首页 > 最新文献

Applied Neuropsychology: Child最新文献

英文 中文
Early neurobehavioral outcomes in infants with suspected abusive head trauma: Performance across and relationship between measures. 疑似头部外伤婴儿的早期神经行为结果:各测量指标之间的表现和关系。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-28 DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2023.2206030
Ashley Fournier-Goodnight, Haley Bednarz Holm

Early neurobehavioral outcomes among infants with abusive head trauma (AHT) have not been well characterized. Though there are standardized measures for assessing infants, the ability of these measures to detect deficits may be limited. The Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU) Network Neurobehavioral Scale, Second Edition (NNNS-II) has been correlated with neurobehavioral outcomes as early as birth but has not been used with this clinical group. There is no strong evidence of the concurrent validity of this measure. The primary goal was to examine the concurrent validity of the NNNS-II in patients with suspected AHT by comparing it to the Mullen Scales of Early Learning, American Guidance Service (AGS) Edition (Mullen). A secondary goal was to characterize early neurobehavioral outcomes among infants with suspected AHT across two measures. This retrospective study included 11 infants who sustained a traumatic brain injury (TBI) around 40 days of age. The sample's performance was variable and ranged from average to below average across measures. Participants experienced the most difficulty with visuospatial processing, attentional abilities, physiologic regulation, and asymmetric reflexes, and data suggested the NNNS-II may be more sensitive to deficits. There was evidence of concurrent validity of the NNNS-II based on strong to moderate correlations with the Mullen. Use of the NNNS-II shortly after the injury is more likely to showcase deficits, which may increase the likelihood that patients receive early intervention. Establishing concurrent validity of the NNNS-II further contributes to the evidence base regarding its criterion related validity, which may promote its more regular use.

虐待性头部创伤(AHT)婴儿的早期神经行为结果尚未得到很好的表征。虽然有标准的措施来评估婴儿,这些措施的能力发现缺陷可能是有限的。新生儿重症监护病房(NICU)网络神经行为量表,第二版(NNNS-II)早在出生时就与神经行为结果相关,但尚未用于该临床组。没有强有力的证据表明这一措施同时有效。本研究的主要目的是通过将NNNS-II与美国指导服务(AGS)版马伦早期学习量表(Mullen)进行比较,来检验疑似AHT患者的NNNS-II的并发效度。第二个目标是通过两种测量方法来描述疑似AHT婴儿的早期神经行为结果。本回顾性研究纳入了11例40天大的创伤性脑损伤(TBI)婴儿。样本的表现是可变的,从平均水平到低于平均水平不等。参与者在视觉空间处理、注意力能力、生理调节和不对称反射方面经历了最大的困难,数据表明NNNS-II可能对缺陷更敏感。有证据表明,NNNS-II与Mullen量表有很强到中等程度的相关性。在损伤后不久使用NNNS-II更有可能显示出缺陷,这可能增加患者接受早期干预的可能性。建立NNNS-II的并发效度有助于进一步完善其效度相关的证据基础,促进其更规范的使用。
{"title":"Early neurobehavioral outcomes in infants with suspected abusive head trauma: Performance across and relationship between measures.","authors":"Ashley Fournier-Goodnight, Haley Bednarz Holm","doi":"10.1080/21622965.2023.2206030","DOIUrl":"10.1080/21622965.2023.2206030","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Early neurobehavioral outcomes among infants with abusive head trauma (AHT) have not been well characterized. Though there are standardized measures for assessing infants, the ability of these measures to detect deficits may be limited. The Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU) Network Neurobehavioral Scale, Second Edition (NNNS-II) has been correlated with neurobehavioral outcomes as early as birth but has not been used with this clinical group. There is no strong evidence of the concurrent validity of this measure. The primary goal was to examine the concurrent validity of the NNNS-II in patients with suspected AHT by comparing it to the Mullen Scales of Early Learning, American Guidance Service (AGS) Edition (Mullen). A secondary goal was to characterize early neurobehavioral outcomes among infants with suspected AHT across two measures. This retrospective study included 11 infants who sustained a traumatic brain injury (TBI) around 40 days of age. The sample's performance was variable and ranged from average to below average across measures. Participants experienced the most difficulty with visuospatial processing, attentional abilities, physiologic regulation, and asymmetric reflexes, and data suggested the NNNS-II may be more sensitive to deficits. There was evidence of concurrent validity of the NNNS-II based on strong to moderate correlations with the Mullen. Use of the NNNS-II shortly after the injury is more likely to showcase deficits, which may increase the likelihood that patients receive early intervention. Establishing concurrent validity of the NNNS-II further contributes to the evidence base regarding its criterion related validity, which may promote its more regular use.</p>","PeriodicalId":8047,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology: Child","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9417485","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Improved attentional abilities after playing five weeks of active video games in children with and without developmental coordination disorder. 有或没有发育性协调障碍的儿童在玩五周活跃的电子游戏后,注意力能力得到改善。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-21 DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2023.2190024
Dorothee Jelsma, Tatiane Targino Gomes Draghi, Jorge Cavalcante Neto, Bouwien Smits-Engelsman

Background: Besides motor coordination problems, attentional impairments are reported in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD), but the connection or trainability is still unclear.

