首页 > 最新文献

Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs最新文献

英文 中文
Chinese adolescents' perceptions of family functioning: personal, school-related, and family correlates. 中国青少年对家庭功能的认知:个人、学校和家庭相关。
Daniel T L Shek

The author used two translated measures (Self-Report Family Inventory; D. T. L. Shek, 1998); and Family Assessment Device; N. B. Epstein, L. M. Baldwin, & D. S. Bishop, 1983) and a locally developed scale (Chinese Family Assessment Instrument; D. T. L. Shek, in press-a) to assess 3,649 Chinese adolescents' perceptions of how well their families function. He found that boys perceived their families to function worse than did girls and that younger adolescents perceived their families to function better than did older adolescents. Perceived family functioning was negatively related to grade level; students attending schools with higher academic standards perceived their families to function better than did students attending schools with lower academic standards; and students attending government and aided schools had higher levels of family functioning than did students attending private schools. Family types (intact vs. nonintact families) and the duration of parents' stay in Hong Kong were also related to the adolescents' perceptions of family functioning. Findings for the personal, school-related, and family correlates of perceived family functioning were statistically significant and stable across different measures of family functioning, but the practical significance of the findings was not high.

作者使用了两种翻译的测量方法(自我报告家庭量表;石德立,1998);和家庭评估装置;N. B. Epstein, L. M. Baldwin, & D. S. Bishop, 1983)和本地开发的量表(中国家庭评估工具;D. T. L. Shek,出版a)评估3,649名中国青少年对其家庭运作好坏的看法。他发现,男孩比女孩更容易理解家庭的运作,而青少年比年长的青少年更容易理解家庭的运作。感知家庭功能与年级水平负相关;在学术水平较高的学校上学的学生比在学术水平较低的学校上学的学生认为他们的家庭运作得更好;公立学校和资助学校的学生比私立学校的学生有更高的家庭功能水平。家庭类型(完整家庭与非完整家庭)及父母在港逗留时间亦与青少年对家庭功能的认知有关。感知家庭功能的个人、学校相关和家庭相关的发现在不同的家庭功能测量中具有统计学意义和稳定性,但研究结果的实际意义并不高。
{"title":"Chinese adolescents' perceptions of family functioning: personal, school-related, and family correlates.","authors":"Daniel T L Shek","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The author used two translated measures (Self-Report Family Inventory; D. T. L. Shek, 1998); and Family Assessment Device; N. B. Epstein, L. M. Baldwin, & D. S. Bishop, 1983) and a locally developed scale (Chinese Family Assessment Instrument; D. T. L. Shek, in press-a) to assess 3,649 Chinese adolescents' perceptions of how well their families function. He found that boys perceived their families to function worse than did girls and that younger adolescents perceived their families to function better than did older adolescents. Perceived family functioning was negatively related to grade level; students attending schools with higher academic standards perceived their families to function better than did students attending schools with lower academic standards; and students attending government and aided schools had higher levels of family functioning than did students attending private schools. Family types (intact vs. nonintact families) and the duration of parents' stay in Hong Kong were also related to the adolescents' perceptions of family functioning. Findings for the personal, school-related, and family correlates of perceived family functioning were statistically significant and stable across different measures of family functioning, but the practical significance of the findings was not high.</p>","PeriodicalId":77145,"journal":{"name":"Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs","volume":"128 4","pages":"358-80"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"22318721","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's and adults' understanding of illness: evidence in support of a coexistence model. 儿童和成人对疾病的理解:支持共存模型的证据。
Lakshmi Raman, Gerald A Winer

The authors investigated three seemingly contradictory views of the conception of illness: (a) the traditional, developmental "naive child" view; (b) a more contemporary, "sophisticated child" outlook; and (c) a largely social-psychological, "irrational adult" approach, They concluded that participants had a variety of views of the conception of illness and that they used different views in different experimental contexts. They found that, under certain conditions, children appeared to have sophisticated beliefs; under other conditions, children, and even adults, showed signs of folkloric and immanent justice reasoning. They also found that, with advancing grade level, children increasingly recognized psychological contributors to the cause of illness.

