首页 > 最新文献

Applied Animal Behaviour Science最新文献

英文 中文
Identifying barriers to providing daily playtime for cats: A survey-based approach using COM-B analysis 识别为猫咪提供日常游戏时间的障碍:基于调查的 COM-B 分析法
IF 2.2 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-10-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106420
Mikel Delgado , Ellen Marcinkiewicz , Paul Rhodes , Sarah L.H. Ellis
Play behaviour is often considered a sign of good welfare in companion animals. To meet their pets’ environmental needs, owners are encouraged to provide opportunities for the expression of natural behaviours. Owners can provide cats with an outlet for hunting behaviour through interactive play, where they move a wand toy in a way that mimics prey. However, not all owners play this way regularly, or at all, with their cats. In this study, we implemented part of the Behaviour Change Wheel framework, specifically the COM-B model in relation to human behaviour change, via an online survey to assess barriers (in the areas of Capability, Opportunity, and Motivation) for owners playing with their cats with wand toys and to quantify and understand current owner-cat play behaviour. Forty-seven percent of owners reported playing with their cat regularly in a baseline survey of 1599 respondents from 47 countries. Using COM-B analysis, we identified several barriers to playing relating to capability, opportunity, and motivation. Specific examples included being too mentally or physically tired, forgetting to play with their cat, not having time due to work, not being able to make play a daily habit, life getting in the way and the perception that their cat seems to prefer other toys or activities to wand toys. Greater knowledge about play and identifying fewer barriers were associated with more frequent interactive wand toy play. Interventions to improve feline welfare should focus on helping cat owners overcome these identified capability, opportunity, and motivation barriers.
玩耍行为通常被认为是伴侣动物良好福利的标志。为了满足宠物对环境的需求,我们鼓励主人为宠物提供表现自然行为的机会。主人可以通过互动游戏为猫咪提供狩猎行为的出口,在互动游戏中,猫咪会以模仿猎物的方式移动魔杖玩具。然而,并不是所有的主人都经常或根本不与猫咪玩这种游戏。在这项研究中,我们通过在线调查实施了 "行为改变轮 "框架的一部分,特别是与人类行为改变相关的 COM-B 模型,以评估主人与猫咪玩魔杖玩具的障碍(能力、机会和动机方面),并量化和了解当前主人与猫咪的游戏行为。在对来自 47 个国家的 1599 名受访者进行的基线调查中,47% 的主人表示经常与猫咪玩耍。通过 COM-B 分析,我们发现了与能力、机会和动机有关的玩耍障碍。具体例子包括精神或身体过于疲惫、忘记与猫咪玩耍、因工作原因没有时间、无法将玩耍作为日常习惯、生活受到阻碍以及认为猫咪似乎更喜欢其他玩具或活动而不是魔杖玩具。对游戏的更多了解和发现更少的障碍与更频繁地玩互动魔杖玩具有关。改善猫科动物福利的干预措施应侧重于帮助猫主人克服这些已发现的能力、机会和动机障碍。
{"title":"Identifying barriers to providing daily playtime for cats: A survey-based approach using COM-B analysis","authors":"Mikel Delgado ,&nbsp;Ellen Marcinkiewicz ,&nbsp;Paul Rhodes ,&nbsp;Sarah L.H. Ellis","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106420","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106420","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Play behaviour is often considered a sign of good welfare in companion animals. To meet their pets’ environmental needs, owners are encouraged to provide opportunities for the expression of natural behaviours. Owners can provide cats with an outlet for hunting behaviour through interactive play, where they move a wand toy in a way that mimics prey. However, not all owners play this way regularly, or at all, with their cats. In this study, we implemented part of the Behaviour Change Wheel framework, specifically the COM-B model in relation to human behaviour change, via an online survey to assess barriers (in the areas of Capability, Opportunity, and Motivation) for owners playing with their cats with wand toys and to quantify and understand current owner-cat play behaviour. Forty-seven percent of owners reported playing with their cat regularly in a baseline survey of 1599 respondents from 47 countries. Using COM-B analysis, we identified several barriers to playing relating to capability, opportunity, and motivation. Specific examples included being too mentally or physically tired, forgetting to play with their cat, not having time due to work, not being able to make play a daily habit, life getting in the way and the perception that their cat seems to prefer other toys or activities to wand toys. Greater knowledge about play and identifying fewer barriers were associated with more frequent interactive wand toy play. Interventions to improve feline welfare should focus on helping cat owners overcome these identified capability, opportunity, and motivation barriers.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"280 ","pages":"Article 106420"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142441924","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Physiological and behavioral indicators of water buffaloes with access to heat mitigation resources in a grazing system during the hot season in the subtropics 在亚热带炎热季节的放牧系统中,可获得热量缓解资源的水牛的生理和行为指标
IF 2.2 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-10-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106409
Cindy Anne Klausberger Ximenes, Arthur Fernandes Bettencourt, Bruna Schmitz, Delane Ribas da Rosa, Juliany Ardenghi Guimarães, Betina da Cunha Borges, Caren Paludo Ghedini, Vivian Fischer
Water buffaloes are increasingly being raised in similar way to dairy cows, in grazing systems frequently deprived of shade and water for immersion. This study aimed to evaluate the effects of different heat mitigation resources, 1) only shade and 2) shade plus water for immersion, on the behavioral repertoire and physiological characteristics of female grazing buffaloes in the subtropics during the hot season. Twenty buffaloes (Bubalus bubalis), including calves, heifers and non-lactating cows were allocated throughout the hot season (from December 2021 to April 2022) in a grazing system with two different heat mitigation resources (treatments): only shade (SH) and shade plus water for immersion (SHW). Diurnal behavioral repertoire was recorded every 15 days, individually and focally, at every 5 minutes for 12 hours per day. Heart rate (HR; beats/min), respiration rate (RR; breaths/min), rectal temperature (RT; °C) and panting score were also measured. Buffaloes reduced grazing time especially with THI above 75. Buffaloes at SHW spent more time grazing and lying than SH buffaloes (P < 0.05). Cows and heifers had higher ruminating (P < 0.05) and standing time (P < 0.05) compared with calves. Physiological variables did not differ (P > 0.05) between treatments (SH vs. SHW) and were positively correlated with air temperature and THI (P < 0.05). HR was higher for heifers (59.6) and calves (63.5) than for cows (53.3; P < 0.05). RT tended to be higher for heifers (38.4) and calves (38.3) than for cows (38.1; 0.05 < P < 0.10). However, respiration rate did not differ across buffalo categories (P > 0.05). Panting behavior was not reported throughout the observation days (panting score = 0). When both heat mitigation resources, shade and water for immersion were available, buffaloes preferred water for immersion to mitigate the effects of heat stress. Regardless animal category, buffaloes were able to maintain physiological variables within the normal range, indicating that providing shade and/or water for immersion, as environmental enrichment resources for thermoregulation, is suitable for heat stress mitigation in water buffaloes throughout the hot season in the subtropics.