Aim: To test changes on attentional abilities after playing active video games (AVG) in children with DCD and their peers (TD), to evaluate near transfer and the relationship between omissions in attentional and motor tests.

Methods: Seventy children (35 DCD; 35 TD), 7-12 years old, were assessed on three types of attention: distractibility (DIS); divided-attention (DA); sustained-attention (SA) and on the Wii Fit test pre- and post-training.

Results: A significant decrease of errors in attentional tasks was found after training, independent of group (TD/DCD) and console (Wii Fit/Xbox) with medium-strong effect sizes (DIS η2p =.42; DA η2p =.51; SA η2p =.41). The groups responded similarly to the training but the DCD children scored poorer on the DA. A positive transfer-effect to the non-trained Wii Fit test was found in accuracy (missed gates), while speed proved console-specific. A medium/strong relation was found between omissions in attentional tasks and the missed gates (Wii Fit test).

Conclusion: More accurate responses on attentional tasks were found after AVG-training in both groups. A clear relationship between attentional abilities and motor performance was found.

背景:除了运动协调问题外,儿童发育协调障碍(DCD)也有注意障碍的报道,但两者之间的联系或可训练性尚不清楚。目的:观察动态视频游戏(AVG)对DCD患儿和同龄儿童(TD)注意力能力的影响,评价近转移及注意和运动测试中遗漏的关系。方法:70例儿童(35 DCD;对7-12岁儿童(35 TD)的三种注意类型进行评估:注意力分散(DIS);注意分配(DA);持续注意力(SA)和训练前后Wii Fit测试。结果:训练后注意任务错误显著减少,与组(TD/DCD)和主机(Wii Fit/Xbox)无关,具有中等强效应(DIS η2p = 0.42;DA η2p = 0.51;SA η2p = 0.41)。各组对训练的反应相似,但DCD组的孩子在DA上得分较低。未经训练的Wii Fit测试在准确性(漏门)上发现了积极的转移效应,而速度则被证明是主机特有的。在注意力任务的遗漏与遗漏门之间发现了中等/强的关系(Wii Fit测试)。结论:avg训练后两组在注意任务上的反应更准确。注意能力和运动表现之间有明显的关系。
{"title":"Improved attentional abilities after playing five weeks of active video games in children with and without developmental coordination disorder.","authors":"Dorothee Jelsma, Tatiane Targino Gomes Draghi, Jorge Cavalcante Neto, Bouwien Smits-Engelsman","doi":"10.1080/21622965.2023.2190024","DOIUrl":"10.1080/21622965.2023.2190024","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Besides motor coordination problems, attentional impairments are reported in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD), but the connection or trainability is still unclear.</p><p><strong>Aim: </strong>To test changes on attentional abilities after playing active video games (AVG) in children with DCD and their peers (TD), to evaluate near transfer and the relationship between omissions in attentional and motor tests.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Seventy children (35 DCD; 35 TD), 7-12 years old, were assessed on three types of attention: distractibility (DIS); divided-attention (DA); sustained-attention (SA) and on the Wii Fit test pre- and post-training.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>A significant decrease of errors in attentional tasks was found after training, independent of group (TD/DCD) and console (Wii Fit/Xbox) with medium-strong effect sizes (DIS <i>η<sup>2</sup><sub>p</sub></i> =.42; DA <i>η<sup>2</sup><sub>p</sub></i> =.51; SA <i>η<sup>2</sup><sub>p</sub></i> =.41). The groups responded similarly to the training but the DCD children scored poorer on the DA. A positive transfer-effect to the non-trained Wii Fit test was found in accuracy (missed gates), while speed proved console-specific. A medium/strong relation was found between omissions in attentional tasks and the missed gates (Wii Fit test).</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>More accurate responses on attentional tasks were found after AVG-training in both groups. A clear relationship between attentional abilities and motor performance was found.</p>","PeriodicalId":8047,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology: Child","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9500468","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychometric properties of the Children's Time Awareness Questionnaire (CTAQ): A study on the validity of a Dutch 20-item questionnaire measuring time awareness in children. 儿童时间意识问卷(CTAQ)的心理测量特性:荷兰 20 项儿童时间意识调查问卷的有效性研究。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-02-20 DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2023.2177855
Raisy B W Timmerman, Christine Resch, Petra M Hurks, Renske Wassenberg, Jos G M Hendriksen

This study investigates the validity of Children's Time Awareness Questionnaire (CTAQ), a 20-item task for assessing children's time awareness. The CTAQ was administered to a group of typically developing children (n = 107) and children with any developmental problems reported by parents (non-typically developing children, n = 28), aged 4-8 years old. We found some support for a one-factor structure (EFA), yet the explained variance is relatively low (21%). Our proposed structure of two additional subscales, i.e., "time words" and "time estimation," was not supported by (confirmatory and exploratory) factor analyses. In contrast, exploratory factor analyses (EFA) indicated a six-factor structure, which needs further investigation. We found low, yet non-significant correlations between CTAQ scales and caregiver reports on children's time awareness, planning and impulsivity, and no significant correlations between CTAQ scales and scores on cognitive performance tasks. As expected, we found that older children have higher CTAQ scores than younger children. Non typically developing children had lower scores on CTAQ scales, compared to typically developing children. The CTAQ has sufficient internal consistency. The CTAQ has potential to measure time awareness, future research is indicated to further develop the CTAQ and enhance clinical applicability.

