首页 > 最新文献

Frontiers in evolutionary neuroscience最新文献

英文 中文
Language development and the ontogeny of the dorsal pathway. 语言发展与背侧通路的个体发生。
Pub Date : 2012-02-06 eCollection Date: 2012-01-01 DOI: 10.3389/fnevo.2012.00003
Angela D Friederici

In the absence of clear phylogenetic data on the neurobiological basis of the evolution of language, comparative studies across species and across ontogenetic stages within humans may inform us about the possible neural prerequisites of language. In the adult human brain, language-relevant regions located in the frontal and temporal cortex are connected via different fiber tracts: ventral and dorsal pathways. Ontogenetically, it has been shown that newborns display an adult-like ventral pathway at birth. The dorsal pathway, however, seems to display two subparts which mature at different rates: one part, connecting the temporal cortex to the premotor cortex, is present at birth, whereas the other part, connecting the temporal cortex to Broca's area, develops much later and is still not fully matured at the age of seven. At this age, typically developing children still have problems in processing syntactically complex sentences. We therefore suggest that the mastery of complex syntax, which is at the core of human language, crucially depends on the full maturation of the fiber connection between the temporal cortex and Broca's area.

在语言进化的神经生物学基础上缺乏明确的系统发育数据的情况下,跨物种和人类个体发育阶段的比较研究可能会告诉我们语言可能的神经先决条件。在成年人的大脑中,位于额叶皮层和颞叶皮层的语言相关区域通过不同的纤维束连接:腹侧和背侧通路。从个体发育的角度来看,新生儿在出生时表现出与成人相似的腹侧通路。然而,背侧通路似乎显示出以不同速度成熟的两个子部分:一部分连接颞叶皮层和运动前皮层,在出生时就存在,而另一部分连接颞叶皮层和布洛卡区,发育得晚得多,在7岁时仍未完全成熟。在这个年龄,正常发育的孩子在处理句法复杂的句子方面仍然有问题。因此,我们认为,人类语言的核心——对复杂语法的掌握,在很大程度上取决于颞叶皮层和布洛卡区之间的纤维连接是否完全成熟。
{"title":"Language development and the ontogeny of the dorsal pathway.","authors":"Angela D Friederici","doi":"10.3389/fnevo.2012.00003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2012.00003","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the absence of clear phylogenetic data on the neurobiological basis of the evolution of language, comparative studies across species and across ontogenetic stages within humans may inform us about the possible neural prerequisites of language. In the adult human brain, language-relevant regions located in the frontal and temporal cortex are connected via different fiber tracts: ventral and dorsal pathways. Ontogenetically, it has been shown that newborns display an adult-like ventral pathway at birth. The dorsal pathway, however, seems to display two subparts which mature at different rates: one part, connecting the temporal cortex to the premotor cortex, is present at birth, whereas the other part, connecting the temporal cortex to Broca's area, develops much later and is still not fully matured at the age of seven. At this age, typically developing children still have problems in processing syntactically complex sentences. We therefore suggest that the mastery of complex syntax, which is at the core of human language, crucially depends on the full maturation of the fiber connection between the temporal cortex and Broca's area.</p>","PeriodicalId":88241,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in evolutionary neuroscience","volume":"4 ","pages":"3"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2012-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3389/fnevo.2012.00003","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30470730","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 51
Gestures, vocalizations, and memory in language origins. 语言起源中的手势、发声和记忆。
Pub Date : 2012-02-01 eCollection Date: 2012-01-01 DOI: 10.3389/fnevo.2012.00002
Francisco Aboitiz

THIS ARTICLE DISCUSSES THE POSSIBLE HOMOLOGIES BETWEEN THE HUMAN LANGUAGE NETWORKS AND COMPARABLE AUDITORY PROJECTION SYSTEMS IN THE MACAQUE BRAIN, IN AN ATTEMPT TO RECONCILE TWO EXISTING VIEWS ON LANGUAGE EVOLUTION: one that emphasizes hand control and gestures, and the other that emphasizes auditory-vocal mechanisms. The capacity for language is based on relatively well defined neural substrates whose rudiments have been traced in the non-human primate brain. At its core, this circuit constitutes an auditory-vocal sensorimotor circuit with two main components, a "ventral pathway" connecting anterior auditory regions with anterior ventrolateral prefrontal areas, and a "dorsal pathway" connecting auditory areas with parietal areas and with posterior ventrolateral prefrontal areas via the arcuate fasciculus and the superior longitudinal fasciculus. In humans, the dorsal circuit is especially important for phonological processing and phonological working memory, capacities that are critical for language acquisition and for complex syntax processing. In the macaque, the homolog of the dorsal circuit overlaps with an inferior parietal-premotor network for hand and gesture selection that is under voluntary control, while vocalizations are largely fixed and involuntary. The recruitment of the dorsal component for vocalization behavior in the human lineage, together with a direct cortical control of the subcortical vocalizing system, are proposed to represent a fundamental innovation in human evolution, generating an inflection point that permitted the explosion of vocal language and human communication. In this context, vocal communication and gesturing have a common history in primate communication.