水牛越来越多地采用与奶牛类似的方式饲养,放牧系统经常缺少遮荫和浸泡用水。本研究旨在评估不同的防暑降温资源(1)仅遮荫和(2)遮荫加浸水对亚热带地区雌性放牧水牛在炎热季节的行为和生理特征的影响。在整个炎热季节(2021 年 12 月至 2022 年 4 月),20 头水牛(包括犊牛、小母牛和非哺乳母牛)被分配到一个放牧系统中,该系统有两种不同的热缓解资源(处理):仅遮荫(SH)和遮荫加浸水(SHW)。每隔 15 天记录一次昼夜行为,每天 12 小时,每 5 分钟记录一次。此外,还测量了心率(HR;次/分)、呼吸频率(RR;次/分)、直肠温度(RT;℃)和喘气评分。水牛减少了放牧时间,尤其是在 THI 超过 75 时。与SH水牛相比,SHW水牛吃草和躺卧的时间更长(P < 0.05)。与小牛相比,母牛和小牛的反刍时间(P < 0.05)和站立时间(P < 0.05)更长。不同处理(SH 与 SHW)之间的生理变量没有差异(P > 0.05),但与气温和 THI 呈正相关(P < 0.05)。母牛(59.6)和犊牛(63.5)的 HR 高于母牛(53.3;P < 0.05)。小母牛(38.4)和犊牛(38.3)的 RT 往往高于母牛(38.1;0.05;P <;0.10)。不过,不同类别水牛的呼吸速率并无差异(P >0.05)。在整个观察日期间,水牛都没有气喘行为(气喘评分 = 0)。在有阴凉处和浸泡水这两种防暑降温资源的情况下,水牛更愿意选择浸泡水,以减轻热应激的影响。无论动物类别如何,水牛都能将生理变量维持在正常范围内,这表明在亚热带地区的整个炎热季节,提供遮荫和/或浸泡用水作为体温调节的丰富环境资源,适用于缓解水牛的热应激。
{"title":"Physiological and behavioral indicators of water buffaloes with access to heat mitigation resources in a grazing system during the hot season in the subtropics","authors":"Cindy Anne Klausberger Ximenes,&nbsp;Arthur Fernandes Bettencourt,&nbsp;Bruna Schmitz,&nbsp;Delane Ribas da Rosa,&nbsp;Juliany Ardenghi Guimarães,&nbsp;Betina da Cunha Borges,&nbsp;Caren Paludo Ghedini,&nbsp;Vivian Fischer","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106409","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106409","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Water buffaloes are increasingly being raised in similar way to dairy cows, in grazing systems frequently deprived of shade and water for immersion. This study aimed to evaluate the effects of different heat mitigation resources, 1) only shade and 2) shade plus water for immersion, on the behavioral repertoire and physiological characteristics of female grazing buffaloes in the subtropics during the hot season. Twenty buffaloes (<em>Bubalus bubalis</em>), including calves, heifers and non-lactating cows were allocated throughout the hot season (from December 2021 to April 2022) in a grazing system with two different heat mitigation resources (treatments): only shade (SH) and shade plus water for immersion (SHW). Diurnal behavioral repertoire was recorded every 15 days, individually and focally, at every 5 minutes for 12 hours per day. Heart rate (HR; beats/min), respiration rate (RR; breaths/min), rectal temperature (RT; °C) and panting score were also measured. Buffaloes reduced grazing time especially with THI above 75. Buffaloes at SHW spent more time grazing and lying than SH buffaloes (<em>P</em> &lt; 0.05). Cows and heifers had higher ruminating (<em>P</em> &lt; 0.05) and standing time (<em>P</em> &lt; 0.05) compared with calves. Physiological variables did not differ (<em>P</em> &gt; 0.05) between treatments (SH vs. SHW) and were positively correlated with air temperature and THI (<em>P</em> &lt; 0.05). HR was higher for heifers (59.6) and calves (63.5) than for cows (53.3; P &lt; 0.05). RT tended to be higher for heifers (38.4) and calves (38.3) than for cows (38.1; 0.05 &lt; P &lt; 0.10). However, respiration rate did not differ across buffalo categories (<em>P</em> &gt; 0.05). Panting behavior was not reported throughout the observation days (panting score = 0). When both heat mitigation resources, shade and water for immersion were available, buffaloes preferred water for immersion to mitigate the effects of heat stress. Regardless animal category, buffaloes were able to maintain physiological variables within the normal range, indicating that providing shade and/or water for immersion, as environmental enrichment resources for thermoregulation, is suitable for heat stress mitigation in water buffaloes throughout the hot season in the subtropics.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"280 ","pages":"Article 106409"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142422044","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
IF 2.2 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-10-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106408
Irene Camerlink
{"title":"","authors":"Irene Camerlink","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106408","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106408","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"280 ","pages":"Article 106408"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142422068","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Relationships of dog behavior and cattle illness signs to dogs’ ability to distinguish between sick and healthy cattle 狗的行为和牛的疾病征兆与狗区分病牛和健康牛的能力之间的关系
IF 2.2 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106407
Aiden E. Juge , Nathaniel J. Hall , John T. Richeson , Reinaldo F. Cooke , Courtney L. Daigle
Dogs are highly successful at detecting disease using olfaction; however, performance varies among dogs. Training detection dogs represents a substantial investment of time and resources; thus, identifying behavioral markers of a successful detection dog and factors that affect dog performance is needed. In a previous study, one of four dogs was able to distinguish between nasal and saliva swabs from healthy cattle and cattle undergoing an inflammatory response at a rate greater than chance, with 73 % accuracy. The present study evaluated how dogs’ accuracy changed throughout training and testing, whether dogs’ responses were related to illness signs present in cattle, and whether dogs’ accuracy was related to their own behavior on a per-trial and per-sample basis. Trial number, or the order of a detection trial within a 20-trial training or test session, was slightly positively correlated with accuracy for one dog. Sample number, the order in which the dogs encountered samples, was slightly positively correlated with per-session accuracy for all dogs. Dog accuracy declined across training sessions that used the same sample (F4381 = 3.48, P = 0.01), suggesting a deterioration in sample quality over time. Among the clinical illness signs measured in cattle, the strongest association demonstrated that greater changes in cattle body temperature were positively associated with dog accuracy (r = 0.32, P < 0.01). During training and testing, positive (sickness-model), negative (healthy) and blank (unscented) samples were presented at equal rates. Dogs spent more time sniffing and made more visits to investigate the positive samples than negative samples, and spent more time sniffing and made more visits to negative samples than blank samples, indicating that less time was required for dogs to rule out unscented stations than to distinguish between cattle swabs. Visits to the negative sample and visits to the positive sample were negatively (r = −0.581, P < 0.01) and positively (r = 0.761, P < 0.01) correlated with accuracy, respectively. Increased latency to search was negatively correlated with accuracy (r = −0.10, P < 0.01). This represents another potential behavioral marker of successful detection dogs.