本研究调查了儿童时间意识问卷(CTAQ)的有效性,该问卷由 20 个项目组成,用于评估儿童的时间意识。CTAQ 的施测对象是一组 4-8 岁的发育典型儿童(107 人)和由家长报告有任何发育问题的儿童(非发育典型儿童,28 人)。我们发现单因素结构(EFA)得到了一定的支持,但解释方差相对较低(21%)。我们提出的另外两个子量表结构,即 "时间词 "和 "时间估计",并没有得到(确认性和探索性)因素分析的支持。与此相反,探索性因子分析(EFA)显示了一个六因子结构,需要进一步研究。我们发现,CTAQ 量表与看护人关于儿童时间意识、计划性和冲动性的报告之间的相关性较低,但并不显著,而 CTAQ 量表与认知表现任务得分之间的相关性并不显著。不出所料,我们发现年龄较大的儿童的 CTAQ 分数高于年龄较小的儿童。与典型发育儿童相比,非典型发育儿童的 CTAQ 量表得分较低。CTAQ 具有足够的内部一致性。CTAQ 具有测量时间意识的潜力,未来的研究将进一步开发 CTAQ 并提高其临床适用性。
{"title":"Psychometric properties of the Children's Time Awareness Questionnaire (CTAQ): A study on the validity of a Dutch 20-item questionnaire measuring time awareness in children.","authors":"Raisy B W Timmerman, Christine Resch, Petra M Hurks, Renske Wassenberg, Jos G M Hendriksen","doi":"10.1080/21622965.2023.2177855","DOIUrl":"10.1080/21622965.2023.2177855","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study investigates the validity of Children's Time Awareness Questionnaire (CTAQ), a 20-item task for assessing children's time awareness. The CTAQ was administered to a group of typically developing children (<i>n</i> = 107) and children with any developmental problems reported by parents (non-typically developing children, <i>n</i> = 28), aged 4-8 years old. We found some support for a one-factor structure (EFA), yet the explained variance is relatively low (21%). Our proposed structure of two additional subscales, i.e., \"time words\" and \"time estimation,\" was not supported by (confirmatory and exploratory) factor analyses. In contrast, exploratory factor analyses (EFA) indicated a six-factor structure, which needs further investigation. We found low, yet non-significant correlations between CTAQ scales and caregiver reports on children's time awareness, planning and impulsivity, and no significant correlations between CTAQ scales and scores on cognitive performance tasks. As expected, we found that older children have higher CTAQ scores than younger children. Non typically developing children had lower scores on CTAQ scales, compared to typically developing children. The CTAQ has sufficient internal consistency. The CTAQ has potential to measure time awareness, future research is indicated to further develop the CTAQ and enhance clinical applicability.</p>","PeriodicalId":8047,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology: Child","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10740769","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive performance in preschoolers with non-syndromic craniosynostosis undergoing surgery: A comparison with typically developing children. 接受手术的非综合征颅脑发育不良学龄前儿童的认知表现:与发育正常儿童的比较。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-02-25 DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2023.2183476
Julieta Moreno-Villagómez, Guillermina Yáñez-Téllez, Belén Prieto-Corona, Ana Natalia Seubert-Ravelo, Antonio García, Elizabeth Hernández-Echeagaray

This study aimed to determine the cognitive profile of preschool children undergoing surgery to correct non-syndromic craniosynostosis, compare them with typically developing children, and analyze possible cognitive deficits in the most prevalent subtypes: sagittal and unicoronal. Thirty-one children aged 3 years to 5 years and 11 months with non-syndromic craniosynostosis (11 sagittal, 9 unicoronal, 4 metopic, 3 lambdoid, 4 multisutural) who underwent surgery were compared with thirty-one typically developing children. The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Third Edition (WPPSI-III) was used to assess cognitive function. Children with non-syndromic craniosynostosis scored below the typically developing children in the Verbal Intelligence Quotient (VIQ) and Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient (FISQ). When specific subtypes were compared, children with sagittal synostosis scored similarly to the typically developing children; in contrast, children with unicoronal synostosis had lower performance in the Processing Speed Quotient and FISQ. The proportion of participants scoring below one standard deviation on the VIQ, General Language Composite, and FISQ was greater in the non-syndromic craniosynostosis group. This study supports the finding that children with non-syndromic craniosynostosis, particularly those with unicoronal synostosis, have more cognitive difficulties than those with normal development. Assessing cognition at preschool age in children with non-syndromic craniosynostosis is important in order to detect difficulties before they become more apparent at school age.

