首页 > 最新文献

Organizational behavior and human performance最新文献

英文 中文
Affirmative action, negative reaction? Some moderating conditions 平权行动,消极反应?一些缓和条件
Pub Date : 1984-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90021-7
Madeline E. Heilman, Joyce Mardenfeld Herlihy

One hundred and seventy-five male and female college-bound high school students reviewed a description of a managerial job, indicated their interest in the job, and rated it on a number of descriptive dimensions. The proportion of women currently holding this position (8%, 28%) and how they came to acquire their jobs (merit, preferential treatment based on gender, no information) were systematically varied. Results indicated that increased proportions of women job-holders produced greater job interest among females only when it was believed they had acquired their positions on the basis of merit. Furthermore, lower job interest among males was evidenced in preferential treatment as compared to merit conditions. An additional finding of note was the tendency for males to treat no information about position acquisition as they did information of preferential treatment and for females to treat no information about position acquisition as they did information of merit-based placement. The implications of these findings for affirmative action programs are discussed.

175名即将上大学的男女高中生阅读了一份管理工作的描述,表明了他们对这份工作的兴趣,并在一些描述性的维度上给它打分。目前担任这一职位的妇女比例(8%,28%)以及她们获得工作的方式(业绩、基于性别的优惠待遇、无信息)各不相同。结果表明,女性工作人员比例的增加,只有当人们认为她们是根据业绩获得职位时,才会使女性对工作产生更大的兴趣。此外,与绩效条件相比,男性的工作兴趣较低体现在优惠待遇上。另一个值得注意的发现是,男性倾向于把关于职位获得的信息当作优先待遇的信息来对待,而女性倾向于把关于职位获得的信息当作择优安置的信息来对待。讨论了这些发现对平权行动计划的影响。
{"title":"Affirmative action, negative reaction? Some moderating conditions","authors":"Madeline E. Heilman,&nbsp;Joyce Mardenfeld Herlihy","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90021-7","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90021-7","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>One hundred and seventy-five male and female college-bound high school students reviewed a description of a managerial job, indicated their interest in the job, and rated it on a number of descriptive dimensions. The proportion of women currently holding this position (8%, 28%) and how they came to acquire their jobs (merit, preferential treatment based on gender, no information) were systematically varied. Results indicated that increased proportions of women job-holders produced greater job interest among females only when it was believed they had acquired their positions on the basis of merit. Furthermore, lower job interest among males was evidenced in preferential treatment as compared to merit conditions. An additional finding of note was the tendency for males to treat no information about position acquisition as they did information of preferential treatment and for females to treat no information about position acquisition as they did information of merit-based placement. The implications of these findings for affirmative action programs are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 2","pages":"Pages 204-213"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90021-7","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53839349","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 85
Escalation of commitment in individual and group decision making 个人和群体决策中的承诺升级
Pub Date : 1984-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90017-5
Max H. Bazerman , Toni Giuliano, Alan Appelman

Previous research (cf. B. M. Staw, Academy of Management Review, 1981, 6, 577–587) has found that when managers are given negative feedback on an initial individual investment decision, they allocate more additional funds to that investment if they, rather than another member of their organization, made the initial allocation decision. Justification is thought to underlie this phenomenon. This study explored commitment in group and individual decisions and examined the plausibility of dissonance processes as the mediator of escalation of commitment. One hundred eighty-three individuals participated in a role-playing exercise in which personal responsibility for an initial decision was manipulated for groups and individuals. As expected, escalation of commitment occurred for both groups and individuals. In support of a dissonance explanation, dissonance processes did vary as a function of the personal responsibility manipulation, and individual variation in dissonance responses accounted for a substantial portion of variance in allocation behavior beyond that accounted for by the experimental manipulations. The results concerning dissonance processes suggest a number of ways in which escalation can be reduced in individuals and groups.