狗在利用嗅觉检测疾病方面非常成功,但不同的狗在这方面的表现却不尽相同。训练检测犬需要投入大量的时间和资源;因此,需要确定成功检测犬的行为标记以及影响犬表现的因素。在之前的一项研究中,四只狗中有一只能够区分健康牛和发炎牛的鼻拭子和唾液拭子,准确率为 73%,高于概率。本研究评估了狗的准确性在整个训练和测试过程中的变化情况,狗的反应是否与牛身上出现的疾病征兆有关,以及狗的准确性是否与每次试验和每个样本的自身行为有关。对于一只狗来说,试验次数或检测试验在 20 次训练或测试过程中的顺序与准确性略呈正相关。样本数量,即狗狗遇到样本的顺序,与所有狗狗每次训练的准确率略呈正相关。在使用相同样本的各次训练中,狗的准确率均有所下降(F4381 = 3.48,P = 0.01),这表明样本质量会随着时间的推移而下降。在牛的临床疾病征兆测量中,牛体温的较大变化与狗的准确性呈正相关(r = 0.32,P = 0.01)。在训练和测试过程中,阳性样本(疾病模型)、阴性样本(健康)和空白样本(无香味)的呈现率相同。与阴性样本相比,狗嗅闻阳性样本的时间更长,调查阴性样本的次数也更多,与空白样本相比,狗嗅闻阴性样本的时间更长,调查阴性样本的次数也更多。访问阴性样本和访问阳性样本的次数分别与准确率呈负相关(r = -0.581,P < 0.01)和正相关(r = 0.761,P < 0.01)。搜索延迟的增加与准确率呈负相关(r = -0.10,P < 0.01)。这代表了成功探测犬的另一个潜在行为标记。
{"title":"Relationships of dog behavior and cattle illness signs to dogs’ ability to distinguish between sick and healthy cattle","authors":"Aiden E. Juge ,&nbsp;Nathaniel J. Hall ,&nbsp;John T. Richeson ,&nbsp;Reinaldo F. Cooke ,&nbsp;Courtney L. Daigle","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106407","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106407","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Dogs are highly successful at detecting disease using olfaction; however, performance varies among dogs. Training detection dogs represents a substantial investment of time and resources; thus, identifying behavioral markers of a successful detection dog and factors that affect dog performance is needed. In a previous study, one of four dogs was able to distinguish between nasal and saliva swabs from healthy cattle and cattle undergoing an inflammatory response at a rate greater than chance, with 73 % accuracy. The present study evaluated how dogs’ accuracy changed throughout training and testing, whether dogs’ responses were related to illness signs present in cattle, and whether dogs’ accuracy was related to their own behavior on a per-trial and per-sample basis. Trial number, or the order of a detection trial within a 20-trial training or test session, was slightly positively correlated with accuracy for one dog. Sample number, the order in which the dogs encountered samples, was slightly positively correlated with per-session accuracy for all dogs. Dog accuracy declined across training sessions that used the same sample (F<sub>4381</sub> = 3.48, P = 0.01), suggesting a deterioration in sample quality over time. Among the clinical illness signs measured in cattle, the strongest association demonstrated that greater changes in cattle body temperature were positively associated with dog accuracy (r = 0.32, P &lt; 0.01). During training and testing, positive (sickness-model), negative (healthy) and blank (unscented) samples were presented at equal rates. Dogs spent more time sniffing and made more visits to investigate the positive samples than negative samples, and spent more time sniffing and made more visits to negative samples than blank samples, indicating that less time was required for dogs to rule out unscented stations than to distinguish between cattle swabs. Visits to the negative sample and visits to the positive sample were negatively (r = −0.581, P &lt; 0.01) and positively (r = 0.761, P &lt; 0.01) correlated with accuracy, respectively. Increased latency to search was negatively correlated with accuracy (r = −0.10, P &lt; 0.01). This represents another potential behavioral marker of successful detection dogs.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"279 ","pages":"Article 106407"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142423909","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
One Ethology, understanding animal behaviour to improve the lives of animals, humans and society: Preface for the special issue of ISAE’s 56th international congress 一种动物学,了解动物行为,改善动物、人类和社会的生活:第 56 届国际动物学会大会特刊序言
IF 2.2 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106382
Kris Descovich, Peta Taylor
{"title":"One Ethology, understanding animal behaviour to improve the lives of animals, humans and society: Preface for the special issue of ISAE’s 56th international congress","authors":"Kris Descovich,&nbsp;Peta Taylor","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106382","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106382","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"279 ","pages":"Article 106382"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142181656","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Deleterious effect of social instability on broiler chicken learning abilities and behaviour 社会不稳定对肉鸡学习能力和行为的有害影响
IF 2.2 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106398
Cécile Arnould , Carole Foucher , Ludovic Calandreau , Aline Bertin , Vérane Gigaud , Cécile Berri
Broiler chickens are reared in very large groups and are submitted to repeated encounters with unknown conspecifics. Our aim was to assess the consequences of these encounters on broiler chickens learning abilities, social behaviour and emotional reactivity. Groups of 5 male medium-growing broiler chickens (JA 957) were reared under stable or unstable social conditions (n=16 groups in each condition). Under the unstable conditions, the 5 birds of the groups changed every 3–4 days from 1 to 52 days of age. Broiler chickens were tested in an associative learning task (conditioned place preference) between 14 and 18 days of age. A highly palatable food (mealworms) was delivered in a particular environment with coloured stripes and preference for this environment was then tested. Social behaviours were analysed between 21 and 43 days of age. Emotional reactivity of the birds when faced with novelty (reactivity to unknown food, object and human) was assessed at the end of the rearing period (46–51 days of age). Only birds from the stable condition were able to associate the palatable food to the environment in which it was delivered (p = 0.02). Furthermore, these birds had a higher social proximity than those from the unstable condition (p < 0.01) and showed less aggressive interactions such as aggressive pecking (p = 0.06), threat (p < 0.01) or facing each other with wing flapping in front of another bird (p = 0.03). The social conditions tested had no significant effect on their emotional reactivity. Our results evidenced that chickens exposed to social instability have some learning abilities impaired compared to chickens maintained in stable social conditions. In addition, social instability increases aggressive interactions between conspecifics, while social stability favours bird proximity. In conclusion, being reared in unstable social conditions likely impairs bird adaptation to their rearing environment and increases deleterious consequences of aggressive interactions with conspecifics.