本研究旨在确定接受非颅骨发育不良矫正手术的学龄前儿童的认知概况,将他们与发育正常的儿童进行比较,并分析最常见亚型(矢状颅骨发育不良和单角颅骨发育不良)中可能存在的认知缺陷。研究人员将 31 名年龄在 3 岁至 5 岁 11 个月之间、患有非综合征颅骨发育不良(11 名矢状、9 名单冠、4 名偏位、3 名羊角、4 名多utural)并接受了手术的儿童与 31 名发育正常的儿童进行了比较。韦氏学前和小学智能量表-第三版(WPPSI-III)用于评估认知功能。非综合征颅脑发育不良儿童的言语智商(VIQ)和全面智商(FISQ)得分低于发育正常的儿童。在对特定亚型进行比较时,矢状突触症儿童的得分与发育正常儿童相似;相反,单冠突触症儿童的处理速度商数和全量表智商(FISQ)得分较低。在VIQ、普通语言综合和FISQ中,得分低于一个标准差的参与者比例在非颅骨发育不良组中更高。这项研究证实,患有非综合畸形颅骨发育不良症的儿童,尤其是患有单冠合体症的儿童,比发育正常的儿童有更多的认知困难。在学龄前对患有非颅骨突触症的儿童进行认知能力评估非常重要,以便在学龄前发现他们的认知困难。
{"title":"Cognitive performance in preschoolers with non-syndromic craniosynostosis undergoing surgery: A comparison with typically developing children.","authors":"Julieta Moreno-Villagómez, Guillermina Yáñez-Téllez, Belén Prieto-Corona, Ana Natalia Seubert-Ravelo, Antonio García, Elizabeth Hernández-Echeagaray","doi":"10.1080/21622965.2023.2183476","DOIUrl":"10.1080/21622965.2023.2183476","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study aimed to determine the cognitive profile of preschool children undergoing surgery to correct non-syndromic craniosynostosis, compare them with typically developing children, and analyze possible cognitive deficits in the most prevalent subtypes: sagittal and unicoronal. Thirty-one children aged 3 years to 5 years and 11 months with non-syndromic craniosynostosis (11 sagittal, 9 unicoronal, 4 metopic, 3 lambdoid, 4 multisutural) who underwent surgery were compared with thirty-one typically developing children. The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Third Edition (WPPSI-III) was used to assess cognitive function. Children with non-syndromic craniosynostosis scored below the typically developing children in the Verbal Intelligence Quotient (VIQ) and Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient (FISQ). When specific subtypes were compared, children with sagittal synostosis scored similarly to the typically developing children; in contrast, children with unicoronal synostosis had lower performance in the Processing Speed Quotient and FISQ. The proportion of participants scoring below one standard deviation on the VIQ, General Language Composite, and FISQ was greater in the non-syndromic craniosynostosis group. This study supports the finding that children with non-syndromic craniosynostosis, particularly those with unicoronal synostosis, have more cognitive difficulties than those with normal development. Assessing cognition at preschool age in children with non-syndromic craniosynostosis is important in order to detect difficulties before they become more apparent at school age.</p>","PeriodicalId":8047,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology: Child","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10830311","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of brain-based instruction on executive function and habits of mind among young children at-risk for learning disabilities. 基于大脑的教学对有学习障碍风险的幼儿的执行功能和思维习惯的影响。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-12 DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2022.2161904
Loul Saleh Al Rasheed, Ali Abdelnabbi Mohamed Hanafy

Purpose: The aim of this study was to investigate the effects of brain-based instruction on executive function and habits of mind among young children at-risk for learning disabilities.

Method: This study employed a quantitative approach using a quasi-experimental, pretest, post-test, and follow-up design. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), repeated-measures, pre-, post-, and follow-up testing design was used. Total number of participants were 40 children: treatment group (n = 20, 8 males, 12 females) and control group (n = 20, 9 males, 11 females). Accordingly, 40 students were chosen from four kindergartens in Qassim, Saudi Arabia. The sampling methodology employed used convenience sampling.

Results: The results found that using brain-based instruction to improve functions and habits of mind indicate the effectiveness of this method for children at-risk with learning disabilities.

Conclusions: Using multisensory instructional techniques to actively engage students presents a channel for magnifying student learning experiences. Brain-based learning is a constructivism teaching method where in the brain constructing learning process finds and structures knowledge by means of association with previous experiences and knowledge.