本文讨论了人类语言网络与猕猴大脑中类似听觉投射系统之间可能存在的同源性,试图重新整合关于语言进化的两种现有观点:一种强调手部控制和手势,另一种强调听觉-发声机制。语言能力的基础是相对明确的神经基质,其雏形已在非人灵长类大脑中找到。这一回路的核心是听觉-发声感觉运动回路,主要由两部分组成:连接前听觉区和前额叶前腹外侧区的 "腹侧通路",以及通过弓状筋束和上纵筋束连接听觉区和顶叶区以及前额叶后腹外侧区的 "背侧通路"。在人类中,背侧回路对于语音处理和语音工作记忆尤为重要,这些能力对于语言习得和复杂的句法处理至关重要。在猕猴中,背侧回路的同源物与下顶叶-前运动网络重叠,该网络用于手和手势的选择,受自主控制,而发声在很大程度上是固定和非自主的。在人类血统中,发声行为需要背侧成分的参与,皮层对皮层下发声系统的直接控制,被认为是人类进化过程中的一个根本性创新,它产生了一个拐点,使发声语言和人类交流得以爆发。在这种情况下,发声交流和手势在灵长类动物的交流中有着共同的历史。
{"title":"Gestures, vocalizations, and memory in language origins.","authors":"Francisco Aboitiz","doi":"10.3389/fnevo.2012.00002","DOIUrl":"10.3389/fnevo.2012.00002","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>THIS ARTICLE DISCUSSES THE POSSIBLE HOMOLOGIES BETWEEN THE HUMAN LANGUAGE NETWORKS AND COMPARABLE AUDITORY PROJECTION SYSTEMS IN THE MACAQUE BRAIN, IN AN ATTEMPT TO RECONCILE TWO EXISTING VIEWS ON LANGUAGE EVOLUTION: one that emphasizes hand control and gestures, and the other that emphasizes auditory-vocal mechanisms. The capacity for language is based on relatively well defined neural substrates whose rudiments have been traced in the non-human primate brain. At its core, this circuit constitutes an auditory-vocal sensorimotor circuit with two main components, a \"ventral pathway\" connecting anterior auditory regions with anterior ventrolateral prefrontal areas, and a \"dorsal pathway\" connecting auditory areas with parietal areas and with posterior ventrolateral prefrontal areas via the arcuate fasciculus and the superior longitudinal fasciculus. In humans, the dorsal circuit is especially important for phonological processing and phonological working memory, capacities that are critical for language acquisition and for complex syntax processing. In the macaque, the homolog of the dorsal circuit overlaps with an inferior parietal-premotor network for hand and gesture selection that is under voluntary control, while vocalizations are largely fixed and involuntary. The recruitment of the dorsal component for vocalization behavior in the human lineage, together with a direct cortical control of the subcortical vocalizing system, are proposed to represent a fundamental innovation in human evolution, generating an inflection point that permitted the explosion of vocal language and human communication. In this context, vocal communication and gesturing have a common history in primate communication.</p>","PeriodicalId":88241,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in evolutionary neuroscience","volume":"4 ","pages":"2"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2012-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/07/4c/fnevo-04-00002.PMC3269654.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30470731","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Relationship between Intelligence and Anxiety: An Association with Subcortical White Matter Metabolism. 智力与焦虑的关系:与皮层下白质代谢的关系。
Pub Date : 2012-02-01 eCollection Date: 2011-01-01 DOI: 10.3389/fnevo.2011.00008
Jeremy D Coplan, Sarah Hodulik, Sanjay J Mathew, Xiangling Mao, Patrick R Hof, Jack M Gorman, Dikoma C Shungu

We have demonstrated in a previous study that a high degree of worry in patients with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) correlates positively with intelligence and that a low degree of worry in healthy subjects correlates positively with intelligence. We have also shown that both worry and intelligence exhibit an inverse correlation with certain metabolites in the subcortical white matter. Here we re-examine the relationships among generalized anxiety, worry, intelligence, and subcortical white matter metabolism in an extended sample. Results from the original study were combined with results from a second study to create a sample comprised of 26 patients with GAD and 18 healthy volunteers. Subjects were evaluated using the Penn State Worry Questionnaire, the Wechsler Brief intelligence quotient (IQ) assessment, and proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging ((1)H-MRSI) to measure subcortical white matter metabolism of choline and related compounds (CHO). Patients with GAD exhibited higher IQ's and lower metabolite concentrations of CHO in the subcortical white matter in comparison to healthy volunteers. When data from GAD patients and healthy controls were combined, relatively low CHO predicted both relatively higher IQ and worry scores. Relatively high anxiety in patients with GAD predicted high IQ whereas relatively low anxiety in controls also predicted high IQ. That is, the relationship between anxiety and intelligence was positive in GAD patients but inverse in healthy volunteers. The collective data suggest that both worry and intelligence are characterized by depletion of metabolic substrate in the subcortical white matter and that intelligence may have co-evolved with worry in humans.