肉鸡饲养在非常大的群体中,会与未知的同类反复相遇。我们的目的是评估这些接触对肉鸡学习能力、社会行为和情绪反应的影响。我们在稳定或不稳定的社会条件下饲养了5只雄性中等生长肉鸡(JA 957)(每组16只)。在不稳定条件下,从1日龄到52日龄,每3-4天更换一组5只鸡。在 14 到 18 日龄期间,对肉鸡进行联想学习任务(条件性位置偏好)测试。在带有彩色条纹的特定环境中提供高适口性食物(黄粉虫),然后测试对该环境的偏好。在 21 到 43 日龄期间对鸟类的社交行为进行了分析。在饲养期结束时(46-51 日龄),对鸟类面对新事物时的情绪反应(对未知食物、物体和人类的反应)进行了评估。只有处于稳定状态的鸽子能够将适口食物与提供食物的环境联系起来(p = 0.02)。此外,这些鸟的社会亲和力比不稳定条件下的鸟高(p = 0.01),并且表现出较少的攻击性互动,如攻击性啄食(p = 0.06)、威胁(p = 0.01)或在另一只鸟面前面对面拍打翅膀(p = 0.03)。测试的社会条件对鸡的情绪反应没有明显影响。我们的研究结果表明,与保持稳定的社会条件下的鸡相比,暴露于社会不稳定环境中的鸡的某些学习能力会受到影响。此外,社会不稳定会增加同类之间的攻击性互动,而社会稳定则有利于鸟类的亲近。总之,在不稳定的社会条件下饲养可能会影响鸟类对饲养环境的适应,并增加与同类之间攻击性互动的有害后果。
{"title":"Deleterious effect of social instability on broiler chicken learning abilities and behaviour","authors":"Cécile Arnould ,&nbsp;Carole Foucher ,&nbsp;Ludovic Calandreau ,&nbsp;Aline Bertin ,&nbsp;Vérane Gigaud ,&nbsp;Cécile Berri","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106398","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106398","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Broiler chickens are reared in very large groups and are submitted to repeated encounters with unknown conspecifics. Our aim was to assess the consequences of these encounters on broiler chickens learning abilities, social behaviour and emotional reactivity. Groups of 5 male medium-growing broiler chickens (JA 957) were reared under stable or unstable social conditions (n=16 groups in each condition). Under the unstable conditions, the 5 birds of the groups changed every 3–4 days from 1 to 52 days of age. Broiler chickens were tested in an associative learning task (conditioned place preference) between 14 and 18 days of age. A highly palatable food (mealworms) was delivered in a particular environment with coloured stripes and preference for this environment was then tested. Social behaviours were analysed between 21 and 43 days of age. Emotional reactivity of the birds when faced with novelty (reactivity to unknown food, object and human) was assessed at the end of the rearing period (46–51 days of age). Only birds from the stable condition were able to associate the palatable food to the environment in which it was delivered (p = 0.02). Furthermore, these birds had a higher social proximity than those from the unstable condition (p &lt; 0.01) and showed less aggressive interactions such as aggressive pecking (p = 0.06), threat (p &lt; 0.01) or facing each other with wing flapping in front of another bird (p = 0.03). The social conditions tested had no significant effect on their emotional reactivity. Our results evidenced that chickens exposed to social instability have some learning abilities impaired compared to chickens maintained in stable social conditions. In addition, social instability increases aggressive interactions between conspecifics, while social stability favours bird proximity. In conclusion, being reared in unstable social conditions likely impairs bird adaptation to their rearing environment and increases deleterious consequences of aggressive interactions with conspecifics.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"279 ","pages":"Article 106398"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142423910","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Startle magnitude is a repeatable measure of reactive temperament in sheep 惊吓幅度是绵羊反应性情的一种可重复测量方法
IF 2.2 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106404
Hannah E. Salvin , Angela Lees , Linda M. Cafe , Stephen Morris , Caroline Lee
Behavioural reactivity is an expression of temperament that may influence an animal’s response to stressful situations and impact on handleability and the human-animal relationship. Shortcomings of previously described tests led to the development of a standardized startle protocol in sheep using a tactile stimulus (airblast to the face) and the presence of conspecifics to minimize the effects of social isolation. Here we tested the repeatability of the protocol, the consistency of the response and habituation to the protocol. Twenty sheep were tested across four startle interval test phases. Phases were applied consecutively such that each sheep received up to 17 startles across a six-week period with two to six days between test phases. Phases were; within – up to 5 startles over a 7 min test, Bi-hourly – four repetitions of a single 4-minute startle test 2 hours apart on one day, daily – a single 4-minute startle test repeated over four consecutive days and, weekly – a single 4-minute startle test repeated once per week over four consecutive weeks. Three measures of startle magnitude, retreat distance (zone 1–6) and startle duration (s), startle force (gs), were recorded for each startle. A reduction in response to the startle stimulus over time was identified; however, consistent responses of sheep for the measures of startle magnitude were generally observed across test phases. Repeatability of retreat distance and startle force were moderate to high within test phases (0.56–0.82), whereas startle duration had low to moderate repeatability (0.26–0.50). Strong consistency in individual responses, good repeatability, and the presence of a distinct startle response after repeated applications of the stimulus indicate that the startle test shows promise as a measure of reactive temperament in sheep. Further development to improve the tests access in commercial environments may also facilitate producers use of startle magnitude for the selection of less reactive temperaments to improve handleability, safety and welfare on farm.