作者调查了关于疾病概念的三种看似矛盾的观点:(a)传统的、发展性的“幼稚儿童”观点;(b)更现代、更“成熟”的儿童观;他们的结论是,参与者对疾病的概念有各种各样的看法,并且他们在不同的实验环境中使用不同的观点。他们发现,在某些条件下,孩子们似乎有复杂的信仰;在其他条件下,儿童,甚至成人,表现出民间传说和内在正义推理的迹象。他们还发现,随着年级的提高,孩子们越来越认识到心理因素是导致疾病的原因。
{"title":"Children's and adults' understanding of illness: evidence in support of a coexistence model.","authors":"Lakshmi Raman,&nbsp;Gerald A Winer","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The authors investigated three seemingly contradictory views of the conception of illness: (a) the traditional, developmental \"naive child\" view; (b) a more contemporary, \"sophisticated child\" outlook; and (c) a largely social-psychological, \"irrational adult\" approach, They concluded that participants had a variety of views of the conception of illness and that they used different views in different experimental contexts. They found that, under certain conditions, children appeared to have sophisticated beliefs; under other conditions, children, and even adults, showed signs of folkloric and immanent justice reasoning. They also found that, with advancing grade level, children increasingly recognized psychological contributors to the cause of illness.</p>","PeriodicalId":77145,"journal":{"name":"Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs","volume":"128 4","pages":"325-55"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"22318720","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distractibility after frontal lobe lesions: behavioral and event-related brain potential evidence. 额叶损伤后的注意力分散:行为和事件相关的脑电位证据。
Smadar Birnboim, Zvia Breznitz, Hillel Pratt, Yehudit Aharon

The authors used a semantic priming paradigm to investigate the distractibility phenomenon of patients with frontal lobe lesions (FLPs). They tested two distractibility categories: an inhibition category requiring inhibition of an automatic response and a distraction category caused by irrelevant, unexpected stimuli. Fifteen FLPs were compared with 2 matched control groups: 14 posterior-lesion patients and 15 normal controls. Both behavioral and event-related brain potential (ERP) measures were used. The results suggest that, although there were differences in performance between the FLP group and the control groups, there was no evidence that the FLPs had difficulty specifically with the inhibition category. The most consistent ERP result was that the FLP group had longer N100 latencies than either control group. On the basis of these results, the authors hypothesize that FLPs have difficulty focusing on and starting to process a new stimulus. The authors also discuss the unique contribution ERP evidence has made to cognitive studies.

作者使用语义启动范式来研究额叶病变患者的注意力分散现象。他们测试了两种分散注意力的类别:一种是需要抑制自动反应的抑制类别,另一种是由无关的、意外的刺激引起的分散注意力类别。将15例FLPs与2个匹配的对照组进行比较:14例后病变患者和15例正常对照组。使用了行为和事件相关脑电位(ERP)测量。结果表明,尽管FLP组和对照组之间的表现存在差异,但没有证据表明FLP组在抑制类别方面有特别的困难。最一致的ERP结果是,FLP组的N100潜伏期比任何一个对照组都长。基于这些结果,作者假设flp很难集中注意力并开始处理新的刺激。作者还讨论了ERP证据对认知研究的独特贡献。
{"title":"Distractibility after frontal lobe lesions: behavioral and event-related brain potential evidence.","authors":"Smadar Birnboim,&nbsp;Zvia Breznitz,&nbsp;Hillel Pratt,&nbsp;Yehudit Aharon","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The authors used a semantic priming paradigm to investigate the distractibility phenomenon of patients with frontal lobe lesions (FLPs). They tested two distractibility categories: an inhibition category requiring inhibition of an automatic response and a distraction category caused by irrelevant, unexpected stimuli. Fifteen FLPs were compared with 2 matched control groups: 14 posterior-lesion patients and 15 normal controls. Both behavioral and event-related brain potential (ERP) measures were used. The results suggest that, although there were differences in performance between the FLP group and the control groups, there was no evidence that the FLPs had difficulty specifically with the inhibition category. The most consistent ERP result was that the FLP group had longer N100 latencies than either control group. On the basis of these results, the authors hypothesize that FLPs have difficulty focusing on and starting to process a new stimulus. The authors also discuss the unique contribution ERP evidence has made to cognitive studies.</p>","PeriodicalId":77145,"journal":{"name":"Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs","volume":"128 4","pages":"382-407"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"22317487","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hierarchical categorization by bilingual Latino children: does a basic-level bias exist? 双语拉丁裔儿童的等级分类:是否存在基本水平的偏见?
Valerie Malabonga, Robert Pasnak

