首页 > 最新文献

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology最新文献

英文 中文
Mechanisms for polyandry evolution in a complex social bee 复杂社会性蜜蜂的多雄性进化机制
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-09 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03450-x
Keith Delaplane, Katherine Hagan, Kevin Vogel, Lewis Bartlett

Abstract

Polyandry in social Hymenoptera is associated with reduced within-colony relatedness and increased colony-level ecologic fitness. One explanation for this sees increasing within-nest genetic diversity as a mechanism for improving group task efficiency and colony competitiveness. A queen on her mating flight captures nearly 90% of her breeding population’s allele potential by her tenth effective mating (me ~ 10 males). Under this population allele capture (PAC) model, colony fitness gains track mating number in an asymptotic manner, leveling out after about the tenth mating. A supporting theory we call the genotype composition (GC) model sees genetic novelty at mating levels higher than the me ~ 10 asymptote, the hyperpolyandry zone, resulting from unique genotype compositions whose number are potentially infinite. Colony fitness gains under the GC model will track mating number in a linear manner. We set up field colonies with Apis mellifera queens each instrumentally mated with 1, 2, 4, 8, 16, or 32 males, creating a polyandry gradient bracketing the qualitative divide of me ~ 10, measured tokens of colony level fitness, and collected observation hive data. Our results lead us to conclude that (1) ancestral colony traits fundamental to eusociality (cooperative brood care) respond to mating level changes at or below me ~ 10 in a manner consistent with the PAC model, whereas (2) more derived specialized colony phenotypes (resistance to the non-native parasite Varroa destructor) continue improving with increasing me in a manner consistent with the GC model. By either model, (3) the mechanism for increasing colony fitness is an increase in worker task specialisms and task efficiency.

Significance statement

Polyandry is a female’s practice of mating with many males, storing their sperm, and using it to produce genetically diverse offspring. In complex social bees, a queen captures nearly 90% of her breeding population’s diversity potential by her tenth mating; however, queens in nature routinely mate with many more than ten males. We tested two models that, together, explain how social bee colonies ecologically benefit from queen mating numbers ranging from 2 to potential infinity. A population allele capture (PAC) model focuses on colony fitness gains at mating numbers at or below 10, and we provide evidence that it was at these polyandry levels that significant gains were made in an ancestral eusocial trait, cooperative brood care. A genotype composition (GC) model focuses on colony fitness gains at higher mating numbers, and we believe these gains are centered around more recently evolved ecologic specialisms such as parasite resistance.