行为反应性是性情的一种表现形式,可能会影响动物对应激情况的反应,并对动物的可操控性和人与动物的关系产生影响。由于之前描述的测试存在缺陷,因此我们开发了一种标准化的绵羊惊吓方案,该方案使用触觉刺激(向面部喷气),并有同种动物在场,以尽量减少社会隔离的影响。在此,我们测试了该方案的可重复性、反应的一致性以及对该方案的习惯性。20 只绵羊在四个惊吓间歇测试阶段接受了测试。各阶段连续进行,每只绵羊在六周内最多受到 17 次惊吓,测试阶段之间间隔 2 到 6 天。这些阶段分别是:内--7 分钟测试中最多 5 次惊吓;每两小时--一天中间隔 2 小时重复 4 次单次 4 分钟惊吓测试;每天--连续四天重复单次 4 分钟惊吓测试;每周--连续四周每周重复一次单次 4 分钟惊吓测试。每次惊吓都记录了惊吓幅度、后退距离(1-6区)和惊吓持续时间(秒)、惊吓力(克)的三个测量值。结果表明,随着时间的推移,绵羊对惊吓刺激的反应有所减弱;但是,在不同的测试阶段,绵羊对惊吓幅度的反应基本一致。在测试阶段内,后退距离和惊吓力的重复性为中高水平(0.56-0.82),而惊吓持续时间的重复性为中低水平(0.26-0.50)。个体反应的一致性强、可重复性好以及重复刺激后出现明显的惊吓反应表明,惊吓试验有望成为绵羊反应性情的一种测量方法。进一步开发改进商业环境中的测试,还能促进生产者利用惊吓程度来选择反应性较低的性情,从而提高农场的可操作性、安全性和福利性。
{"title":"Startle magnitude is a repeatable measure of reactive temperament in sheep","authors":"Hannah E. Salvin ,&nbsp;Angela Lees ,&nbsp;Linda M. Cafe ,&nbsp;Stephen Morris ,&nbsp;Caroline Lee","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106404","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106404","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Behavioural reactivity is an expression of temperament that may influence an animal’s response to stressful situations and impact on handleability and the human-animal relationship. Shortcomings of previously described tests led to the development of a standardized startle protocol in sheep using a tactile stimulus (airblast to the face) and the presence of conspecifics to minimize the effects of social isolation. Here we tested the repeatability of the protocol, the consistency of the response and habituation to the protocol. Twenty sheep were tested across four startle interval test phases. Phases were applied consecutively such that each sheep received up to 17 startles across a six-week period with two to six days between test phases. Phases were; <em>within</em> – up to 5 startles over a 7 min test, <em>Bi-hourly</em> – four repetitions of a single 4-minute startle test 2 hours apart on one day, <em>daily</em> – a single 4-minute startle test repeated over four consecutive days and, <em>weekly</em> – a single 4-minute startle test repeated once per week over four consecutive weeks. Three measures of startle magnitude, retreat distance (zone 1–6) and startle duration (s), startle force (gs), were recorded for each startle. A reduction in response to the startle stimulus over time was identified; however, consistent responses of sheep for the measures of startle magnitude were generally observed across test phases. Repeatability of retreat distance and startle force were moderate to high within test phases (0.56–0.82), whereas startle duration had low to moderate repeatability (0.26–0.50). Strong consistency in individual responses, good repeatability, and the presence of a distinct startle response after repeated applications of the stimulus indicate that the startle test shows promise as a measure of reactive temperament in sheep. Further development to improve the tests access in commercial environments may also facilitate producers use of startle magnitude for the selection of less reactive temperaments to improve handleability, safety and welfare on farm.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"279 ","pages":"Article 106404"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142356859","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Measures of spatial memory, peripheral inflammation and negative emotional state predict social-cognitive skills in healthy aging cats 测量空间记忆、外周炎症和消极情绪状态可预测健康老龄猫的社交认知能力
IF 2.2 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106406
Federica Pirrone , Patrizia Piotti , Holly Memoli , Irit Grader , Paola Scarpa , Eleonora Biffi , Joel Filipe , Mariangela Albertini , Carlo Siracusa
Aging is a complex, multidimensional process. Normative aging is typically accompanied by declines in several cognitive domains, including memory and social cognition. As aging progresses, the often-overlooked early manifestations of cognitive decline can advance, leading to an inability to adapt to new situations and environments. Potential mechanisms influencing cognitive decline remain elusive, but, among other mechanisms, inflammaging, an age-related increase in the chronic production of pro-inflammatory mediators, and emotional stress are implicated. Cognitive function in cats is under-researched, with no studies on the link between potential risk factors and cognitive signs in aging subjects. Understanding this link is essential as it may facilitate the early identification of cognitive decline and encourage preventative, individualized interventions. We carried out a citizen science study to investigate, for the first time, the relationship between cognitive functions, stress, and the inflammatory markers interleukin (IL)-1β and IL-10 in 44 healthy, privately owned pet cats (7–14 years of age), who underwent standardized testing in their home environment. A task was employed to test spatial memory, requiring the cat to recall the location of a food-baited container. An unsolvable task was used to assess cats' social cognition and cognitive flexibility by measuring how they used referential communication (i.e., gaze alternation between the apparatus and the caregivers) compared to their persistence in trying to obtain food which was out of reach. Generalized linear regression analysis, conducted while adjusting for demographic factors and stress behaviors, revealed: 1) a predictive positive relationship connecting spatial memory and the use of social referencing with the caregiver (more gaze alternation) in the unsolvable task, highlighting the importance of the interplay between cognitive domains in sustaining complex behaviors, and 2) a reduced engagement in social referencing toward caregivers (less gaze alternation) in the presence of higher serum levels of IL-1β in the unsolvable task, especially as cats get older. A cat’s social cognitive performance was also negatively predicted by stress-related behaviors, which is not surprising given that cats are sentient creatures whose everyday behavior is also influenced by emotional states. These findings provide new insights into the crosstalk between different cognitive domains during aging and the potential contributions of emotions and inflammation to cognitive changes in healthy aging cats. Further research using this multidimensional approach will help explore these mechanisms of cognitive aging and potential markers for early detection of cognitive changes in older cats, which is critical for timely interventions.