我们在之前的一项研究中已经证明,广泛性焦虑障碍(GAD)患者的高度担忧与智力呈正相关,而健康受试者的低程度担忧与智力呈正相关。我们还表明,焦虑和智力都与皮层下白质中的某些代谢物呈负相关。在这里,我们在一个扩展的样本中重新检查了广泛性焦虑、担忧、智力和皮层下白质代谢之间的关系。原始研究的结果与第二项研究的结果相结合,形成了一个由26名广泛性焦虑症患者和18名健康志愿者组成的样本。采用宾夕法尼亚州立大学焦虑问卷、韦氏简短智商(IQ)评估和质子磁共振波谱成像((1)H-MRSI)测量脑皮层下白质胆碱及相关化合物(CHO)代谢。与健康志愿者相比,广泛性焦虑症患者表现出更高的智商和更低的皮质下白质CHO代谢物浓度。当将广泛性焦虑症患者和健康对照组的数据结合起来时,相对较低的CHO预示着相对较高的智商和焦虑得分。广泛性焦虑症患者相对高的焦虑预示着高智商,而对照组相对低的焦虑也预示着高智商。也就是说,焦虑和智力之间的关系在广泛性焦虑症患者中呈正相关,而在健康志愿者中呈负相关。这些数据表明,焦虑和智力都以皮层下白质中代谢底物的消耗为特征,人类的智力可能与焦虑共同进化。
{"title":"The Relationship between Intelligence and Anxiety: An Association with Subcortical White Matter Metabolism.","authors":"Jeremy D Coplan,&nbsp;Sarah Hodulik,&nbsp;Sanjay J Mathew,&nbsp;Xiangling Mao,&nbsp;Patrick R Hof,&nbsp;Jack M Gorman,&nbsp;Dikoma C Shungu","doi":"10.3389/fnevo.2011.00008","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00008","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We have demonstrated in a previous study that a high degree of worry in patients with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) correlates positively with intelligence and that a low degree of worry in healthy subjects correlates positively with intelligence. We have also shown that both worry and intelligence exhibit an inverse correlation with certain metabolites in the subcortical white matter. Here we re-examine the relationships among generalized anxiety, worry, intelligence, and subcortical white matter metabolism in an extended sample. Results from the original study were combined with results from a second study to create a sample comprised of 26 patients with GAD and 18 healthy volunteers. Subjects were evaluated using the Penn State Worry Questionnaire, the Wechsler Brief intelligence quotient (IQ) assessment, and proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging ((1)H-MRSI) to measure subcortical white matter metabolism of choline and related compounds (CHO). Patients with GAD exhibited higher IQ's and lower metabolite concentrations of CHO in the subcortical white matter in comparison to healthy volunteers. When data from GAD patients and healthy controls were combined, relatively low CHO predicted both relatively higher IQ and worry scores. Relatively high anxiety in patients with GAD predicted high IQ whereas relatively low anxiety in controls also predicted high IQ. That is, the relationship between anxiety and intelligence was positive in GAD patients but inverse in healthy volunteers. The collective data suggest that both worry and intelligence are characterized by depletion of metabolic substrate in the subcortical white matter and that intelligence may have co-evolved with worry in humans.</p>","PeriodicalId":88241,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in evolutionary neuroscience","volume":"3 ","pages":"8"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2012-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00008","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30470729","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 40
Mental state attribution and body configuration in women. 女性的心理状态归因与身体结构。
Pub Date : 2012-01-30 eCollection Date: 2012-01-01 DOI: 10.3389/fnevo.2012.00001
Jennifer A Bremser, Gordon G Gallup
Body configuration is a sexually dimorphic trait. In humans, men tend to have high shoulder-to-hip ratios. Women in contrast, often have low waist-to-hip ratios (WHR); i.e., narrow waists and broad hips that approximate an hour-glass configuration. Women with low WHR’s are rated as more attractive, healthier, and more fertile. They also tend to have more attractive voices, lose their virginity sooner, and have more sex partners. WHR has also been linked with general cognitive performance. In the present study we expand upon previous research examining the role of WHR in cognition. We hypothesized that more feminine body types, as indexed by a low WHR, would be associated with cognitive measures of the female “brain type,” such as mental state attribution and empathy because both may depend upon the activational effects of estrogens at puberty. We found that women with low WHRs excel at identifying emotional states of other people and show a cognitive style that favors empathizing over systemizing. We suggest this relationship may be a byproduct of greater gluteofemoral fat stores which are high in the essential fatty acids needed to support brain development and cellular functioning. It is interesting to note that our findings suggest lower WHR females, who are more likely to be targeted for dishonest courtship, may be better at identifying disingenuous claims of commitment.
身体形态是一种两性二态特征。在人类中,男性往往有较高的肩臀比。相反,女性的腰臀比(WHR)通常较低;也就是说,窄腰宽臀近似沙漏型。WHR低的女性被认为更有吸引力,更健康,更有生育能力。他们也倾向于拥有更迷人的声音,更早失去童贞,并且有更多的性伴侣。腰宽比也与一般认知能力有关。在本研究中,我们扩展了先前的研究,考察了腰宽比在认知中的作用。我们假设,更女性化的体型(以低腰宽比为指标)与女性“大脑类型”的认知测量有关,比如精神状态归因和同理心,因为这两者都可能取决于青春期雌激素的激活作用。我们发现,低whr的女性擅长识别他人的情绪状态,并表现出一种倾向于移情而不是系统化的认知风格。我们认为这种关系可能是臀股脂肪储存增加的副产品,臀股脂肪储存富含支持大脑发育和细胞功能所需的必需脂肪酸。有趣的是,我们的研究结果表明,WHR较低的女性,更有可能成为不诚实求爱的目标,可能更善于识别不诚实的承诺。
{"title":"Mental state attribution and body configuration in women.","authors":"Jennifer A Bremser,&nbsp;Gordon G Gallup","doi":"10.3389/fnevo.2012.00001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2012.00001","url":null,"abstract":"Body configuration is a sexually dimorphic trait. In humans, men tend to have high shoulder-to-hip ratios. Women in contrast, often have low waist-to-hip ratios (WHR); i.e., narrow waists and broad hips that approximate an hour-glass configuration. Women with low WHR’s are rated as more attractive, healthier, and more fertile. They also tend to have more attractive voices, lose their virginity sooner, and have more sex partners. WHR has also been linked with general cognitive performance. In the present study we expand upon previous research examining the role of WHR in cognition. We hypothesized that more feminine body types, as indexed by a low WHR, would be associated with cognitive measures of the female “brain type,” such as mental state attribution and empathy because both may depend upon the activational effects of estrogens at puberty. We found that women with low WHRs excel at identifying emotional states of other people and show a cognitive style that favors empathizing over systemizing. We suggest this relationship may be a byproduct of greater gluteofemoral fat stores which are high in the essential fatty acids needed to support brain development and cellular functioning. It is interesting to note that our findings suggest lower WHR females, who are more likely to be targeted for dishonest courtship, may be better at identifying disingenuous claims of commitment.","PeriodicalId":88241,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in evolutionary neuroscience","volume":"4 ","pages":"1"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2012-01-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3389/fnevo.2012.00001","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30447669","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 46
Changes in Physiology before, during, and after Yawning. 打哈欠前、中、后的生理变化。
Pub Date : 2012-01-03 eCollection Date: 2011-01-01 DOI: 10.3389/fnevo.2011.00007
Timothy P Corey, Melanie L Shoup-Knox, Elana B Gordis, Gordon G Gallup