摘要社会性膜翅目昆虫的多雄性与群内亲缘关系的降低和群级生态适应性的提高有关。对此的一种解释是,增加巢内遗传多样性是提高群体任务效率和群落竞争力的一种机制。一只蜂后在其第十次有效交配(我 ~ 10 只雄蜂)之前,会捕获其繁殖种群近 90% 的等位基因潜力。在这一群体等位基因捕获(PAC)模型下,蜂群适应性的提高以渐进的方式跟踪交配次数,大约在第十次交配后趋于平稳。我们称之为基因型组成(GC)模型的支持理论认为,在交配水平高于 me ~ 10 的渐近线(即超多雄区)时,遗传新颖性来自于独特的基因型组成,其数量可能是无限的。在 GC 模型下,蜂群的适应性收益将以线性方式跟踪交配数量。我们在野外建立了蜂群,每只蜂王分别与 1、2、4、8、16 或 32 只雄蜂进行工具交配,形成了 "我"~"10 "定性分界线范围内的多雄性梯度,测量了蜂群水平的适应性标记,并收集了观察蜂巢的数据。我们的研究结果使我们得出以下结论:(1)蜂群的祖先性状(合作育雏)对 me ~ 10 或更低的交配水平变化的反应与 PAC 模式一致,而(2)更多衍生的专门化蜂群表型(对非本地寄生虫破坏者 Varroa 的抵抗力)随着 me 的增加而继续改善,其方式与 GC 模式一致。无论采用哪种模式,(3) 提高蜂群适合度的机制都是提高工蜂的任务专业性和任务效率。重要意义声明多配偶制是雌蜂与许多雄蜂交配,储存雄蜂的精子,并利用这些精子培育出基因多样化的后代。在复杂的社会性蜜蜂中,蜂王在第十次交配时可获得其繁殖种群近 90% 的多样性潜力;然而,自然界中的蜂王通常会与十多只雄蜂交配。我们对两个模型进行了测试,这两个模型共同解释了社会蜂群如何从蜂王交配数量(从 2 到潜在的无穷大)中获得生态效益。种群等位基因捕获(PAC)模型侧重于交配数为 10 或低于 10 时蜂群的适应性收益,我们提供的证据表明,正是在这些多育水平上,祖先的社会性特征--合作育雏--获得了显著收益。基因型组成(GC)模型的重点是交配数较高时的蜂群适应性收益,我们认为这些收益主要集中在最近进化出的生态特异性上,如寄生虫抵抗力。
{"title":"Mechanisms for polyandry evolution in a complex social bee","authors":"Keith Delaplane, Katherine Hagan, Kevin Vogel, Lewis Bartlett","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03450-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03450-x","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Polyandry in social Hymenoptera is associated with reduced within-colony relatedness and increased colony-level ecologic fitness. One explanation for this sees increasing within-nest genetic diversity as a mechanism for improving group task efficiency and colony competitiveness. A queen on her mating flight captures nearly 90% of her breeding population’s allele potential by her tenth effective mating (<i>m</i><sub><i>e</i></sub> ~ 10 males). Under this population allele capture (PAC) model, colony fitness gains track mating number in an asymptotic manner, leveling out after about the tenth mating. A supporting theory we call the genotype composition (GC) model sees genetic novelty at mating levels higher than the <i>m</i><sub><i>e</i></sub> ~ 10 asymptote, the hyperpolyandry zone, resulting from unique genotype compositions whose number are potentially infinite. Colony fitness gains under the GC model will track mating number in a linear manner. We set up field colonies with <i>Apis mellifera</i> queens each instrumentally mated with 1, 2, 4, 8, 16, or 32 males, creating a polyandry gradient bracketing the qualitative divide of <i>m</i><sub><i>e</i></sub> ~ 10, measured tokens of colony level fitness, and collected observation hive data. Our results lead us to conclude that (1) ancestral colony traits fundamental to eusociality (cooperative brood care) respond to mating level changes at or below <i>m</i><sub><i>e</i></sub> ~ 10 in a manner consistent with the PAC model, whereas (2) more derived specialized colony phenotypes (resistance to the non-native parasite <i>Varroa destructor</i>) continue improving with increasing <i>m</i><sub><i>e</i></sub> in a manner consistent with the GC model. By either model, (3) the mechanism for increasing colony fitness is an increase in worker task specialisms and task efficiency.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>Polyandry is a female’s practice of mating with many males, storing their sperm, and using it to produce genetically diverse offspring. In complex social bees, a queen captures nearly 90% of her breeding population’s diversity potential by her tenth mating; however, queens in nature routinely mate with many more than ten males. We tested two models that, together, explain how social bee colonies ecologically benefit from queen mating numbers ranging from 2 to potential infinity. A population allele capture (PAC) model focuses on colony fitness gains at mating numbers at or below 10, and we provide evidence that it was at these polyandry levels that significant gains were made in an ancestral eusocial trait, cooperative brood care. A genotype composition (GC) model focuses on colony fitness gains at higher mating numbers, and we believe these gains are centered around more recently evolved ecologic specialisms such as parasite resistance.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140075460","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Performance and efficiency in leaf transport: unveiling the task allocation puzzle in Acromyrmex subterraneus 叶片运输的性能和效率:揭开亚特兰毛蟹任务分配之谜
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-08 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03454-7
Antônio Marcos Oliveira Toledo, Arthur Zimerer, Juliane Floriano Santos Lopes