衰老是一个复杂的多维过程。正常衰老通常伴随着多个认知领域的衰退,包括记忆和社会认知。随着衰老的进展,往往被忽视的认知能力衰退的早期表现会提前出现,导致无法适应新的情况和环境。影响认知能力衰退的潜在机制仍然难以捉摸,但除其他机制外,炎症反应、与年龄相关的促炎症介质慢性生成的增加以及情绪压力都与此有关。对猫的认知功能研究不足,目前还没有研究表明潜在的风险因素与衰老对象的认知症状之间存在联系。了解这种联系至关重要,因为这有助于及早发现认知功能衰退,并鼓励采取个性化的预防性干预措施。我们开展了一项公民科学研究,首次调查了 44 只健康的私人饲养的宠物猫(7-14 岁)的认知功能、压力和炎症标志物白细胞介素 (IL)-1β 和 IL-10之间的关系。测试中使用了一项测试空间记忆的任务,要求猫回忆食物诱饵容器的位置。此外,还采用了一项无法解决的任务来评估猫的社会认知能力和认知灵活性,方法是测量猫如何使用参照物交流(即在仪器和看护人之间交替注视),以及猫在试图获取无法触及的食物时的坚持程度。在对人口统计学因素和压力行为进行调整后,进行的广义线性回归分析表明1)在无法解决的任务中,空间记忆与使用与照顾者的社会参照(更多的目光交替)之间存在预测性的正相关关系,这突出了认知领域之间的相互作用在维持复杂行为中的重要性;2)在无法解决的任务中,如果猫的血清中 IL-1β 水平较高,则猫对照顾者的社会参照(较少的目光交替)的参与度会降低,尤其是随着猫年龄的增长。猫的社会认知能力也会受到压力相关行为的负面影响,这并不奇怪,因为猫是有知觉的动物,其日常行为也会受到情绪状态的影响。这些发现为研究衰老过程中不同认知领域之间的相互影响以及情绪和炎症对健康衰老猫认知变化的潜在影响提供了新的视角。利用这种多维方法开展的进一步研究将有助于探索这些认知老化机制以及早期检测老年猫认知变化的潜在标记物,这对于及时干预至关重要。
{"title":"Measures of spatial memory, peripheral inflammation and negative emotional state predict social-cognitive skills in healthy aging cats","authors":"Federica Pirrone ,&nbsp;Patrizia Piotti ,&nbsp;Holly Memoli ,&nbsp;Irit Grader ,&nbsp;Paola Scarpa ,&nbsp;Eleonora Biffi ,&nbsp;Joel Filipe ,&nbsp;Mariangela Albertini ,&nbsp;Carlo Siracusa","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106406","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106406","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Aging is a complex, multidimensional process. Normative aging is typically accompanied by declines in several cognitive domains, including memory and social cognition. As aging progresses, the often-overlooked early manifestations of cognitive decline can advance, leading to an inability to adapt to new situations and environments. Potential mechanisms influencing cognitive decline remain elusive, but, among other mechanisms, inflammaging, an age-related increase in the chronic production of pro-inflammatory mediators, and emotional stress are implicated. Cognitive function in cats is under-researched, with no studies on the link between potential risk factors and cognitive signs in aging subjects. Understanding this link is essential as it may facilitate the early identification of cognitive decline and encourage preventative, individualized interventions. We carried out a citizen science study to investigate, for the first time, the relationship between cognitive functions, stress, and the inflammatory markers interleukin (IL)-1β and IL-10 in 44 healthy, privately owned pet cats (7–14 years of age), who underwent standardized testing in their home environment. A task was employed to test spatial memory, requiring the cat to recall the location of a food-baited container. An unsolvable task was used to assess cats' social cognition and cognitive flexibility by measuring how they used referential communication (i.e., gaze alternation between the apparatus and the caregivers) compared to their persistence in trying to obtain food which was out of reach. Generalized linear regression analysis, conducted while adjusting for demographic factors and stress behaviors, revealed: 1) a predictive positive relationship connecting spatial memory and the use of social referencing with the caregiver (more gaze alternation) in the unsolvable task, highlighting the importance of the interplay between cognitive domains in sustaining complex behaviors, and 2) a reduced engagement in social referencing toward caregivers (less gaze alternation) in the presence of higher serum levels of IL-1β in the unsolvable task, especially as cats get older. A cat’s social cognitive performance was also negatively predicted by stress-related behaviors, which is not surprising given that cats are sentient creatures whose everyday behavior is also influenced by emotional states. These findings provide new insights into the crosstalk between different cognitive domains during aging and the potential contributions of emotions and inflammation to cognitive changes in healthy aging cats. Further research using this multidimensional approach will help explore these mechanisms of cognitive aging and potential markers for early detection of cognitive changes in older cats, which is critical for timely interventions.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"280 ","pages":"Article 106406"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142422069","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Behavioral responses during and after a postpartum human-animal interaction in rangeland breeding ewes 牧场繁殖母羊产后人兽互动期间和之后的行为反应
IF 2.2 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106405
Kaleiah Schiller, Kristina Horback
<div><div>Lamb welfare, performance and survival on rangeland is partially determined by the quality of maternal care received (i.e., high durations of sniffing/grooming, allowing udder access, and staying close to the lamb). Previous studies have explored maternal behavior scores (MBSs), as a tool to select ewes based on their proximity to the lamb and the shepherd, finding variable evidence towards sensitivity and robustness in scores in ability to gauge lamb outcome (birth weight and weaning weight), survival, and maternal care throughout the lambing season. Though this scoring system is convenient, there may be another behavioral attribute related to fear of human or relationship to the human handler may be elicited during lamb collection and processing. This longitudinal study investigated the presence of a trait in response to the human handler employing a Lamb Handling (during processing) and a Lamb Tie Down (after processing) test with N= 42 total ewes and n=26 ewes consistently present across both years of the study. The current study also set out to investigate the relationship between proximity to the shepherd and lamb outcome. Attempts to perform principal component analysis revealed that variables from the Lamb Handling and Lamb Tie down test were not suitable for factor analysis. In the Lamb Handling test, proportion of ‘pacing’ was a nearly significant, negative predictor (P=0.07) term in the binary ‘investigating human’ model, improved model fit and explained more variance compared to the null model [(cumulative weight: 90 vs 10 %); (R<sup>2</sup><sub>adjusted</sub>: 