先前的研究(参见b.m. Staw, Academy of Management Review, 1981, 6,577 - 587)发现,当管理者在最初的个人投资决策中得到负面反馈时,如果做出最初分配决策的是他们自己,而不是组织中的其他成员,他们会为该投资分配更多的额外资金。正当理由被认为是这种现象的基础。本研究探讨了团体和个人决策中的承诺,并检验了失调过程作为承诺升级中介的合理性。183个人参加了一个角色扮演练习,在这个练习中,个人对最初决定的责任被操纵为群体和个人。正如预期的那样,团队和个人的承诺都出现了升级。为了支持失调的解释,失调过程确实作为个人责任操纵的函数而变化,而失调反应的个体差异在分配行为的差异中占了很大一部分,超出了实验操作的影响。有关失调过程的研究结果表明,在个人和群体中,有许多方法可以减少失调升级。
{"title":"Escalation of commitment in individual and group decision making","authors":"Max H. Bazerman ,&nbsp;Toni Giuliano,&nbsp;Alan Appelman","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90017-5","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90017-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research (<span>cf. B. M. Staw, <em>Academy of Management Review</em>, 1981,</span> <strong>6</strong>, 577–587) has found that when managers are given negative feedback on an initial individual investment decision, they allocate more additional funds to that investment if they, rather than another member of their organization, made the initial allocation decision. Justification is thought to underlie this phenomenon. This study explored commitment in group and individual decisions and examined the plausibility of dissonance processes as the mediator of escalation of commitment. One hundred eighty-three individuals participated in a role-playing exercise in which personal responsibility for an initial decision was manipulated for groups and individuals. As expected, escalation of commitment occurred for both groups and individuals. In support of a dissonance explanation, dissonance processes did vary as a function of the personal responsibility manipulation, and individual variation in dissonance responses accounted for a substantial portion of variance in allocation behavior beyond that accounted for by the experimental manipulations. The results concerning dissonance processes suggest a number of ways in which escalation can be reduced in individuals and groups.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 2","pages":"Pages 141-152"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90017-5","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53839005","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 282
Factors appearing late in practice 实践中出现较晚的因素
Pub Date : 1984-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90018-7
Marshall B. Jones, William P. Dunlap, Ina McD. Bilodeau

With extended practice on a task a shift seems to occur from controlled to automatic processing. If differential factors associated with automatic processing exist, they could only be observed after this shift occurs. Hence, any such factor would be late-appearing, in the sense that it could only be identified late in practice. The present paper reports two tests of the existence of late-appearing factors. Both tests involved extended practice on five video games; the two tests were carried out in two different populations approximately 1 year apart. The results of the two experiments were in complete agreement. In both cases all factors, with one possible exception in the second experiment, were identified by content exclusively and not by stage of practice. The results, therefore, are negative. Other studies using other materials, other subjects, or other conditions of practice may reach different conclusions; but the studies reported in this paper offer no support for the existence of late-appearing factors.

随着对一项任务的长期练习,似乎发生了从控制到自动处理的转变。如果存在与自动加工相关的差异因素,则只有在这种转变发生后才能观察到。因此,任何这类因素都是晚出现的,也就是说,它只能在实践中晚被发现。本文报道了对迟发因素存在性的两个检验。两项测试都涉及五种电子游戏的扩展练习;这两项试验是在两个不同的种群中进行的,间隔大约1年。这两个实验的结果完全一致。在这两种情况下,除了第二个实验中的一个可能的例外,所有因素都是由内容而不是由实践阶段确定的。因此,结果是负面的。使用其他材料、其他科目或者其他实践条件进行的其他研究可能得出不同的结论;但本文的研究并未支持迟发性因素的存在。
{"title":"Factors appearing late in practice","authors":"Marshall B. Jones,&nbsp;William P. Dunlap,&nbsp;Ina McD. Bilodeau","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90018-7","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90018-7","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>With extended practice on a task a shift seems to occur from controlled to automatic processing. If differential factors associated with automatic processing exist, they could only be observed after this shift occurs. Hence, any such factor would be late-appearing, in the sense that it could only be identified late in practice. The present paper reports two tests of the existence of late-appearing factors. Both tests involved extended practice on five video games; the two tests were carried out in two different populations approximately 1 year apart. The results of the two experiments were in complete agreement. In both cases all factors, with one possible exception in the second experiment, were identified by content exclusively and not by stage of practice. The results, therefore, are negative. Other studies using other materials, other subjects, or other conditions of practice may reach different conclusions; but the studies reported in this paper offer no support for the existence of late-appearing factors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 2","pages":"Pages 153-173"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90018-7","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53839116","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Reducing the influence of irrelevant information on experienced decision makers 减少不相关信息对有经验的决策者的影响
Pub Date : 1984-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90024-2
Gary J. Gaeth, James Shanteau