Research has shown that children classify most easily at the basic level where objects in the same category look similar enough to each other to be grouped together but are distinct enough from objects in other categories to be discriminated (e.g., animal/bird/duck). In this article, the authors report on 2 experiments they conducted to determine whether children maintain this basic category bias when the perceptual similarity of stimuli at different hierarchical levels is equalized. Pictures within and across 3 hierarchical levels were made perceptually equivalent and shown to 71 Latino children who were bilingual in Spanish and English. In Experiment 1, the pictures used as exemplars could be categorized on any of the 3 hierarchical levels. In Experiment 2, example pictures unambiguously defined the level of categorization that would be accurate, and linguistic cues were given that might assist in the selection of the correct category. In both experiments, the children sorted pictures from all 3 levels equally well, but they found it harder to justify their sorting of superordinate pictures. English competence predicted sorting on the more ambiguous sorting task in Experiment 1; and English competence predicted verbal justifications in both experiments, even though the experiments were conducted in Spanish. Competence in Spanish or English was an equally good predictor of sorting in the better defined sorting task in Experiment 2. These findings indicate that a superordinate level deficiency remains after perceptual differences are eliminated and that the deficiency is cognitive in nature. Differences in the performances of children who differed in bilingualism support the hypothesis that a threshold of proficiency in both languages is an important determinant of the effect of bilingualism on categorization.

研究表明,儿童最容易在基本水平上进行分类,同一类别的物体彼此看起来足够相似,可以归类在一起,但与其他类别的物体足够不同,可以区分(例如,动物/鸟/鸭子)。在这篇文章中,作者报告了他们进行的两个实验,以确定当不同层次刺激的感知相似性相等时,儿童是否保持这种基本类别偏见。三个层次内和层次间的图片在感知上是相等的,并展示给71名西班牙语和英语双语的拉丁裔儿童。在实验1中,作为范例的图片可以在三个层次中的任何一个层次上进行分类。在实验2中,示例图片明确地定义了准确的分类水平,并给出了可能有助于选择正确类别的语言线索。在这两个实验中,孩子们对所有三个级别的图片都分类得很好,但他们发现很难证明自己对高级图片的分类是正确的。在实验1中,英语能力对较为模糊的排序任务有预测作用;在这两个实验中,英语能力预测了口头辩护,尽管实验是用西班牙语进行的。在实验2中,西班牙语或英语的能力同样能很好地预测排序能力。这些发现表明,在消除知觉差异后,仍然存在上级水平缺陷,并且这种缺陷本质上是认知的。不同双语儿童的表现差异支持了两种语言熟练程度阈值是双语对分类影响的重要决定因素这一假设。
{"title":"Hierarchical categorization by bilingual Latino children: does a basic-level bias exist?","authors":"Valerie Malabonga,&nbsp;Robert Pasnak","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research has shown that children classify most easily at the basic level where objects in the same category look similar enough to each other to be grouped together but are distinct enough from objects in other categories to be discriminated (e.g., animal/bird/duck). In this article, the authors report on 2 experiments they conducted to determine whether children maintain this basic category bias when the perceptual similarity of stimuli at different hierarchical levels is equalized. Pictures within and across 3 hierarchical levels were made perceptually equivalent and shown to 71 Latino children who were bilingual in Spanish and English. In Experiment 1, the pictures used as exemplars could be categorized on any of the 3 hierarchical levels. In Experiment 2, example pictures unambiguously defined the level of categorization that would be accurate, and linguistic cues were given that might assist in the selection of the correct category. In both experiments, the children sorted pictures from all 3 levels equally well, but they found it harder to justify their sorting of superordinate pictures. English competence predicted sorting on the more ambiguous sorting task in Experiment 1; and English competence predicted verbal justifications in both experiments, even though the experiments were conducted in Spanish. Competence in Spanish or English was an equally good predictor of sorting in the better defined sorting task in Experiment 2. These findings indicate that a superordinate level deficiency remains after perceptual differences are eliminated and that the deficiency is cognitive in nature. Differences in the performances of children who differed in bilingualism support the hypothesis that a threshold of proficiency in both languages is an important determinant of the effect of bilingualism on categorization.</p>","PeriodicalId":77145,"journal":{"name":"Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs","volume":"128 4","pages":"409-41"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"22317488","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Categories and continua: a review of taxometric research. 分类与连续:分类计量研究综述。
Nick Haslam, Helen C Kim