目的:本研究旨在调查基于大脑的教学对有学习障碍风险的幼儿的执行功能和思维习惯的影响:本研究采用准实验、前测、后测和跟踪设计的定量方法。采用方差分析(ANCOVA)、重复测量、前测、后测和跟踪测试设计。参加者共 40 名儿童:治疗组(n = 20,8 名男生,12 名女生)和对照组(n = 20,9 名男生,11 名女生)。因此,40 名学生选自沙特阿拉伯卡西姆的四所幼儿园。采用的抽样方法是便利抽样:结果发现,使用基于大脑的教学方法来改善功能和思维习惯,表明这种方法对有学习障碍的高危儿童很有效:结论:使用多感官教学技术让学生积极参与,是放大学生学习体验的一个渠道。基于大脑的学习是一种建构主义教学方法,在大脑建构学习过程中,通过与以往经验和知识的联想来发现和构建知识。
{"title":"Effects of brain-based instruction on executive function and habits of mind among young children at-risk for learning disabilities.","authors":"Loul Saleh Al Rasheed, Ali Abdelnabbi Mohamed Hanafy","doi":"10.1080/21622965.2022.2161904","DOIUrl":"10.1080/21622965.2022.2161904","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Purpose: </strong>The aim of this study was to investigate the effects of brain-based instruction on executive function and habits of mind among young children at-risk for learning disabilities.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>This study employed a quantitative approach using a quasi-experimental, pretest, post-test, and follow-up design. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), repeated-measures, pre-, post-, and follow-up testing design was used. Total number of participants were 40 children: treatment group (<i>n</i> = 20, 8 males, 12 females) and control group (<i>n</i> = 20, 9 males, 11 females). Accordingly, 40 students were chosen from four kindergartens in Qassim, Saudi Arabia. The sampling methodology employed used convenience sampling.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The results found that using brain-based instruction to improve functions and habits of mind indicate the effectiveness of this method for children at-risk with learning disabilities.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Using multisensory instructional techniques to actively engage students presents a channel for magnifying student learning experiences. Brain-based learning is a constructivism teaching method where in the brain constructing learning process finds and structures knowledge by means of association with previous experiences and knowledge.</p>","PeriodicalId":8047,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology: Child","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9088755","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effectiveness of a mindfulness-based intervention on improving executive functions and reducing the symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in young children. 正念干预对改善幼儿执行功能和减少注意缺陷多动障碍症状的有效性。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-27 DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2023.2203321
Nawal Ahmed Aboalola

The aim was to investigate the effectiveness of a mindfulness-based intervention on improving executive functions and reducing the symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in young children. This study employed a quantitative approach using a quasi-experimental, pretest and post-test and follow up design. 56 children between 5 and 7 years of age(M = 6.9, SD= 1.7) with ADHD or with symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity are being recruited to participate in the mindfulness-based intervention. To determine whether there are any significant differences between the two independent (unrelated) groups on executive functions and symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, pre-post-follow up test and multiple comparisons are used. Pretest assessments were conducted for both the control and training groups. Following the pretest, a four-weeks of training commenced for the intervention group only. T-test results for the differences in post- test mean scores between the two groups in EF and in ADHD symptoms showed that the training group outperformed the control group. There were statistical differences in EF and in ADHD symptoms between pre and post measures in favor of post test, and between pre and follow up measures in favor of follow up test, but no statistical differences between post and follow up test. This study demonstrates that it is possible to enhance EF skills using mindfulness-based intervention in which young with ADHD are engaged in a series of group play based activities.

目的是调查基于正念的干预在改善幼儿执行功能和减少注意缺陷多动障碍症状方面的有效性。本研究采用准实验、前测、后测和随访设计的定量方法。56名5 - 7岁的儿童(M = 6.9, SD= 1.7)患有多动症或有注意力不集中和多动症状,他们被招募参加以正念为基础的干预。为了确定两个独立(不相关)组在执行功能和注意缺陷多动障碍症状上是否存在显著差异,采用随访前-随访后测试和多重比较。对对照组和训练组进行了测试前评估。在预测之后,干预组开始了为期四周的训练。两组在EF和ADHD症状的测试后均分差异的t检验结果显示,训练组优于对照组。在EF和ADHD症状方面,前后测量有利于后测,前后测量有利于随访测试,而前后测量有利于随访测试,但前后测试无统计学差异。这项研究表明,使用以正念为基础的干预是有可能提高EF技能的,在这种干预中,患有多动症的年轻人参与了一系列以小组游戏为基础的活动。
{"title":"The effectiveness of a mindfulness-based intervention on improving executive functions and reducing the symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in young children.","authors":"Nawal Ahmed Aboalola","doi":"10.1080/21622965.2023.2203321","DOIUrl":"10.1080/21622965.2023.2203321","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The aim was to investigate the effectiveness of a mindfulness-based intervention on improving executive functions and reducing the symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in young children. This study employed a quantitative approach using a quasi-experimental, pretest and post-test and follow up design. 56 children between 5 and 7 years of age(M = 6.9, SD= 1.7) with ADHD or with symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity are being recruited to participate in the mindfulness-based intervention. To determine whether there are any significant differences between the two independent (unrelated) groups on executive functions and symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, pre-post-follow up test and multiple comparisons are used. Pretest assessments were conducted for both the control and training groups. Following the pretest, a four-weeks of training commenced for the intervention group only. T-test results for the differences in post- test mean scores between the two groups in EF and in ADHD symptoms showed that the training group outperformed the control group. There were statistical differences in EF and in ADHD symptoms between pre and post measures in favor of post test, and between pre and follow up measures in favor of follow up test, but no statistical differences between post and follow up test. This study demonstrates that it is possible to enhance EF skills using mindfulness-based intervention in which young with ADHD are engaged in a series of group play based activities.</p>","PeriodicalId":8047,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology: Child","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9414280","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Processing speed in patients with pediatric cancer: Psychosocial considerations. 儿科癌症患者的处理速度:社会心理因素。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-14 DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2024.2403767
Jenna A Chiang, Paulina T Feghali, Ashley M Whitaker