The ultimate function of yawning continues to be debated. Here, we examine physiological measurements taken before, during, and after yawns in humans, in an attempt to identify key proximate mechanisms associated with this behavior. In two separate studies we measured changes in heart rate, lung volume, eye closure, skin conductance, ear pulse, respiratory sinus arrhythmia, and respiratory rate. Data were depicted from 75 s before and after yawns, and analyzed at baseline, during, and immediately following yawns. Increases in heart rate, lung volume, and eye muscle tension were observed during or immediately following yawning. Patterns of physiological changes during yawning were then compared to data from non-yawning deep inhalations. In one study, respiration period increased following the execution of a yawn. Much of the variance in physiology surrounding yawning was specific to the yawning event. This was not the case for deep inhalation. We consider our findings in light of various hypotheses about the function of yawning and conclude that they are most consistent with the brain cooling hypothesis.

打哈欠的最终功能仍在争论中。在这里,我们检查了人类打哈欠之前、期间和之后的生理测量,试图确定与这种行为相关的关键近似机制。在两个独立的研究中,我们测量了心率、肺容量、闭眼、皮肤电导、耳脉、呼吸性窦性心律失常和呼吸频率的变化。研究人员对打哈欠前后75秒的数据进行了描述,并对打哈欠时、基线和刚打哈欠时的数据进行了分析。在打哈欠期间或之后立即观察到心率、肺容量和眼肌张力的增加。然后将打呵欠时的生理变化模式与不打呵欠的深吸气的数据进行比较。在一项研究中,打哈欠后呼吸时间增加。许多与打哈欠有关的生理变化都与打哈欠事件有关。这与深度吸入的情况不同。我们根据关于打哈欠功能的各种假设来考虑我们的发现,并得出结论,它们与大脑冷却假说最一致。
{"title":"Changes in Physiology before, during, and after Yawning.","authors":"Timothy P Corey,&nbsp;Melanie L Shoup-Knox,&nbsp;Elana B Gordis,&nbsp;Gordon G Gallup","doi":"10.3389/fnevo.2011.00007","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00007","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The ultimate function of yawning continues to be debated. Here, we examine physiological measurements taken before, during, and after yawns in humans, in an attempt to identify key proximate mechanisms associated with this behavior. In two separate studies we measured changes in heart rate, lung volume, eye closure, skin conductance, ear pulse, respiratory sinus arrhythmia, and respiratory rate. Data were depicted from 75 s before and after yawns, and analyzed at baseline, during, and immediately following yawns. Increases in heart rate, lung volume, and eye muscle tension were observed during or immediately following yawning. Patterns of physiological changes during yawning were then compared to data from non-yawning deep inhalations. In one study, respiration period increased following the execution of a yawn. Much of the variance in physiology surrounding yawning was specific to the yawning event. This was not the case for deep inhalation. We consider our findings in light of various hypotheses about the function of yawning and conclude that they are most consistent with the brain cooling hypothesis.</p>","PeriodicalId":88241,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in evolutionary neuroscience","volume":"3 ","pages":"7"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2012-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00007","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30447667","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 26
Continuity, divergence, and the evolution of brain language pathways. 大脑语言通路的连续性、分化和进化。
Pub Date : 2012-01-03 eCollection Date: 2011-01-01 DOI: 10.3389/fnevo.2011.00011
James K Rilling, Matthew F Glasser, Saad Jbabdi, Jesper Andersson, Todd M Preuss
Recently, the assumption of evolutionary continuity between humans and non-human primates has been used to bolster the hypothesis that human language is mediated especially by the ventral extreme capsule pathway that mediates auditory object recognition in macaques. Here, we argue for the importance of evolutionary divergence in understanding brain language evolution. We present new comparative data reinforcing our previous conclusion that the dorsal arcuate fasciculus pathway was more significantly modified than the ventral extreme capsule pathway in human evolution. Twenty-six adult human and twenty-six adult chimpanzees were imaged with diffusion-weighted MRI and probabilistic tractography was used to track and compare the dorsal and ventral language pathways. Based on these and other data, we argue that the arcuate fasciculus is likely to be the pathway most essential for higher-order aspects of human language such as syntax and lexical–semantics.
最近,人类和非人类灵长类动物之间进化连续性的假设被用来支持人类语言介导的假设,特别是腹侧极端囊通路介导猕猴的听觉物体识别。在这里,我们认为进化分歧在理解大脑语言进化中的重要性。我们提供了新的比较数据来支持我们之前的结论,即在人类进化过程中,背弓状束通路比腹侧极端囊通路更明显地改变。对26名成年人和26名成年黑猩猩进行了弥散加权核磁共振成像成像,并使用概率神经束造影来跟踪和比较背侧和腹侧的语言通路。基于这些和其他数据,我们认为弓形神经束可能是人类语言的高阶方面(如语法和词汇语义)最重要的通路。
{"title":"Continuity, divergence, and the evolution of brain language pathways.","authors":"James K Rilling,&nbsp;Matthew F Glasser,&nbsp;Saad Jbabdi,&nbsp;Jesper Andersson,&nbsp;Todd M Preuss","doi":"10.3389/fnevo.2011.00011","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00011","url":null,"abstract":"Recently, the assumption of evolutionary continuity between humans and non-human primates has been used to bolster the hypothesis that human language is mediated especially by the ventral extreme capsule pathway that mediates auditory object recognition in macaques. Here, we argue for the importance of evolutionary divergence in understanding brain language evolution. We present new comparative data reinforcing our previous conclusion that the dorsal arcuate fasciculus pathway was more significantly modified than the ventral extreme capsule pathway in human evolution. Twenty-six adult human and twenty-six adult chimpanzees were imaged with diffusion-weighted MRI and probabilistic tractography was used to track and compare the dorsal and ventral language pathways. Based on these and other data, we argue that the arcuate fasciculus is likely to be the pathway most essential for higher-order aspects of human language such as syntax and lexical–semantics.","PeriodicalId":88241,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in evolutionary neuroscience","volume":"3 ","pages":"11"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2012-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00011","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30447668","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 151
The evolution of syntax: an exaptationist perspective. 语法的演变:一个解释主义的视角。
Pub Date : 2011-12-23 eCollection Date: 2011-01-01 DOI: 10.3389/fnevo.2011.00009
W Tecumseh Fitch