In this study two training procedures designed to reduce the adverse influence of irrelevant information were tested for their efficacy. One training procedure involved a lecture (paralleling typical classroom experience), while the other involved interaction and practice (paralleling typical laboratory experience). In a pretest, irrelevant information was shown to influence the judgments of 12 experienced student soil judges. The judges were then given lecture training; this was found to be of minimal help in reducing the influence of irrelevance. Next, the judges received the interactive training and that had a significant impact. In addition to reducing the influence of irrelevance, the interactive training also improved the accuracy of the judgments. In a followup study, five of the soil judges were reevaluated over a year later; the training appeared to have continued impact. Implications and extensions of the training procedures to other areas are discussed.

在本研究中,两种训练程序旨在减少不相关信息的不利影响,以测试其有效性。一个训练过程包括讲座(与典型的课堂经验平行),而另一个包括互动和实践(与典型的实验室经验平行)。在前测中,不相关信息影响了12名经验丰富的学生土壤裁判的判断。然后对评委进行讲座培训;研究发现,这对减少无关性的影响帮助极小。接下来,评委们接受了互动培训,这产生了显著的影响。除了减少不相关的影响外,交互式训练还提高了判断的准确性。在一项后续研究中,五名土壤评判员在一年后被重新评估;培训似乎产生了持续的影响。讨论了培训程序对其他领域的影响和扩展。
{"title":"Reducing the influence of irrelevant information on experienced decision makers","authors":"Gary J. Gaeth,&nbsp;James Shanteau","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90024-2","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90024-2","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In this study two training procedures designed to reduce the adverse influence of irrelevant information were tested for their efficacy. One training procedure involved a lecture (paralleling typical classroom experience), while the other involved interaction and practice (paralleling typical laboratory experience). In a pretest, irrelevant information was shown to influence the judgments of 12 experienced student soil judges. The judges were then given lecture training; this was found to be of minimal help in reducing the influence of irrelevance. Next, the judges received the interactive training and that had a significant impact. In addition to reducing the influence of irrelevance, the interactive training also improved the accuracy of the judgments. In a followup study, five of the soil judges were reevaluated over a year later; the training appeared to have continued impact. Implications and extensions of the training procedures to other areas are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 2","pages":"Pages 263-282"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90024-2","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53839398","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 219
Subjective evaluation and allocation of resources in routine decision making 日常决策中的主观评价和资源分配
Pub Date : 1984-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90020-5
Gerry Gingrich, Sigfrid D. Soli

Routine decision making is a process of identifying goals, evaluating alternatives, and formulating appropriate strategies for attaining these goals. The latter two stages of this process were examined in a two-part experiment. In the first part, subjects scaled alternative combinations of the resources required for goal attainment. The results showed that an explicitly defined goal directly influenced the utility of resources. In essence, mean utilities were equivalent to point estimates of cost-benefit analyses. In the second part, subjects performed a decision-making task, allocating the initial resource combinations to achieve an explicitly defined goal. Task performance was modeled with linear programming techniques which provide a means of evaluating both the subjects' decisions and the process of resource allocation and strategy formulation. Results indicated that only 1 of 12 subjects allocated resources optimally in making decisions, even though all of the subjects had appropriately scaled the utility of these resources in the first part of the experiment. Moreover, half the subjects failed to utilize the maximum available resources in making their decisions. This outcome is discussed in terms of the memory and attentional constraints on routine decision-making processes. The results suggest that these constraints are most severe at the time of strategy formulation, even when the utility of resources and the explicit goal are known.