Pediatric cancer treatments may contribute to slower processing, while cultural considerations (e.g., SES) can influence outcomes and tend to be disproportionately lower in racial/ethnic minorities. Given increased risk for certain cancers in Hispanic/Latine children and rising Spanish exposure in the United States, the purpose of this study was to examine differences in processing speed between cancer survivors based on household language exposure: English vs. mixed language (ML; i.e. monolingual Spanish-speaking or bilingual Spanish/English-speaking). 128 patients ages 8-21 with leukemia/lymphoma completed screening. As expected, SES was lower in patients from ML households based on parental education (U = 355.00, p<.001) and estimated household income (U = 1031.500, p<.001). Despite this, processing speed (assessed using the written and oral trials of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test; SDMT) was average (SDMT-W x̅=-.13, SDMT-O x̅=.32), with no significant differences between language groups (F(2,120)=0.966, p=.384). Post-hoc analyses revealed time since diagnosis did not predict performance on oral trial for either group or the whole sample, while poorer performance on written trial was noted among the English-only group when further from diagnosis (SDMT-W F(1,57)=7.829, p=.007). Stable ML group trajectory regardless of time since diagnosis may reflect resiliency among children with Spanish exposure.

儿科癌症治疗可能会导致处理速度减慢,而文化因素(如社会经济地位)则会影响治疗结果,而且在少数种族/族裔中,处理速度往往过低。鉴于西班牙裔/拉丁裔儿童罹患某些癌症的风险增加,以及在美国接触西班牙语的人数不断增加,本研究旨在根据家庭语言接触情况(英语与混合语言(ML;即单语西班牙语或双语西班牙语/英语))检查癌症幸存者处理速度的差异。128 名 8-21 岁的白血病/淋巴瘤患者完成了筛查。正如预期的那样,根据父母的教育程度,来自西班牙语家庭的患者的社会经济地位较低(U = 355.00,pU = 1031.500,pF(2,120)=0.966,p=.384)。事后分析表明,诊断后的时间并不能预测任何一组或整个样本的口试成绩,而在距离诊断时间更长的情况下,纯英语组的笔试成绩较差(SDMT-W F(1,57)=7.829, p=.007)。无论诊断时间长短,ML 组的轨迹都很稳定,这可能反映了接触西班牙语的儿童的恢复能力。
{"title":"Processing speed in patients with pediatric cancer: Psychosocial considerations.","authors":"Jenna A Chiang, Paulina T Feghali, Ashley M Whitaker","doi":"10.1080/21622965.2024.2403767","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2024.2403767","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Pediatric cancer treatments may contribute to slower processing, while cultural considerations (e.g., SES) can influence outcomes and tend to be disproportionately lower in racial/ethnic minorities. Given increased risk for certain cancers in Hispanic/Latine children and rising Spanish exposure in the United States, the purpose of this study was to examine differences in processing speed between cancer survivors based on household language exposure: English vs. mixed language (ML; i.e. monolingual Spanish-speaking or bilingual Spanish/English-speaking). 128 patients ages 8-21 with leukemia/lymphoma completed screening. As expected, SES was lower in patients from ML households based on parental education (<i>U</i> = 355.00, <i>p</i><.001) and estimated household income (<i>U</i> = 1031.500, <i>p</i><.001). Despite this, processing speed (assessed using the written and oral trials of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test; SDMT) was average (SDMT-W x̅=-.13, SDMT-O x̅=.32), with no significant differences between language groups (<i>F</i>(2,120)=0.966<i>, p</i>=.384). Post-hoc analyses revealed time since diagnosis did not predict performance on oral trial for either group or the whole sample, while poorer performance on written trial was noted among the English-only group when further from diagnosis (SDMT-W F(1,57)=7.829, <i>p</i>=.007). Stable ML group trajectory regardless of time since diagnosis may reflect resiliency among children with Spanish exposure.</p>","PeriodicalId":8047,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology: Child","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142279683","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evaluating the effect of cochlear implantation age on pragmatic abilities before and after age of 3. 评估 3 岁前后人工耳蜗植入年龄对实用能力的影响。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q4 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-13 DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2024.2403100
Pegah Nikrah,Rasool Ghareh Chahie,Atieh Ghazvini,Alemeh Hajizadeh
INTRODUCTIONThe cochlear implant (CI) is crucial in developing hearing, speech, language, and communication skills in children with profound hearing loss (HL). The study aimed to assess how the age at which children receive a CI affects the development of pragmatic abilities between the ages of 5 and 8 for those who received a CI before or after the age of 3.METHODSForty children with CI were evaluated. The children between the ages of 5 and 8 were divided into two groups who received CIs before or after age 3. The Persian version of the Children's Communication Checklist (CCC) was used to assess pragmatic abilities with a parent rating scale.RESULTSChildren implanted under 3 got higher scores in social relationships, using context, conversational rapport, syntax, and speech. Also, their performance in inappropriate and stereotyped conversations was better than those over 3 years. They have a significant difference in the pragmatic composite than those who received CI over 3 except for coherence and interest (p < 0.05).CONCLUSIONAccording to the results, children with CI under 3 performed better in pragmatic composites, which means they have better pragmatic abilities. These findings suggest that early CI is important. Of course, other individual and environmental factors must also be considered.
引言 人工耳蜗(CI)对重度听力损失(HL)儿童的听力、言语、语言和沟通能力的发展至关重要。本研究旨在评估儿童接受 CI 的年龄如何影响那些在 3 岁之前或之后接受 CI 的儿童在 5 至 8 岁期间语用能力的发展。这些 5 至 8 岁的儿童被分为两组,分别在 3 岁之前或之后接受 CI。结果 3 岁以下植入 CI 的儿童在社交关系、语境使用、对话默契、句法和言语方面得分更高。此外,他们在不恰当会话和刻板会话中的表现也优于 3 岁以上的儿童。除了连贯性和兴趣(P < 0.05)外,他们在语用综合能力方面与 3 岁以上接受 CI 的儿童相比有明显差异。这些结果表明,早期 CI 非常重要。当然,还必须考虑其他个人和环境因素。