The evolution of language required elaboration of a number of independent mechanisms in the hominin lineage, including systems involved in signaling, semantics, and syntax. Two perspectives on the evolution of syntax can be contrasted. The "continuist" perspective seeks the evolutionary roots of complex human syntax in simpler combinatory systems used in animal communication systems, such as iteration and sequencing. The "exaptationist" perspective posits evolutionary change of function, so that systems today used for linguistic communication might previously have served quite different functions in earlier hominids. I argue that abundant biological evidence supports an exaptationist perspective, in general, and that it must be taken seriously when considering language evolution. When applied to syntax, this suggests that core computational components used today in language could have originally served non-linguistic functions such as motor control, non-verbal thought, or spatial reasoning. I outline three specific exaptationist hypotheses for spoken language. These three hypotheses each posit a change of functionality in a precursor circuit, and its transformation into a neural circuit or region specifically involved in language today. Hypothesis 1 suggests that the precursor mechanism for intentional vocal control, specifically direct cortical control over the larynx, was manual motor control subserved by the cortico-spinal tract. The second is that the arcuate fasciculus, which today connects syntactic and lexical regions, had its origin in intracortical connections subserving vocal imitation. The third is that the specialized components of Broca's area, specifically BA 45, had their origins in non-linguistic motor control, and specifically hierarchical planning of action. I conclude by illustrating the importance of both homology (studied via primates) and convergence (typically analyzed in birds) for testing such evolutionary hypotheses.

