439–449) model is an example of such a model. In the present study, the Beach and Mitchell (1978) model is used as a framework for generating hypotheses about the effects of two contextual variables—state uncertainty and significance of the decision—on the selection of decision strategies for the management problem of whether to investigate the cause of a cost variance. The hypotheses are supported by an experimental test.
行为决策研究的一个重要主题是,决策行为在很大程度上取决于任务和环境的特征。这一主题的一个变体是权变模型的出现,该模型根据任务和环境的变化对每种可用决策策略的相对净收益的影响来解释决策行为的变化。L. R. Beach和T. R. Mitchell (1978, Academy of Management Review, July, 439-449)模型就是这种模型的一个例子。在本研究中,Beach和Mitchell(1978)的模型被用作一个框架,用于产生关于是否调查成本差异原因这一管理问题的决策策略选择的两个上下文变量——状态不确定性和决策的重要性——的影响的假设。这些假设得到了实验检验的支持。
{"title":"The effects of context on the selection of decision strategies for the cost variance investigation problem","authors":"William S. Waller, Terence R. Mitchell","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90030-8","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90030-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>An important theme in research on behavioral decision making is that decision behavior is largely contingent on characteristics of the task and context. One variation on this theme is the appearance of contingency models that explain variation in decision behavior in terms of the effects that changes in task and context have on the relative net benefit of each available decision strategy. The <span>L. R. Beach and T. R. Mitchell (1978</span>, <em>Academy of Management Review</em>, July, 439–449) model is an example of such a model. In the present study, the Beach and Mitchell (1978) model is used as a framework for generating hypotheses about the effects of two contextual variables—state uncertainty and significance of the decision—on the selection of decision strategies for the management problem of whether to investigate the cause of a cost variance. The hypotheses are supported by an experimental test.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 3","pages":"Pages 397-413"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90030-8","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53840019","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 1984-06-01DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90028-X
Bernard Grofman, Scott L. Feld, Guillermo Owen
The judgmental accuracy of group majority decision making in binary choice, multiple-item prediction tasks as a function of group size, average competence of group members, and the shape of the overall distribution of judgmental accuracy in the group are discussed. For the case where the addition of an (N + 1)th member to a group of size N has a known cost function and the value of a correct group decision can be specified, the optimal group size is calculated.
{"title":"Group size and the performance of a composite group majority: Statistical truths and empirical results","authors":"Bernard Grofman, Scott L. Feld, Guillermo Owen","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90028-X","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90028-X","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The judgmental accuracy of group majority decision making in binary choice, multiple-item prediction tasks as a function of group size, average competence of group members, and the shape of the overall distribution of judgmental accuracy in the group are discussed. For the case where the addition of an (<em>N</em> + 1)th member to a group of size <em>N</em> has a known cost function and the value of a correct group decision can be specified, the optimal group size is calculated.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 3","pages":"Pages 350-359"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90028-X","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53839610","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 1984-04-01DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90023-0
Leonard Adelman, Paul J. Sticha, Michael L. Donnell
Most studies comparing the relative effectiveness of multiattribute weighting techniques have found few differences. These studies have, however, failed to systematically vary attribute properties such as number of attributes and distribution of correct attribute weights. It was hypothesized that any weighting technique would be more effective at arriving at the correct weights the smaller the number of attributes because of lessened information processing requirements. In addition, it was hypothesized that the relative effectiveness of different weighting techniques would depend on the peakedness of the distribution of correct attribute weights because different weighting techniques should generate more peaked distributions than others. Two experiments were conducted to test these hypotheses. The first experiment focused on the accuracy of the weights assigned to attributes by individuals; the second on the accuracy of groups. Both experiments confirmed the first hypothesis regarding the number of attributes, but only the first experiment confirmed the second hypothesis regarding the peakedness of the distribution of attribute weights.
{"title":"The role of task properties in determining the relative effectiveness of multiattribute weighting techniques","authors":"Leonard Adelman, Paul J. Sticha, Michael L. Donnell","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90023-0","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90023-0","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Most studies comparing the relative effectiveness of multiattribute weighting techniques have found few differences. These studies have, however, failed to systematically vary attribute properties such as number of attributes and distribution of correct attribute weights. It was hypothesized that any weighting technique would be more effective at arriving at the correct weights the smaller the number of attributes because of lessened information processing requirements. In addition, it was hypothesized that the relative effectiveness of different weighting techniques would depend on the peakedness of the distribution of correct attribute weights because different weighting techniques should generate more peaked distributions than others. Two experiments were conducted to test these hypotheses. The first experiment focused on the accuracy of the weights assigned to attributes by individuals; the second on the accuracy of groups. Both experiments confirmed the first hypothesis regarding the number of attributes, but only the first experiment confirmed the second hypothesis regarding the peakedness of the distribution of attribute weights.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 2","pages":"Pages 243-262"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90023-0","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53838893","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}