The taxometric procedures developed by Paul Meehl and his colleagues have been used in a large body of research seeking to distinguish between categorical and continuous models of latent variables. In this article the authors survey taxometric studies of psychopathological, personality, and other variables and draw conclusions about the taxonicity of latent variables in these domains. In addition, the authors review research on the validation and refinement of taxometric methods and make proposals for improving the application of taxometric research. The authors consider questions that remain about the conceptual status of taxometrics and raise new ones. They show that taxometric methodology has made an accelerating number of contributions to psychological research, has resolved several longstanding controversies, and has challenged some entrenched theoretical assumptions in differential psychology. Moreover, they contend that the research possibilities that it affords have yet to be fully exploited.

由Paul Meehl和他的同事开发的分类方法已经在大量的研究中得到应用,这些研究试图区分潜在变量的分类模型和连续模型。本文综述了心理病理、人格和其他变量的分类计量学研究,并得出了这些领域潜在变量的分类性的结论。此外,作者还对分类方法的验证和改进进行了综述,并提出了改进分类研究应用的建议。作者考虑了关于分类计量学概念地位的问题,并提出了新的问题。这些研究表明,分类方法为心理学研究做出了越来越多的贡献,解决了几个长期存在的争议,并挑战了差异心理学中一些根深蒂固的理论假设。此外,他们认为,它所提供的研究可能性尚未得到充分利用。
{"title":"Categories and continua: a review of taxometric research.","authors":"Nick Haslam,&nbsp;Helen C Kim","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The taxometric procedures developed by Paul Meehl and his colleagues have been used in a large body of research seeking to distinguish between categorical and continuous models of latent variables. In this article the authors survey taxometric studies of psychopathological, personality, and other variables and draw conclusions about the taxonicity of latent variables in these domains. In addition, the authors review research on the validation and refinement of taxometric methods and make proposals for improving the application of taxometric research. The authors consider questions that remain about the conceptual status of taxometrics and raise new ones. They show that taxometric methodology has made an accelerating number of contributions to psychological research, has resolved several longstanding controversies, and has challenged some entrenched theoretical assumptions in differential psychology. Moreover, they contend that the research possibilities that it affords have yet to be fully exploited.</p>","PeriodicalId":77145,"journal":{"name":"Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs","volume":"128 3","pages":"271-320"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"22084769","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Creative style, personality, and artistic endeavor. 有创造性的风格、个性和艺术上的努力。
Garry A Gelade

Research has shown that creative style, as measured by the Kirton Adaption-Innovation Inventory (KAI; M. J. Kirton, 1976), is correlated with more than 30 different personality traits. In this article, the author demonstrates that many of these correlations can be understood within the framework of the Five-Factor Model of personality and shows that the predominant correlates of creative style are personality indicators in the domains of the factors Conscientiousness, Openness to Experience, and, to a lesser extent, Extraversion. These findings provide a basis for comparing the personality traits associated with creative style and occupational creativity. High scorers on the KAI (innovators) differ from both average and creative scientists but have personality characteristics similar to those of artists. This finding suggests that the artistic personality may be more common than is generally supposed and that common factors might underlie both artistic endeavor and creative style.