语言的进化需要古人类谱系中许多独立机制的阐述,包括涉及信号、语义和句法的系统。关于句法演变的两种观点可以进行对比。“持续论”的观点在动物交流系统中使用的简单组合系统(如迭代和排序)中寻找复杂人类语法的进化根源。“灭绝论者”的观点假定功能的进化变化,因此今天用于语言交流的系统在早期的原始人中可能具有完全不同的功能。我认为,总的来说,大量的生物学证据支持灭绝论者的观点,在考虑语言进化时,必须认真对待这一观点。当应用于语法时,这表明今天语言中使用的核心计算组件最初可能具有非语言功能,如运动控制、非语言思维或空间推理。我概述了口语的三个具体的解释主义假设。这三种假设都假定前体回路的功能发生了变化,并将其转化为与今天的语言特别相关的神经回路或区域。假设1表明,有意发声控制的前驱机制,特别是对喉部的直接皮层控制,是由皮质-脊髓束支持的手动运动控制。第二种说法是,今天连接句法和词汇区域的弓形神经束,起源于支持声音模仿的皮质内连接。第三种解释是,布洛卡区(Broca’s area)的专门组成部分,特别是ba45,起源于非语言运动控制,特别是行动的分层规划。最后,我举例说明了同源性(通过灵长类动物研究)和趋同性(通常在鸟类中分析)对于测试这些进化假设的重要性。
{"title":"The evolution of syntax: an exaptationist perspective.","authors":"W Tecumseh Fitch","doi":"10.3389/fnevo.2011.00009","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00009","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The evolution of language required elaboration of a number of independent mechanisms in the hominin lineage, including systems involved in signaling, semantics, and syntax. Two perspectives on the evolution of syntax can be contrasted. The \"continuist\" perspective seeks the evolutionary roots of complex human syntax in simpler combinatory systems used in animal communication systems, such as iteration and sequencing. The \"exaptationist\" perspective posits evolutionary change of function, so that systems today used for linguistic communication might previously have served quite different functions in earlier hominids. I argue that abundant biological evidence supports an exaptationist perspective, in general, and that it must be taken seriously when considering language evolution. When applied to syntax, this suggests that core computational components used today in language could have originally served non-linguistic functions such as motor control, non-verbal thought, or spatial reasoning. I outline three specific exaptationist hypotheses for spoken language. These three hypotheses each posit a change of functionality in a precursor circuit, and its transformation into a neural circuit or region specifically involved in language today. Hypothesis 1 suggests that the precursor mechanism for intentional vocal control, specifically direct cortical control over the larynx, was manual motor control subserved by the cortico-spinal tract. The second is that the arcuate fasciculus, which today connects syntactic and lexical regions, had its origin in intracortical connections subserving vocal imitation. The third is that the specialized components of Broca's area, specifically BA 45, had their origins in non-linguistic motor control, and specifically hierarchical planning of action. I conclude by illustrating the importance of both homology (studied via primates) and convergence (typically analyzed in birds) for testing such evolutionary hypotheses.</p>","PeriodicalId":88241,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in evolutionary neuroscience","volume":"3 ","pages":"9"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2011-12-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00009","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30356488","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 66
Maternal programming of reproductive function and behavior in the female rat. 雌性大鼠生殖功能和行为的母体编程。
Pub Date : 2011-12-22 eCollection Date: 2011-01-01 DOI: 10.3389/fnevo.2011.00010
Nicole M Cameron

Parental investment can be used as a forecast for the environmental conditions in which offspring will develop to adulthood. In the rat, maternal behavior is transmitted to the next generation through epigenetic modifications such as methylation and histone acetylation, resulting in variations in estrogen receptor alpha expression. Natural variations in maternal care also influence the sexual strategy adult females will adopt later in life. Lower levels of maternal care are associated with early onset of puberty as well as increased motivation to mate and greater receptivity toward males during mating. Lower levels of maternal care are also correlated with greater activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-gonadal axis, responsible for the expression of these behaviors. Contrary to the transition of maternal care, sexual behavior cannot simply be explained by maternal attention, since adoption studies changed the sexual phenotypes of offspring born to low caring mothers but not those from high caring dams. Indeed, mothers showing higher levels of licking/grooming have embryos that are exposed to high testosterone levels during development, and adoption studies suggest that this androgen exposure may protect their offspring from lower levels of maternal care. We propose that in the rat, maternal care and the in utero environment interact to influence the reproductive strategy female offspring display in adulthood and that this favors the species by allowing it to thrive under different environmental conditions.