日常决策是一个确定目标、评估备选方案和制定实现这些目标的适当策略的过程。这一过程的后两个阶段在两个部分的实验中进行了检验。在第一部分中,受试者衡量实现目标所需资源的不同组合。结果表明,明确的目标直接影响资源的利用。从本质上讲,平均效用相当于成本效益分析的点估计。在第二部分中,受试者执行决策任务,分配初始资源组合以实现明确定义的目标。利用线性规划技术对任务绩效进行建模,为评估被试的决策、资源分配和策略制定过程提供了一种手段。结果表明,尽管在实验的第一部分,所有的被试都适当地衡量了这些资源的效用,但在12个被试中,只有1个被试在做决策时分配了最佳的资源。此外,一半的受试者在做决定时未能最大限度地利用可用资源。这一结果在日常决策过程的记忆和注意力限制方面进行了讨论。结果表明,这些限制在战略制定时最为严重,即使资源的效用和明确的目标是已知的。
{"title":"Subjective evaluation and allocation of resources in routine decision making","authors":"Gerry Gingrich,&nbsp;Sigfrid D. Soli","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90020-5","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90020-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Routine decision making is a process of identifying goals, evaluating alternatives, and formulating appropriate strategies for attaining these goals. The latter two stages of this process were examined in a two-part experiment. In the first part, subjects scaled alternative combinations of the resources required for goal attainment. The results showed that an explicitly defined goal directly influenced the utility of resources. In essence, mean utilities were equivalent to point estimates of cost-benefit analyses. In the second part, subjects performed a decision-making task, allocating the initial resource combinations to achieve an explicitly defined goal. Task performance was modeled with linear programming techniques which provide a means of evaluating both the subjects' decisions and the process of resource allocation and strategy formulation. Results indicated that only 1 of 12 subjects allocated resources optimally in making decisions, even though all of the subjects had appropriately scaled the utility of these resources in the first part of the experiment. Moreover, half the subjects failed to utilize the maximum available resources in making their decisions. This outcome is discussed in terms of the memory and attentional constraints on routine decision-making processes. The results suggest that these constraints are most severe at the time of strategy formulation, even when the utility of resources and the explicit goal are known.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 2","pages":"Pages 187-203"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90020-5","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53839218","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 15
Sources of environmental structuring and participant responses 环境结构的来源和参与者的反应
Pub Date : 1984-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90022-9
Jon L. Pierce, Randall B. Dunham, L.L. Cummings

This study investigated the effects of four sources of environmental structuring on employee responses. These sources are job, technology, work unit, and leader behavior. Main, joint, and interactive effects were examined. Job structure had the strongest effect. It was demonstrated that technology, job, and work unit structure were found to be substitutes for leader structure. Leader structure had little unique association with employee reactions except when the other sources of environmental structure were weak. Two alternative theoretical interpretations of the findings are offered: reactivity and “closeness.”

本研究探讨了环境建构的四种来源对员工反应的影响。这些来源是工作、技术、工作单位和领导行为。研究了主要效应、联合效应和交互效应。工作结构的影响最大。结果表明,技术结构、岗位结构和工作单位结构可以替代领导结构。除了环境结构的其他来源较弱外,领导结构对员工反应的影响不大。对这些发现提出了两种不同的理论解释:反应性和“亲近性”。
{"title":"Sources of environmental structuring and participant responses","authors":"Jon L. Pierce,&nbsp;Randall B. Dunham,&nbsp;L.L. Cummings","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90022-9","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90022-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study investigated the effects of four sources of environmental structuring on employee responses. These sources are job, technology, work unit, and leader behavior. Main, joint, and interactive effects were examined. Job structure had the strongest effect. It was demonstrated that technology, job, and work unit structure were found to be substitutes for leader structure. Leader structure had little unique association with employee reactions except when the other sources of environmental structure were weak. Two alternative theoretical interpretations of the findings are offered: reactivity and “closeness.”</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 2","pages":"Pages 214-242"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90022-9","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53839363","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 33
Information as a deterrent against sex discrimination: The effects of applicant sex and information type on preliminary employment decisions 信息对性别歧视的威慑作用:申请人性别和信息类型对初步就业决定的影响
Pub Date : 1984-04-01 DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90019-9
Madeline E. Heilman

To investigate the idea that providing information about a job applicant's past performance can avert sex discrimination in preliminary employment decisions, an experiment was conducted in which both Applicant Sex and Type of Information were varied. As predicted, highly job-relevant information was found to produce less differential treatment of male and female applicants than did information of low job relevance or no information at all. Also as predicted, the type of information provided had more impact on reactions to female applicants than male applicants, with high job-relevance information producing the most favorable responses and, unexpectedly, low job-relevance information producing the least favorable responses to female applicants. Additional results suggested that these effects were mediated by the degree to which female job applicants were characterized by stereotypic attributes. The findings are interpreted as supportive of the idea that undermining the information value of sex stereotypes as a basis of inference about the attributes of a given woman can function to reduce sex discrimination in employment settings.