研究表明,通过柯顿适应创新量表(KAI;M. J. Kirton, 1976),与30多种不同的人格特质相关。在这篇文章中,作者证明了许多这些相关性可以在人格五因素模型的框架内被理解,并表明创造性风格的主要相关因素是尽责性,开放性,以及较小程度上的外向性等因素领域的人格指标。这些发现为比较与创造风格和职业创造力相关的人格特征提供了基础。KAI(创新者)的高分者不同于普通科学家和创造性科学家,但他们的个性特征与艺术家相似。这一发现表明,艺术个性可能比通常认为的更普遍,共同的因素可能是艺术努力和创作风格的基础。
{"title":"Creative style, personality, and artistic endeavor.","authors":"Garry A Gelade","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research has shown that creative style, as measured by the Kirton Adaption-Innovation Inventory (KAI; M. J. Kirton, 1976), is correlated with more than 30 different personality traits. In this article, the author demonstrates that many of these correlations can be understood within the framework of the Five-Factor Model of personality and shows that the predominant correlates of creative style are personality indicators in the domains of the factors Conscientiousness, Openness to Experience, and, to a lesser extent, Extraversion. These findings provide a basis for comparing the personality traits associated with creative style and occupational creativity. High scorers on the KAI (innovators) differ from both average and creative scientists but have personality characteristics similar to those of artists. This finding suggests that the artistic personality may be more common than is generally supposed and that common factors might underlie both artistic endeavor and creative style.</p>","PeriodicalId":77145,"journal":{"name":"Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs","volume":"128 3","pages":"213-34"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"22084767","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The two paradigms of persistence. 坚持的两个范例。
David J Pittenger

Persistence refers to the extent to which an individual pursues reinforcement that is no longer available. The most common generalization regarding persistence is the partial reinforcement extinction effect, which states that partial, rather than continuous, reinforcement creates the greatest level of persistence. Although the partial reinforcement effect is the most common effect in humans, exceptions exist, namely the generalized and the reversed partial reinforcement effect. Since the 1930s, psychologists have used 2 general paradigms for studying persistence in humans: the experimental paradigm and the cognitive/individual differences paradigm. For the experimental paradigm, the primary independent variable is the schedule of reinforcement used to establish the behavior prior to the removal of reinforcement. Explanations of persistence from the experimental perspective depend on associative principles derived from various theories of learning. By contrast, the cognitive/individual differences paradigm treats persistence as a function of trait variables, including locus of control and self-esteem, or general cognitive processes, such as cognitive dissonance or social cognition. In this article, the author reviews the status of the current literature on persistence and recommends directions for future research.

持久性是指个体追求不再可用的强化的程度。关于持久性,最常见的概括是部分强化消退效应,即部分强化而不是持续强化创造了最大程度的持久性。虽然部分强化效应是人类最常见的效应,但也存在例外,即广义部分强化效应和反向部分强化效应。自20世纪30年代以来,心理学家使用了两种通用范式来研究人类的持久性:实验范式和认知/个体差异范式。在实验范式中,主要的自变量是用于在移除强化之前建立行为的强化时间表。从实验的角度解释持久性依赖于从各种学习理论中衍生出来的联想原理。相比之下,认知/个体差异范式将持久性视为特征变量(包括控制点和自尊)或一般认知过程(如认知失调或社会认知)的函数。本文对持久性的研究现状进行了综述,并对今后的研究方向进行了展望。
{"title":"The two paradigms of persistence.","authors":"David J Pittenger","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Persistence refers to the extent to which an individual pursues reinforcement that is no longer available. The most common generalization regarding persistence is the partial reinforcement extinction effect, which states that partial, rather than continuous, reinforcement creates the greatest level of persistence. Although the partial reinforcement effect is the most common effect in humans, exceptions exist, namely the generalized and the reversed partial reinforcement effect. Since the 1930s, psychologists have used 2 general paradigms for studying persistence in humans: the experimental paradigm and the cognitive/individual differences paradigm. For the experimental paradigm, the primary independent variable is the schedule of reinforcement used to establish the behavior prior to the removal of reinforcement. Explanations of persistence from the experimental perspective depend on associative principles derived from various theories of learning. By contrast, the cognitive/individual differences paradigm treats persistence as a function of trait variables, including locus of control and self-esteem, or general cognitive processes, such as cognitive dissonance or social cognition. In this article, the author reviews the status of the current literature on persistence and recommends directions for future research.</p>","PeriodicalId":77145,"journal":{"name":"Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs","volume":"128 3","pages":"237-68"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"22084768","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Organizational cynicism: bases and consequences. 组织玩世不恭:基础和后果。
R Abraham

Organizational cynicism is the belief that an organization lacks integrity, which, when coupled with a powerful negative emotional reaction, leads to disparaging and critical behavior. In this article, the author attempts to theoretically clarify the process by which five forms of cynicism develop in the workplace and to empirically relate them to affective outcomes. Societal, employee, and organizational change cynicisms may be attributed to psychological contract violations; work cynicism may be related to burnout; and person-role conflict and personality cynicism may be related to innate hostility. Empirically, personality cynicism emerged as the strongest predictor of organizational cynicism, adversely affecting all of the criteria. Other forms of cynicism had more selective effects. Organizational change cynicism induced job dissatisfaction and alienation, and employee cynicism affected organizational commitment. Societal cynicism actually increased both job satisfaction and commitment. Both personality and work cynicisms were related to organizational citizenship indirectly, through alienation. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.