亲代投资可以用来预测后代发育到成年的环境条件。在大鼠中,母性行为通过表观遗传修饰(如甲基化和组蛋白乙酰化)传递给下一代,导致雌激素受体α表达的变化。母性照顾的自然变化也影响成年女性在以后的生活中所采取的性策略。较低的母性护理水平与青春期提前、交配动机增加以及在交配过程中更容易接受男性有关。低水平的母性护理也与下丘脑-垂体-性腺轴的更大活动相关,下丘脑-垂体-性腺轴负责表达这些行为。与母性关爱的转变相反,性行为不能简单地用母性关注来解释,因为收养研究改变了低关爱母亲所生后代的性表型,而不是那些高关爱母亲所生后代的性表型。事实上,表现出较高舔/梳理水平的母亲的胚胎在发育过程中暴露于较高的睾丸激素水平,而收养研究表明,这种雄性激素暴露可能会保护它们的后代免受较低水平的母亲照顾。我们认为,在大鼠中,母性护理和子宫内环境相互作用,影响雌性后代成年后的生殖策略,这有利于物种在不同的环境条件下茁壮成长。
{"title":"Maternal programming of reproductive function and behavior in the female rat.","authors":"Nicole M Cameron","doi":"10.3389/fnevo.2011.00010","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00010","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Parental investment can be used as a forecast for the environmental conditions in which offspring will develop to adulthood. In the rat, maternal behavior is transmitted to the next generation through epigenetic modifications such as methylation and histone acetylation, resulting in variations in estrogen receptor alpha expression. Natural variations in maternal care also influence the sexual strategy adult females will adopt later in life. Lower levels of maternal care are associated with early onset of puberty as well as increased motivation to mate and greater receptivity toward males during mating. Lower levels of maternal care are also correlated with greater activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-gonadal axis, responsible for the expression of these behaviors. Contrary to the transition of maternal care, sexual behavior cannot simply be explained by maternal attention, since adoption studies changed the sexual phenotypes of offspring born to low caring mothers but not those from high caring dams. Indeed, mothers showing higher levels of licking/grooming have embryos that are exposed to high testosterone levels during development, and adoption studies suggest that this androgen exposure may protect their offspring from lower levels of maternal care. We propose that in the rat, maternal care and the in utero environment interact to influence the reproductive strategy female offspring display in adulthood and that this favors the species by allowing it to thrive under different environmental conditions.</p>","PeriodicalId":88241,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in evolutionary neuroscience","volume":"3 ","pages":"10"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2011-12-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00010","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30353290","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 49
Three billion years of Fatty Acid metabolism shape human cognitive performance. 30亿年的脂肪酸代谢塑造了人类的认知能力。
Pub Date : 2011-12-15 eCollection Date: 2011-01-01 DOI: 10.3389/fnevo.2011.00006
Paul M Nealen
Animal evolutionary history has progressed, in fits and starts, over several billions of years of changing environmental conditions on this planet. Ancient environments were strikingly different from modern conditions, and, in some cases, have left a permanent stamp on animal (including human) anatomy and physiology. Recently Lassek and Gaulin (2011) assessed the role which dietary fatty acids may play in shaping human cognitive performance, and in doing so, provide an intriguing glimpse into the evolution of animal nervous systems. Using a large sample of American children aged 6–16 from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey [Center for Disease Control (CDC), 1988–1994], Lassek and Gaulin explored the statistical relationships between human cognitive performance (assessed via standardized math and reading tests) and dietary components, while controlling for other measures of subjects’ social and environmental backgrounds (including lead exposure, family size and income, ethnicity). They found that dietary fatty acids, particularly n − 3 (“omega-3”) and n − 6 forms, were positively and negatively related, respectively, to cognition scores in both male and female children. The benefits of dietary n − 3 fatty acids were especially important for cognition in female children (Lassek and Gaulin, 2011). Both n − 3 and n − 6 fatty acids are essential nutrients that must be obtained from dietary sources, and our Western diets are known to have relatively low n − 3:n − 6 ratios (Blasbalg et al., 2011). Why their opposite utility? And why of differential importance for males and females? Lassek and Gaulin (2011) suggest explanations which are rooted in our evolutionary past. Our nervous systems contain a predominance of n − 3 fatty acids, which Lassek and Gaulin (2011) hypothesize is due to the fact that animal neurons first evolved in an environment rich in n − 3 fatty acids but limiting in n − 6 fatty acids. Under these conditions, n − 3 fatty acids became, and remain, critical for complete nervous development. Lassek and Gaulin cite corroborative evidence for the ability of n − 6 fatty acids to compete with n − 3 fatty acids for enzymatic access, suggesting that this competition is a direct mechanism for the differential utility of dietary n − 3 and n − 6 forms. Dietary intake of n − 3 fatty acids is shown by Lassek and Gaulin to be especially important for females, which they suggest is due to the fact that female children must partition their dietary intake of these essential nutrients for both their own use as well as toward fat stores for later use as a nutritive source for the provisioning of developing offspring. Here, too, an ancient evolutionary shaping of our animal parental roles continues to represent itself in our utilization of dietary components, with implications for cognitive performance. It is well understood that n − 3 fatty acids are profoundly bio-active in neural as well as other tissues, with influe
{"title":"Three billion years of Fatty Acid metabolism shape human cognitive performance.","authors":"Paul M Nealen","doi":"10.3389/fnevo.2011.00006","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00006","url":null,"abstract":"Animal evolutionary history has progressed, in fits and starts, over several billions of years of changing environmental conditions on this planet. Ancient environments were strikingly different from modern conditions, and, in some cases, have left a permanent stamp on animal (including human) anatomy and physiology. Recently Lassek and Gaulin (2011) assessed the role which dietary fatty acids may play in shaping human cognitive performance, and in doing so, provide an intriguing glimpse into the evolution of animal nervous systems. \u0000 \u0000Using a large sample of American children aged 6–16 from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey [Center for Disease Control (CDC), 1988–1994], Lassek and Gaulin explored the statistical relationships between human cognitive performance (assessed via standardized math and reading tests) and dietary components, while controlling for other measures of subjects’ social and environmental backgrounds (including lead exposure, family size and income, ethnicity). They found that dietary fatty acids, particularly n − 3 (“omega-3”) and n − 6 forms, were positively and negatively related, respectively, to cognition scores in both male and female children. The benefits of dietary n − 3 fatty acids were especially important for cognition in female children (Lassek and Gaulin, 2011). \u0000 \u0000Both n − 3 and n − 6 fatty acids are essential nutrients that must be obtained from dietary sources, and our Western diets are known to have relatively low n − 3:n − 6 ratios (Blasbalg et al., 2011). Why their opposite utility? And why of differential importance for males and females? Lassek and Gaulin (2011) suggest explanations which are rooted in our evolutionary past. \u0000 \u0000Our nervous systems contain a predominance of n − 3 fatty acids, which Lassek and Gaulin (2011) hypothesize is due to the fact that animal neurons first evolved in an environment rich in n − 3 fatty acids but limiting in n − 6 fatty acids. Under these conditions, n − 3 fatty acids became, and remain, critical for complete nervous development. Lassek and Gaulin cite corroborative evidence for the ability of n − 6 fatty acids to compete with n − 3 fatty acids for enzymatic access, suggesting that this competition is a direct mechanism for the differential utility of dietary n − 3 and n − 6 forms. Dietary intake of n − 3 fatty acids is shown by Lassek and Gaulin to be especially important for females, which they suggest is due to the fact that female children must partition their dietary intake of these essential nutrients for both their own use as well as toward fat stores for later use as a nutritive source for the provisioning of developing offspring. Here, too, an ancient evolutionary shaping of our animal parental roles continues to represent itself in our utilization of dietary components, with implications for cognitive performance. \u0000 \u0000It is well understood that n − 3 fatty acids are profoundly bio-active in neural as well as other tissues, with influe","PeriodicalId":88241,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in evolutionary neuroscience","volume":"3 ","pages":"6"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2011-12-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00006","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30334798","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Affective infrastructures: toward a cultural neuropsychology of sport. 情感基础设施:走向体育的文化神经心理学。
Pub Date : 2011-11-04 eCollection Date: 2011-01-01 DOI: 10.3389/fnevo.2011.00004
Leslie L Heywood