为了研究提供求职者过去的工作表现信息是否可以避免初步就业决策中的性别歧视,我们进行了一项实验,在实验中,求职者的性别和信息类型都是不同的。正如预测的那样,高度工作相关的信息比低工作相关的信息或根本没有信息对男性和女性求职者产生的差别待遇要小。正如预测的那样,所提供的信息类型对女性申请人的反应比男性申请人的反应有更大的影响,高工作相关性的信息产生了最有利的反应,而出乎意料的是,低工作相关性的信息产生了对女性申请人最不利的反应。另外的结果表明,这些影响是由女性求职者的刻板印象特征程度所介导的。这些发现被解释为支持这样一种观点,即破坏性别刻板印象的信息价值,作为推断特定女性特征的基础,可以减少就业环境中的性别歧视。
{"title":"Information as a deterrent against sex discrimination: The effects of applicant sex and information type on preliminary employment decisions","authors":"Madeline E. Heilman","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90019-9","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90019-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>To investigate the idea that providing information about a job applicant's past performance can avert sex discrimination in preliminary employment decisions, an experiment was conducted in which both Applicant Sex and Type of Information were varied. As predicted, highly job-relevant information was found to produce less differential treatment of male and female applicants than did information of low job relevance or no information at all. Also as predicted, the type of information provided had more impact on reactions to female applicants than male applicants, with high job-relevance information producing the most favorable responses and, unexpectedly, low job-relevance information producing the least favorable responses to female applicants. Additional results suggested that these effects were mediated by the degree to which female job applicants were characterized by stereotypic attributes. The findings are interpreted as supportive of the idea that undermining the information value of sex stereotypes as a basis of inference about the attributes of a given woman can function to reduce sex discrimination in employment settings.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 2","pages":"Pages 174-186"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90019-9","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53839149","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 170
Job stress and job performance controversy: An empirical assessment 工作压力与工作绩效之争:一项实证评估
Pub Date : 1984-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90009-6
Muhammad Jamal

This study examined the relationship between job stress and employees' performance and withdrawal behavior among nurses (N = 440) in two hospitals in a metropolitan Canadian city on the east coast. Job stressors assessed included role ambiguity, role overload, role conflict, and resource inadequacy. Employees' performance was operationalized in terms of job performance, motivation, and patient care skill. Withdrawal behaviors assessed were absenteeism, tardiness, and anticipated turnover. Multiple regressions, curvilinear correlation coefficients, and canonical correlations were computed to test the nature of the relationship between stressors and the criterion variables of the study. In general, data were more supportive of the negative linear relationship between stress and performance than for positive linear or curvilinear relationship. However, the stressor role ambiguity did exhibit a monotonic nonlinear relationship with a number of criterion variables. Employees' professional and organizational commitment were proposed to moderate the stress—performance relationship. However, the data only partially supported the role of the moderators.

本研究调查了加拿大东海岸某大城市两家医院护士(N = 440)的工作压力与员工绩效和退缩行为的关系。评估的工作压力源包括角色模糊、角色超载、角色冲突和资源不足。员工绩效从工作绩效、动机和病人护理技能三个方面进行操作化。评估的退出行为包括旷工、迟到和预期离职。计算多元回归、曲线相关系数和典型相关来检验压力源与研究标准变量之间关系的本质。总体而言,数据更支持压力与绩效之间的负线性关系,而不是正线性或曲线关系。然而,压力源角色模糊性与多个判据变量之间存在单调的非线性关系。提出员工的专业承诺和组织承诺对压力-绩效关系起到调节作用。然而,数据只是部分地支持了主持人的作用。
{"title":"Job stress and job performance controversy: An empirical assessment","authors":"Muhammad Jamal","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90009-6","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90009-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study examined the relationship between job stress and employees' performance and withdrawal behavior among nurses (<em>N</em> = 440) in two hospitals in a metropolitan Canadian city on the east coast. Job stressors assessed included role ambiguity, role overload, role conflict, and resource inadequacy. Employees' performance was operationalized in terms of job performance, motivation, and patient care skill. Withdrawal behaviors assessed were absenteeism, tardiness, and anticipated turnover. Multiple regressions, curvilinear correlation coefficients, and canonical correlations were computed to test the <em>nature</em> of the relationship between stressors and the criterion variables of the study. In general, data were more supportive of the negative linear relationship between stress and performance than for positive linear or curvilinear relationship. However, the stressor role ambiguity did exhibit a monotonic nonlinear relationship with a number of criterion variables. Employees' professional and organizational commitment were proposed to moderate the stress—performance relationship. However, the data only partially supported the role of the moderators.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 1","pages":"Pages 1-21"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90009-6","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"21135699","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 422
The role of modeling processes in the “knee deep in the big muddy” phenomenon 建模过程中的作用在“膝深大泥”现象
Pub Date : 1984-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90012-6
Joel Brockner, Sinaia Nathanson, Alan Friend, John Harbeck, Charles Samuelson, Robert Houser, Max H. Bazerman, Jeffrey Z. Rubin