组织玩世不恭是认为组织缺乏诚信,当加上强烈的负面情绪反应时,会导致贬低和批评行为。在这篇文章中,作者试图从理论上阐明五种形式的玩世不恭在工作场所发展的过程,并从经验上将它们与情感结果联系起来。社会、员工和组织变革玩世不恭可能归因于心理契约的违反;玩世不恭可能与职业倦怠有关;人-角色冲突和人格玩世不恭可能与先天敌意有关。从经验上看,人格玩世不恭是组织玩世不恭的最强预测因子,对所有标准都有不利影响。其他形式的玩世不恭有更多的选择性影响。组织变革犬儒主义诱发工作不满和疏离感,员工犬儒主义影响组织承诺。社会冷嘲热讽实际上增加了工作满意度和工作投入。个性玩世不恭和工作玩世不恭都通过异化与组织公民间接相关。讨论了理论和实践意义。
{"title":"Organizational cynicism: bases and consequences.","authors":"R Abraham","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Organizational cynicism is the belief that an organization lacks integrity, which, when coupled with a powerful negative emotional reaction, leads to disparaging and critical behavior. In this article, the author attempts to theoretically clarify the process by which five forms of cynicism develop in the workplace and to empirically relate them to affective outcomes. Societal, employee, and organizational change cynicisms may be attributed to psychological contract violations; work cynicism may be related to burnout; and person-role conflict and personality cynicism may be related to innate hostility. Empirically, personality cynicism emerged as the strongest predictor of organizational cynicism, adversely affecting all of the criteria. Other forms of cynicism had more selective effects. Organizational change cynicism induced job dissatisfaction and alienation, and employee cynicism affected organizational commitment. Societal cynicism actually increased both job satisfaction and commitment. Both personality and work cynicisms were related to organizational citizenship indirectly, through alienation. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":77145,"journal":{"name":"Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs","volume":"126 3","pages":"269-92"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2000-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"21788668","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The quality of experience in adolescents' daily lives: developmental perspectives. 青少年日常生活经验的质量:发展的观点。
A Delle Fave, M Bassi

The authors analyzed the pattern of experience fluctuation in adolescents' daily activities. Italian high school students (N = 120; 16-20 years of age) were tested with the experience sampling method, a technique based on on-line sampling of daily life and experience. A total of 4,794 forms were gathered and analyzed by means of a model for the study of experience fluctuations. Among daily activities, studying at home, doing classwork, watching television, and having structured leisure were selected as the focus of analysis on the basis of their frequency and meaning in the adolescents' lives. Results showed that (a) daily activities have unique experiential profiles, (b) engagement may be used as an index of long-term commitment to a given activity, (c) studying at home and doing classwork share this basic component and can foster behavioral development, (d) structured leisure can play an edifying role at the short-term level for a socially integrated transition to adulthood, and (e) watching television is associated with lack of goals and engagement and is a source of apathy. The results (a) shed light on the role of daily life experience in shaping individual development and (b) provide suggestions for educational and psychosocial intervention in adolescence.