Recently there has been a turn toward considerations of embodiment, cognition, and context in sport studies. Many researchers have argued that the traditional focus on clinical psychology and performance enhancement within the discipline is incomplete, and now emphasize the importance of athletes' social and familial contexts in a research paradigm that examines interconnections between movement, cognition, emotion, and the social and cultural context in which movement takes place. While it is important that the sport studies focus is being expanded to consider these interactions, I will argue that this model is still incomplete in that it is missing a fundamental variable - that of our evolutionary neurobiological roots. I will use the work of affective neuroscientists Jaak Panksepp and Stephen Porges to show that because sport so clearly activates neural systems that function at both proximate and ultimate levels of causation, it can be seen to serve fundamental needs for affective balance. A neurobiology of affect shows how the evolution of the mammalian autonomic nervous system has resulted in neurophysiological substrates for affective processes and stress responses, and has wide-ranging implications for sport studies in terms of suggesting what forms of coaching might be the most effective in what context. I propose the term cultural neuropsychology of sport as a descriptor for a model that examines the relationships between neurophysiological substrates and athletes' social and familial contexts in terms of how these variables facilitate or fail to facilitate athletes' neuroceptions of safety, which in turn have a direct impact on their performance. A cultural neuropsychological model of sport might thereby be seen to elaborate a relationship between proximate and ultimate mechanisms in concretely applied ways.

最近,在体育研究中出现了对体现、认知和语境的考虑。许多研究人员认为,传统的关注临床心理学和学科内的表现增强是不完整的,现在强调运动员的社会和家庭背景的研究范式的重要性,检查运动,认知,情感和运动发生的社会和文化背景之间的相互联系。虽然体育研究的重点被扩展到考虑这些相互作用是很重要的,但我认为这个模型仍然是不完整的,因为它缺少一个基本的变量——我们进化的神经生物学根源。我将使用情感神经科学家Jaak Panksepp和Stephen Porges的研究成果来证明,由于运动如此清楚地激活了在因果关系的近因和终极水平上发挥作用的神经系统,它可以被视为服务于情感平衡的基本需求。情感的神经生物学展示了哺乳动物自主神经系统的进化如何导致情感过程和应激反应的神经生理基础,并对体育研究有广泛的影响,比如在什么情况下哪种形式的教练可能最有效。我提出“体育文化神经心理学”一词,作为一个模型的描述符,该模型研究了神经生理基础与运动员的社会和家庭背景之间的关系,即这些变量如何促进或不促进运动员的安全神经感觉,而安全神经感觉反过来又直接影响他们的表现。因此,体育的文化神经心理学模型可能被视为以具体应用的方式阐述了近因机制和最终机制之间的关系。
{"title":"Affective infrastructures: toward a cultural neuropsychology of sport.","authors":"Leslie L Heywood","doi":"10.3389/fnevo.2011.00004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00004","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recently there has been a turn toward considerations of embodiment, cognition, and context in sport studies. Many researchers have argued that the traditional focus on clinical psychology and performance enhancement within the discipline is incomplete, and now emphasize the importance of athletes' social and familial contexts in a research paradigm that examines interconnections between movement, cognition, emotion, and the social and cultural context in which movement takes place. While it is important that the sport studies focus is being expanded to consider these interactions, I will argue that this model is still incomplete in that it is missing a fundamental variable - that of our evolutionary neurobiological roots. I will use the work of affective neuroscientists Jaak Panksepp and Stephen Porges to show that because sport so clearly activates neural systems that function at both proximate and ultimate levels of causation, it can be seen to serve fundamental needs for affective balance. A neurobiology of affect shows how the evolution of the mammalian autonomic nervous system has resulted in neurophysiological substrates for affective processes and stress responses, and has wide-ranging implications for sport studies in terms of suggesting what forms of coaching might be the most effective in what context. I propose the term cultural neuropsychology of sport as a descriptor for a model that examines the relationships between neurophysiological substrates and athletes' social and familial contexts in terms of how these variables facilitate or fail to facilitate athletes' neuroceptions of safety, which in turn have a direct impact on their performance. A cultural neuropsychological model of sport might thereby be seen to elaborate a relationship between proximate and ultimate mechanisms in concretely applied ways.</p>","PeriodicalId":88241,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in evolutionary neuroscience","volume":"3 ","pages":"4"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2011-11-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00004","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30240839","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
期刊
Frontiers in evolutionary neuroscience
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1