Entrapping conflicts are characterized by the tendency for individuals to make increasing commitments to some failing course of action, in large part to justify the appropriateness of previous investments made in that situation. It has been suggested that (1) these dilemmas frequently arise in organizational settings, and (2) whether decision makers escalate their commitment or withdraw from a failing course of action can be affected by a variety of factors. The present four experiments were designed to determine if an individual's degree of entrapment (i.e., tendency to escalate) may be affected by the behavior of a model in a similar situation. In Experiments 1–3, considerable generality was obtained for the proposition that decision makers in entrapping conflicts can be directly influenced by a model. Individuals who witnessed an entrapped model became significantly more entrapped than those who did not. This effect was obtained (1) for subjects and models of both sexes, (2) on measures of both process and outcome, (3) across two different experimental procedures, and (4) when the model was viewed either during or before the time that subjects were called upon to make their decisions. Experiments 3 and 4 delineated several limiting conditions of the modeling-entrapment relationship. Participants did not invest more of their resources in the presence than in the absence of an entrapped model if the model expressed regret rather than pleasure about his behavior. In fact, under such conditions a significant “reverse modeling” effect was obtained, in which subjects became less entrapped in the presence of the entrapped model. The modeling-entrapment relationship was also significantly reduced when the model was unlikable and unintelligent, and thus not an appropriate person for comparison. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed, including the possibility that models may be skillfully employed to help decision makers stop “throwing good money after bad.”

陷阱冲突的特点是个人倾向于对某些失败的行动方针作出越来越多的承诺,这在很大程度上是为了证明以前在这种情况下所作投资的适当性。有人提出:(1)这些困境经常出现在组织环境中,(2)决策者是升级他们的承诺还是退出失败的行动过程会受到多种因素的影响。目前的四个实验旨在确定一个人的困住程度(即倾向升级)是否会受到类似情况下模型行为的影响。在实验1-3中,陷阱冲突中的决策者可以直接受到模型的影响这一命题获得了相当大的通用性。目睹了陷入困境的模型的个体明显比没有目睹的个体更容易陷入困境。这种影响是(1)对两性的受试者和模型,(2)对过程和结果的测量,(3)跨越两个不同的实验程序,(4)在受试者被要求做出决定的时间期间或之前查看模型时获得的。实验3和4描述了建模-捕获关系的几个极限条件。如果模型对自己的行为表示遗憾而不是高兴,那么参与者在模型存在的情况下不会比在模型不存在的情况下投入更多的资源。事实上,在这种情况下,获得了显著的“反向建模”效应,即被试在被困模型存在的情况下变得不那么被困。当模型不讨人喜欢和不聪明,因此不适合进行比较时,建模-陷阱关系也显着降低。讨论了理论和实践意义,包括模型可能被巧妙地应用于帮助决策者停止“把钱花在坏事上”的可能性。
{"title":"The role of modeling processes in the “knee deep in the big muddy” phenomenon","authors":"Joel Brockner,&nbsp;Sinaia Nathanson,&nbsp;Alan Friend,&nbsp;John Harbeck,&nbsp;Charles Samuelson,&nbsp;Robert Houser,&nbsp;Max H. Bazerman,&nbsp;Jeffrey Z. Rubin","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90012-6","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90012-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Entrapping conflicts are characterized by the tendency for individuals to make increasing commitments to some failing course of action, in large part to justify the appropriateness of previous investments made in that situation. It has been suggested that (1) these dilemmas frequently arise in organizational settings, and (2) whether decision makers escalate their commitment or withdraw from a failing course of action can be affected by a variety of factors. The present four experiments were designed to determine if an individual's degree of entrapment (i.e., tendency to escalate) may be affected by the behavior of a model in a similar situation. In Experiments 1–3, considerable generality was obtained for the proposition that decision makers in entrapping conflicts can be directly influenced by a model. Individuals who witnessed an entrapped model became significantly more entrapped than those who did not. This effect was obtained (1) for subjects and models of both sexes, (2) on measures of both process and outcome, (3) across two different experimental procedures, and (4) when the model was viewed either during or before the time that subjects were called upon to make their decisions. Experiments 3 and 4 delineated several limiting conditions of the modeling-entrapment relationship. Participants did not invest more of their resources in the presence than in the absence of an entrapped model if the model expressed regret rather than pleasure about his behavior. In fact, under such conditions a significant “reverse modeling” effect was obtained, in which subjects became <em>less</em> entrapped in the presence of the entrapped model. The modeling-entrapment relationship was also significantly reduced when the model was unlikable and unintelligent, and thus not an appropriate person for comparison. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed, including the possibility that models may be skillfully employed to help decision makers stop “throwing good money after bad.”</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 1","pages":"Pages 77-99"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90012-6","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53838514","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 60
Individual differences in response to unfavorable group feedback 对不利群体反馈反应的个体差异
Pub Date : 1984-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/0030-5073(84)90013-8
Philip E. Varca, Jodie C. Levy