30.8 vs 15.2 %; deviance= 31 vs 18.5 %)]. A random term for individual also improved fit in this model, indicating a potential behavioral trait driven by the ewe related to fear or perception of the human. The occurrence of grazing (0 or 1) was a nearly significant, positive predictor (P=0.06), improved model fit and explained more variance in the ‘investigating human’ model compared to the null model [(cumulative weight: 76 vs 24 %); (R<sup>2</sup><sub>adjusted</sub>: 21 vs 15.2 %; deviance= 26.8 vs 18.5 %)]. Individual did not improve model fit in the relationship between ‘investigation human’ and ‘grazing’. Weaning weights were a nearly significant positive predictor in the model for ‘close proximity’ at lamb collection (P=0.07) and a significant positive predictor for ‘close proximity’ during processing (P<0.001), however, did not improve model fit over the null. Birth weights improved model fit for behaviours including ‘sniffing/grooming’ and ‘pawing’ in the Lamb Tie Down test and these behaviours may have been moderated by maternal investment in heavier lambs. Behavioural responses, expanding beyond proximity measures, can be useful for gauging maternal attachment and investment in the lamb during and after processing. Fear or perception of humans should also be considered as contributors to the response to the shepherd during human-animal interactions in the
羔羊在牧场上的福利、表现和存活率在一定程度上取决于母性照料的质量(即长时间的嗅闻/梳理、允许接触乳房和靠近羔羊)。以往的研究探讨了母性行为评分(MBSs),将其作为根据母羊与羔羊和牧羊人的亲近程度来选择母羊的工具,结果发现,在衡量羔羊结果(出生体重和断奶体重)、存活率以及整个产羔季节的母性照料能力方面,评分的灵敏度和稳健性各不相同。虽然这种评分系统很方便,但在收集和处理羔羊的过程中,可能会激发出另一种与惧怕人类或与人类饲养者关系有关的行为属性。本纵向研究采用羔羊处理(处理过程中)和羔羊捆绑(处理后)测试,调查了母羊对人类处理者的反应特质的存在情况,在研究的两年中,共有 42 只母羊和 26 只母羊持续存在这种特质。本研究还旨在调查与牧羊人的距离与羔羊结果之间的关系。尝试进行主成分分析后发现,羔羊处理和羔羊绑缚测试中的变量不适合进行因子分析。在羔羊处理测试中,"起搏 "比例是二元 "调查人类 "模型中一个几乎显著的负预测项(P=0.07),与空模型相比,它改善了模型的拟合度并解释了更多的方差[(累计重量:90 vs 10 %);(R2调整后:30.8 vs 15.2 %;偏差= 31 vs 18.5 %)]。个体的随机项也提高了该模型的拟合度,表明母羊的潜在行为特征与对人类的恐惧或感知有关。在 "调查人类 "模型中,放牧发生率(0 或 1)是一个近乎显著的正向预测因子(P=0.06),与无效模型相比,它提高了模型的拟合度并解释了更多的变异[(累计重量:76 vs 24 %);(R2 调整后:21 vs 15.2 %;偏差= 26.8 vs 18.5 %)]。个体并没有改善 "人类调查 "与 "放牧 "之间关系的模型拟合。断奶体重在羔羊采集时的 "接近 "模型中是一个近乎显著的正向预测因子(P=0.07),在加工过程中的 "接近 "模型中也是一个显著的正向预测因子(P<0.001),但与空值相比,并未改善模型拟合。出生体重提高了 "拴系羔羊 "试验中 "嗅闻/梳理 "和 "用爪子抓 "等行为的模型拟合度,这些行为可能受到母体对较重羔羊投资的影响。行为反应超出了近距离测量的范围,可用于衡量母性对羔羊的依恋以及在处理过程中和处理后对羔羊的投资。在产羔季节人与动物的互动中,对人类的恐惧或感知也应被视为对牧羊人反应的影响因素。
{"title":"Behavioral responses during and after a postpartum human-animal interaction in rangeland breeding ewes","authors":"Kaleiah Schiller,&nbsp;Kristina Horback","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106405","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106405","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;div&gt;&lt;div&gt;Lamb welfare, performance and survival on rangeland is partially determined by the quality of maternal care received (i.e., high durations of sniffing/grooming, allowing udder access, and staying close to the lamb). Previous studies have explored maternal behavior scores (MBSs), as a tool to select ewes based on their proximity to the lamb and the shepherd, finding variable evidence towards sensitivity and robustness in scores in ability to gauge lamb outcome (birth weight and weaning weight), survival, and maternal care throughout the lambing season. Though this scoring system is convenient, there may be another behavioral attribute related to fear of human or relationship to the human handler may be elicited during lamb collection and processing. This longitudinal study investigated the presence of a trait in response to the human handler employing a Lamb Handling (during processing) and a Lamb Tie Down (after processing) test with N= 42 total ewes and n=26 ewes consistently present across both years of the study. The current study also set out to investigate the relationship between proximity to the shepherd and lamb outcome. Attempts to perform principal component analysis revealed that variables from the Lamb Handling and Lamb Tie down test were not suitable for factor analysis. In the Lamb Handling test, proportion of ‘pacing’ was a nearly significant, negative predictor (P=0.07) term in the binary ‘investigating human’ model, improved model fit and explained more variance compared to the null model [(cumulative weight: 90 vs 10 %); (R&lt;sup&gt;2&lt;/sup&gt;&lt;sub&gt;adjusted&lt;/sub&gt;: 30.8 vs 15.2 %; deviance= 31 vs 18.5 %)]. A random term for individual also improved fit in this model, indicating a potential behavioral trait driven by the ewe related to fear or perception of the human. The occurrence of grazing (0 or 1) was a nearly significant, positive predictor (P=0.06), improved model fit and explained more variance in the ‘investigating human’ model compared to the null model [(cumulative weight: 76 vs 24 %); (R&lt;sup&gt;2&lt;/sup&gt;&lt;sub&gt;adjusted&lt;/sub&gt;: 21 vs 15.2 %; deviance= 26.8 vs 18.5 %)]. Individual did not improve model fit in the relationship between ‘investigation human’ and ‘grazing’. Weaning weights were a nearly significant positive predictor in the model for ‘close proximity’ at lamb collection (P=0.07) and a significant positive predictor for ‘close proximity’ during processing (P&lt;0.001), however, did not improve model fit over the null. Birth weights improved model fit for behaviours including ‘sniffing/grooming’ and ‘pawing’ in the Lamb Tie Down test and these behaviours may have been moderated by maternal investment in heavier lambs. Behavioural responses, expanding beyond proximity measures, can be useful for gauging maternal attachment and investment in the lamb during and after processing. Fear or perception of humans should also be considered as contributors to the response to the shepherd during human-animal interactions in the","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"280 ","pages":"Article 106405"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142422070","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dairy calves' time spent in the cow herd in a calf-driven cow-calf contact system during two-step separation with a nose flap 在犊牛驱动的母牛-犊牛接触系统中,用鼻翼进行两步分离时,奶牛犊牛在牛群中停留的时间
IF 2.2 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106399