{"title":"Evaluating the effect of cochlear implantation age on pragmatic abilities before and after age of 3.","authors":"Pegah Nikrah,Rasool Ghareh Chahie,Atieh Ghazvini,Alemeh Hajizadeh","doi":"10.1080/21622965.2024.2403100","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2024.2403100","url":null,"abstract":"INTRODUCTIONThe cochlear implant (CI) is crucial in developing hearing, speech, language, and communication skills in children with profound hearing loss (HL). The study aimed to assess how the age at which children receive a CI affects the development of pragmatic abilities between the ages of 5 and 8 for those who received a CI before or after the age of 3.METHODSForty children with CI were evaluated. The children between the ages of 5 and 8 were divided into two groups who received CIs before or after age 3. The Persian version of the Children's Communication Checklist (CCC) was used to assess pragmatic abilities with a parent rating scale.RESULTSChildren implanted under 3 got higher scores in social relationships, using context, conversational rapport, syntax, and speech. Also, their performance in inappropriate and stereotyped conversations was better than those over 3 years. They have a significant difference in the pragmatic composite than those who received CI over 3 except for coherence and interest (p < 0.05).CONCLUSIONAccording to the results, children with CI under 3 performed better in pragmatic composites, which means they have better pragmatic abilities. These findings suggest that early CI is important. Of course, other individual and environmental factors must also be considered.","PeriodicalId":8047,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology: Child","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142255405","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
LITMUS Turkish sentence repetition test: The best items, effect of scoring and diagnostic accuracy. LITMUS 土耳其语句子重复测试:最佳项目、评分效果和诊断准确性。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q4 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2024.2400483
Dilber Kaçar-Kütükçü,Seyhun Topbaş
PURPOSEThis study aimed to examine LITMUS Turkish Sentence Repetition Test's (LITMUS-TR) diagnostic accuracy, as well as the best scoring method and most distinguishing test items. We also sought to ascertain whether age has an impact on the sensitivity and specificity.METHODTwo hundred and fifty children with typical language development (TD) between the ages of 4 and 7, as well as 44 children with developmental language disorder (DLD), took part in the study. Data was collected using TODİL, LITMUS-TR, and the pediatric family interview form. LITMUS-TR was graded using four different methods.RESULTSThe performance of children with DLD in each score type was lower and the number of errors higher than those with TD. All items have excellent or acceptable item difficulty and discrimination values for binary scoring and total number of errors. LITMUS-TR's most distinctive items were complex structures with dependencies, such as syntactic movement and embedding. LITMUS-TR had high diagnostic accuracy for the whole test (0.887) and each scoring method. A separate analysis of each age group showed sensitivity and specificity above 0.80.CONCLUSIONSWhen employed as a supportive objective measure, LITMUS-TR was proven to be an effective diagnostic tool for DLD, with age influencing the diagnostic accuracy outcomes.
目的本研究旨在探讨 LITMUS 土耳其语句子重复测试(LITMUS-TR)的诊断准确性、最佳评分方法和最易区分的测试项目。方法250名年龄在4至7岁之间的典型语言发育(TD)儿童和44名发育性语言障碍(DLD)儿童参加了研究。研究使用TODİL、LITMUS-TR和儿科家庭访谈表收集数据。结果DLD患儿在每种评分类型中的表现均低于TD患儿,错误次数也高于TD患儿。在二元计分和错误总数方面,所有项目的项目难度和区分度值都很好或可以接受。LITMUS-TR 最具特色的项目是具有依赖关系的复杂结构,如句法移动和嵌入。LITMUS-TR 在整个测试(0.887)和每种计分方法上都具有很高的诊断准确性。结论LITMUS-TR作为一种辅助性客观测量方法,被证明是一种有效的DLD诊断工具,其诊断准确性结果受年龄的影响。
{"title":"LITMUS Turkish sentence repetition test: The best items, effect of scoring and diagnostic accuracy.","authors":"Dilber Kaçar-Kütükçü,Seyhun Topbaş","doi":"10.1080/21622965.2024.2400483","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2024.2400483","url":null,"abstract":"PURPOSEThis study aimed to examine LITMUS Turkish Sentence Repetition Test's (LITMUS-TR) diagnostic accuracy, as well as the best scoring method and most distinguishing test items. We also sought to ascertain whether age has an impact on the sensitivity and specificity.METHODTwo hundred and fifty children with typical language development (TD) between the ages of 4 and 7, as well as 44 children with developmental language disorder (DLD), took part in the study. Data was collected using TODİL, LITMUS-TR, and the pediatric family interview form. LITMUS-TR was graded using four different methods.RESULTSThe performance of children with DLD in each score type was lower and the number of errors higher than those with TD. All items have excellent or acceptable item difficulty and discrimination values for binary scoring and total number of errors. LITMUS-TR's most distinctive items were complex structures with dependencies, such as syntactic movement and embedding. LITMUS-TR had high diagnostic accuracy for the whole test (0.887) and each scoring method. A separate analysis of each age group showed sensitivity and specificity above 0.80.CONCLUSIONSWhen employed as a supportive objective measure, LITMUS-TR was proven to be an effective diagnostic tool for DLD, with age influencing the diagnostic accuracy outcomes.","PeriodicalId":8047,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology: Child","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142179899","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evaluating mental chronometry as a quantitative measure of information processing in early childhood autism. 将心理计时法作为幼儿自闭症患者信息处理的定量测量方法进行评估。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-02 DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2024.2394178
Yousif Ali Yaseen, Ahlam Muhammad Taher Saleem, Dindar S Bari, Rahma Tahseen Nayef, Haval Y Yacoob Aldosky