The purpose of this study was to examine how individual differences in repression—sensitization moderate the effects of negative feedback in task groups. Seventy-eight research participants, organized into 26 simulated task groups, were asked to work as a team on a series of anagram problems. Participants received either unfavorable individual or team feedback concerning performance. It was predicted that the direct threat associated with individual feedback would increase defensiveness among repressors. This hypothesis was supported; that is, repressors, in comparison to sensitizers, denied the group task as a measure of ability and effort and reported lower attraction to the task and group. It was predicted further that the indirect threat of team level feedback would increase defensiveness among sensitizers. This hypothesis was also supported. Here sensitizers, in comparison to repressors, denied the task as a measure of ability and effort and indicated lower attraction to the task and group. Additional analyses of covariance revealed that the cognitive processes associated with self-attributions (i.e., ability and effort) mediated the reports of task and group attraction. Theoretical and practical implications of these findings and directions for future study are also discussed.

本研究的目的是检验在任务组中,抑制-敏化的个体差异如何调节负反馈的影响。78名研究参与者被分成26个模拟任务组,被要求以团队形式解决一系列字谜问题。参与者要么收到个人或团队关于表现的不利反馈。据预测,与个体反馈相关的直接威胁会增加压抑者的防御能力。这一假设得到了支持;也就是说,与敏感者相比,压抑者拒绝将群体任务作为能力和努力的衡量标准,并且对任务和群体的吸引力较低。进一步预测,团队层面反馈的间接威胁会增加致敏者的防卫性。这一假设也得到了支持。在这里,与抑制者相比,敏感者拒绝将任务作为能力和努力的衡量标准,并且对任务和群体的吸引力较低。进一步的协方差分析表明,与自我归因相关的认知过程(即能力和努力)介导了任务和群体吸引力的报告。本文还讨论了这些发现的理论和实践意义以及未来研究的方向。
{"title":"Individual differences in response to unfavorable group feedback","authors":"Philip E. Varca,&nbsp;Jodie C. Levy","doi":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90013-8","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0030-5073(84)90013-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The purpose of this study was to examine how individual differences in repression—sensitization moderate the effects of negative feedback in task groups. Seventy-eight research participants, organized into 26 simulated task groups, were asked to work as a team on a series of anagram problems. Participants received either unfavorable individual or team feedback concerning performance. It was predicted that the direct threat associated with individual feedback would increase defensiveness among repressors. This hypothesis was supported; that is, repressors, in comparison to sensitizers, denied the group task as a measure of ability and effort and reported lower attraction to the task and group. It was predicted further that the indirect threat of team level feedback would increase defensiveness among sensitizers. This hypothesis was also supported. Here sensitizers, in comparison to repressors, denied the task as a measure of ability and effort and indicated lower attraction to the task and group. Additional analyses of covariance revealed that the cognitive processes associated with self-attributions (i.e., ability and effort) mediated the reports of task and group attraction. Theoretical and practical implications of these findings and directions for future study are also discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":76928,"journal":{"name":"Organizational behavior and human performance","volume":"33 1","pages":"Pages 100-111"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0030-5073(84)90013-8","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"53838689","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
期刊
Organizational behavior and human performance
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1