Anina Vogt , Kerstin Barth , Marie Schneider , Uta König von Borstel , Susanne Waiblinger
Although originally developed for weaning of beef calves, nose flaps are also used on dairy farms nowadays to wean calves which are reared with contact to their dam or a foster cow. These calves are often weaned younger than beef calves and the time they wear nose flaps is highly variable, since it is unknown for how long younger calves need to wear the nose flap in order to induce an effective weaning. The aim of our study was therefore to track changes in dam-contact initiated by dairy calves after insertion of a nose flap in order to determine the minimum duration a nose flap needs to be worn to effectively reduce suckling motivation of calves. The study was conducted in two sequential experiments using 3-month-old dairy calves that were reared with full-time dam contact. Calves were weaned with a nose flap over either 14 (experiment I, 9 calves in herd A and 9 calves in herd B) or 7 days (experiment II, 11 calves in herd A and 12 calves in herd B) before they were fence-line separated from their dams. The total time a calf spent within the cow herd (TIC) per day was determined from continuous video recordings during 7 days before nose flap insertion (baseline), as well as during the 7 or 14 days in which calves had free access to the cow area while wearing the nose flap. Statistical analysis was conducted using linear mixed effects models. Results from both experiments showed that calves reduced their TIC at the fourth day of wearing the nose flap compared to their individual baseline (p=0.03 and p=0.001 respectively). In experiment I, calves showed a further numerical reduction in TIC from day 4 to day 5, while in experiment II, calves showed no further reduction in TIC after the 4 days of wearing the nose flap. After these 4 or 5 days respectively, TIC of calves in both experiments stayed at a constant low level of several hours per day. Taken together, these results indicate that in 3-month-old dairy calves it needs at least 4 days until the motivation to spend time with the cow decreases after insertion of a nose flap, while there seems to be no further decrease in motivation when using a nose flap longer than this time. However, results were subject to considerable inter-individual variability already during the baseline week and need replication in a pasture-based setting with less space-restrictions.
虽然挡鼻板最初是为肉用犊牛断奶而开发的,但现在奶牛场也使用挡鼻板为与母牛或寄养牛接触饲养的犊牛断奶。这些犊牛的断奶年龄通常比肉用犊牛小,而且它们佩戴鼻翼的时间变化很大,因为不知道小犊牛需要佩戴多长时间的鼻翼才能有效断奶。因此,我们的研究目的是跟踪奶牛犊牛在佩戴鼻翼后与母牛接触的变化,以确定佩戴鼻翼的最短时间,从而有效降低犊牛的吸吮动机。该研究使用 3 个月大的奶牛犊牛进行了两次连续实验,这些犊牛在饲养过程中与母牛保持全时接触。在犊牛与母牛进行栅栏隔离之前,给犊牛佩戴鼻翼14天(实验I,A群9头犊牛和B群9头犊牛)或7天(实验II,A群11头犊牛和B群12头犊牛)。犊牛每天在牛群中逗留的总时间(TIC)是根据插入鼻翼前 7 天(基线)以及犊牛佩戴鼻翼可自由进入牛区的 7 天或 14 天的连续视频记录确定的。统计分析采用线性混合效应模型进行。两项实验的结果表明,与各自的基线相比,小牛在佩戴鼻翼的第四天减少了TIC(分别为p=0.03和p=0.001)。在实验 I 中,从第 4 天到第 5 天,小牛的 TIC 数量进一步减少;而在实验 II 中,佩戴鼻翼 4 天后,小牛的 TIC 没有进一步减少。在这 4 天或 5 天之后,两个实验中的小牛的 TIC 都保持在每天数小时的低水平。综上所述,这些结果表明,3个月大的奶牛犊牛在佩戴鼻翼后至少需要4天才能降低与奶牛相处的积极性,而使用鼻翼的时间超过这段时间后,犊牛的积极性似乎不会进一步降低。不过,在基线周期间,个体间的差异已经很大,因此需要在空间限制较少的牧场环境中进行复制。
{"title":"Dairy calves' time spent in the cow herd in a calf-driven cow-calf contact system during two-step separation with a nose flap","authors":"Anina Vogt ,&nbsp;Kerstin Barth ,&nbsp;Marie Schneider ,&nbsp;Uta König von Borstel ,&nbsp;Susanne Waiblinger","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106399","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106399","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Although originally developed for weaning of beef calves, nose flaps are also used on dairy farms nowadays to wean calves which are reared with contact to their dam or a foster cow. These calves are often weaned younger than beef calves and the time they wear nose flaps is highly variable, since it is unknown for how long younger calves need to wear the nose flap in order to induce an effective weaning. The aim of our study was therefore to track changes in dam-contact initiated by dairy calves after insertion of a nose flap in order to determine the minimum duration a nose flap needs to be worn to effectively reduce suckling motivation of calves. The study was conducted in two sequential experiments using 3-month-old dairy calves that were reared with full-time dam contact. Calves were weaned with a nose flap over either 14 (experiment I, 9 calves in herd A and 9 calves in herd B) or 7 days (experiment II, 11 calves in herd A and 12 calves in herd B) before they were fence-line separated from their dams. The total time a calf spent within the cow herd (TIC) per day was determined from continuous video recordings during 7 days before nose flap insertion (baseline), as well as during the 7 or 14 days in which calves had free access to the cow area while wearing the nose flap. Statistical analysis was conducted using linear mixed effects models. Results from both experiments showed that calves reduced their TIC at the fourth day of wearing the nose flap compared to their individual baseline (p=0.03 and p=0.001 respectively). In experiment I, calves showed a further numerical reduction in TIC from day 4 to day 5, while in experiment II, calves showed no further reduction in TIC after the 4 days of wearing the nose flap. After these 4 or 5 days respectively, TIC of calves in both experiments stayed at a constant low level of several hours per day. Taken together, these results indicate that in 3-month-old dairy calves it needs at least 4 days until the motivation to spend time with the cow decreases after insertion of a nose flap, while there seems to be no further decrease in motivation when using a nose flap longer than this time. However, results were subject to considerable inter-individual variability already during the baseline week and need replication in a pasture-based setting with less space-restrictions.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"280 ","pages":"Article 106399"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142421971","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Applied Animal Behaviour Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1