作者分析了青少年日常活动中的经验波动模式。意大利高中生(N = 120;年龄在16-20岁之间),采用基于日常生活和经验在线抽样的经验抽样法进行测试。通过研究经验波动的模型,共收集和分析了4 794份表格。在日常活动中,根据其在青少年生活中的频率和意义,选择在家学习、做作业、看电视和有组织的休闲活动作为分析重点。结果表明:(a)日常活动具有独特的体验特征,(b)投入可以作为对特定活动长期投入的指标,(c)在家学习和做作业共享这一基本组成部分,可以促进行为发展,(d)有组织的休闲可以在短期水平上对社会整合过渡到成年起教育作用,(e)看电视与缺乏目标和投入有关,是冷漠的来源。研究结果(a)阐明了日常生活经历在塑造个体发展中的作用,(b)为青少年的教育和社会心理干预提供了建议。
{"title":"The quality of experience in adolescents' daily lives: developmental perspectives.","authors":"A Delle Fave,&nbsp;M Bassi","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The authors analyzed the pattern of experience fluctuation in adolescents' daily activities. Italian high school students (N = 120; 16-20 years of age) were tested with the experience sampling method, a technique based on on-line sampling of daily life and experience. A total of 4,794 forms were gathered and analyzed by means of a model for the study of experience fluctuations. Among daily activities, studying at home, doing classwork, watching television, and having structured leisure were selected as the focus of analysis on the basis of their frequency and meaning in the adolescents' lives. Results showed that (a) daily activities have unique experiential profiles, (b) engagement may be used as an index of long-term commitment to a given activity, (c) studying at home and doing classwork share this basic component and can foster behavioral development, (d) structured leisure can play an edifying role at the short-term level for a socially integrated transition to adulthood, and (e) watching television is associated with lack of goals and engagement and is a source of apathy. The results (a) shed light on the role of daily life experience in shaping individual development and (b) provide suggestions for educational and psychosocial intervention in adolescence.</p>","PeriodicalId":77145,"journal":{"name":"Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs","volume":"126 3","pages":"347-67"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2000-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"21788559","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Context effects and the (mal)adaptive nature of guilt and shame in children. 情境效应与儿童内疚和羞耻的(不良)适应性。
T J Ferguson, H Stegge, H L Eyre, R Vollmer, M Ashbaker

Symptoms of internalization were examined in relation to children's self-reports of three emotions in situations that were either ambiguous or unambiguous as to the child's responsibility for various standard violations. Children ranging in age from 6 to 13 years were drawn from elementary schools (61 boys, 79 girls, mean age = 8.7) and from a community mental health center (23 boys, 18 girls, mean age = 8.5) to which they had been referred for problems related to internalization or externalization. Shame proneness was consistently linked to internalizing symptoms across contexts. Guilt proneness, in response to ambiguous scenarios, was also associated with internalization, whereas pride responses were unrelated to symptoms. Few age- or gender-related differences were found. The results cast doubt on notions that self-conscious emotions, such as guilt, are necessarily adaptive or maladaptive. Systematic research is needed to understand which features of any emotion contribute to children's psychological adjustment.

内化症状与儿童自我报告的三种情绪有关,这些情绪在儿童对各种违反标准的责任不明确或不明确的情况下进行了检查。年龄在6岁至13岁之间的儿童分别来自小学(61名男孩,79名女孩,平均年龄= 8.7岁)和社区精神卫生中心(23名男孩,18名女孩,平均年龄= 8.5岁),他们因内化或外化问题被转介到该中心。羞耻倾向始终与各种情况下的内化症状有关。在对模棱两可的情景作出反应时,内疚倾向也与内化有关,而骄傲反应与症状无关。几乎没有发现与年龄或性别相关的差异。研究结果对诸如内疚之类的自我意识情绪必然是适应的或不适应的观念提出了质疑。需要系统的研究来了解任何情绪的哪些特征有助于儿童的心理适应。
{"title":"Context effects and the (mal)adaptive nature of guilt and shame in children.","authors":"T J Ferguson,&nbsp;H Stegge,&nbsp;H L Eyre,&nbsp;R Vollmer,&nbsp;M Ashbaker","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Symptoms of internalization were examined in relation to children's self-reports of three emotions in situations that were either ambiguous or unambiguous as to the child's responsibility for various standard violations. Children ranging in age from 6 to 13 years were drawn from elementary schools (61 boys, 79 girls, mean age = 8.7) and from a community mental health center (23 boys, 18 girls, mean age = 8.5) to which they had been referred for problems related to internalization or externalization. Shame proneness was consistently linked to internalizing symptoms across contexts. Guilt proneness, in response to ambiguous scenarios, was also associated with internalization, whereas pride responses were unrelated to symptoms. Few age- or gender-related differences were found. The results cast doubt on notions that self-conscious emotions, such as guilt, are necessarily adaptive or maladaptive. Systematic research is needed to understand which features of any emotion contribute to children's psychological adjustment.</p>","PeriodicalId":77145,"journal":{"name":"Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs","volume":"126 3","pages":"319-45"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2000-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"21788670","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1