Objectives: Mental chronometry is the scientific study of cognitive processing speed measured by reaction time (RT), which is the elapsed time between the onset of a stimulus and an individual's response. This study aims at measuring the RT among young children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and comparing it with normal (typically developing) children.

Methods: 60 ASD children were selected from different ASD centers, and 60 normal children were selected from different kindergartens for participation in this study. Participants were aged 3-6 years old. The RT was measured using the Fitlight trainer device. The findings were statistically evaluated using independent t-tests and ANOVA tests.

Result: Significant differences (p < 0.0001) were found between both groups in all tasks, and ASD children demonstrated slower RT compared to the normal group. The RT measured through three senses (visual, auditory, and touch) for ASD and normal were 3.64 ± 2.16, 13.19 ± 2.41(trial), 1835.23 ± 757.95, 697.12 ± 87.83 (second), and 1550.89 ± 499.76, 752.67 ± 124.02 (second) respectively.

Conclusion: The evaluated RT showed significant impairment in RT among ASD in comparison to normal children and this was true for the three senses. The Fitlight trainer could be used to assess RT and stimulus-response among ASD children in various cognitive tasks. Similar studies, involving larger samples from different areas and involving other sense organs, are indicated to confirm the results.

目的:心理计时法是通过反应时间(RT)测量认知处理速度的科学研究,反应时间是指从刺激开始到个体做出反应之间所经过的时间。本研究旨在测量自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)幼儿的反应时间,并将其与正常(发育正常)儿童进行比较。参与者年龄为 3-6 岁。使用 Fitlight 训练器测量 RT。研究结果采用独立 t 检验和方差分析检验进行统计评估:差异显著(P评估结果显示,与正常儿童相比,ASD 儿童的 RT 明显受损,三种感官均如此。Fitlight 训练器可用于评估 ASD 儿童在各种认知任务中的实时反应和刺激-反应。类似的研究涉及不同领域的更大样本,并涉及其他感觉器官,以确认结果。
{"title":"Evaluating mental chronometry as a quantitative measure of information processing in early childhood autism.","authors":"Yousif Ali Yaseen, Ahlam Muhammad Taher Saleem, Dindar S Bari, Rahma Tahseen Nayef, Haval Y Yacoob Aldosky","doi":"10.1080/21622965.2024.2394178","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2024.2394178","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objectives: </strong>Mental chronometry is the scientific study of cognitive processing speed measured by reaction time (RT), which is the elapsed time between the onset of a stimulus and an individual's response. This study aims at measuring the RT among young children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and comparing it with normal (typically developing) children.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>60 ASD children were selected from different ASD centers, and 60 normal children were selected from different kindergartens for participation in this study. Participants were aged 3-6 years old. The RT was measured using the Fitlight trainer device. The findings were statistically evaluated using independent t-tests and ANOVA tests.</p><p><strong>Result: </strong>Significant differences (<i>p</i> < 0.0001) were found between both groups in all tasks, and ASD children demonstrated slower RT compared to the normal group. The RT measured through three senses (visual, auditory, and touch) for ASD and normal were 3.64 ± 2.16, 13.19 ± 2.41(trial), 1835.23 ± 757.95, 697.12 ± 87.83 (second), and 1550.89 ± 499.76, 752.67 ± 124.02 (second) respectively.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The evaluated RT showed significant impairment in RT among ASD in comparison to normal children and this was true for the three senses. The Fitlight trainer could be used to assess RT and stimulus-response among ASD children in various cognitive tasks. Similar studies, involving larger samples from different areas and involving other sense organs, are indicated to confirm the results.</p>","PeriodicalId":8047,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology: Child","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142103747","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Applied Neuropsychology: Child
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1