The performance of ant workers in a given task can be highly variable, generating a non-homogenous workload in the colony. However, there is no information on whether high performance workers are more efficient, or have any morphometric and physiological variation that causes them to both start earlier and continue executing the task longer. Here, we demonstrate that non-homogeneous task distribution is common in small colonies and that different performance levels occur even among Acromyrmex subterraneus workers of the same size class. High Activity workers did more work and were faster, so they were more efficient than Low Activity workers. However, their efficiency was not related to their leg length, as it was for Low Activity workers. Also, the delay of the first loaded trip of High Activity workers was shorter than that of Low Activity workers, indicating an earlier response of the former to the task. Delay variation was not affected by mass-specific metabolic rate. Considering the first five trips, we found that the first trip had a longer duration than the others, and High Activity workers were faster than Low Activity workers, suggesting that the higher efficiency of High Activity workers may be related to the reinforcement mechanism, which in turn lessens their response threshold to the task. Finally, workers had similar mandible morphometry (length of the first and second tooth, number of teeth), and body mass components (water content, lean dry weight and lipid content) despite their activity category, indicating that these variables did not explain differences in performance or efficiency among workers. The hypometric mass scaling metabolic rate showed that Non-transporters had proportionally lower energy expenditure than other categories. High Activity workers showed remarkable performance, efficiency, and faster responses to foraging stimulus. We suggest that this heightened level of individual proficiency is in line with the threshold model, explaining the operation of the task allocation mechanism within the same worker size class.

蚂蚁工蚁在某项任务中的表现可能千差万别,从而在蚁群中产生不均匀的工作量。然而,目前还没有任何信息表明高绩效的工蚁是否更有效率,或者它们的形态和生理变化是否会导致它们更早地开始执行任务并持续更长时间。在这里,我们证明了在小型蚁群中非均质的任务分配是很常见的,而且即使是同一体型等级的亚特兰蛙工蚁也会出现不同的表现水平。高活跃度工蚁做的工作更多,速度更快,因此它们比低活跃度工蚁更有效率。不过,它们的效率与腿长无关,而低活动量工蚁的效率则与腿长有关。此外,高活动量工人第一次装载的延迟时间比低活动量工人短,这表明前者对任务的反应更早。延迟变化不受特定质量代谢率的影响。考虑到前五个行程,我们发现第一个行程的持续时间比其他行程长,而且高活动量工蚁比低活动量工蚁更快,这表明高活动量工蚁更高的效率可能与强化机制有关,强化机制进而降低了它们对任务的反应阈值。最后,尽管有不同的活动类别,但工蚁的下颚形态测量(第一和第二颗牙齿的长度、牙齿数量)和体质量成分(含水量、瘦干重和脂质含量)相似,这表明这些变量并不能解释工蚁之间的表现或效率差异。低计量质量缩放代谢率表明,非运输者的能量消耗比其他类别低。高活动量工蚁表现出色,效率高,对觅食刺激的反应更快。我们认为,这种个体熟练程度的提高符合阈值模型,可以解释同一工人体型类别中任务分配机制的运作。
{"title":"Performance and efficiency in leaf transport: unveiling the task allocation puzzle in Acromyrmex subterraneus","authors":"Antônio Marcos Oliveira Toledo, Arthur Zimerer, Juliane Floriano Santos Lopes","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03454-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03454-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The performance of ant workers in a given task can be highly variable, generating a non-homogenous workload in the colony. However, there is no information on whether high performance workers are more efficient, or have any morphometric and physiological variation that causes them to both start earlier and continue executing the task longer. Here, we demonstrate that non-homogeneous task distribution is common in small colonies and that different performance levels occur even among <i>Acromyrmex subterraneus</i> workers of the same size class. High Activity workers did more work and were faster, so they were more efficient than Low Activity workers. However, their efficiency was not related to their leg length, as it was for Low Activity workers. Also, the delay of the first loaded trip of High Activity workers was shorter than that of Low Activity workers, indicating an earlier response of the former to the task. Delay variation was not affected by mass-specific metabolic rate. Considering the first five trips, we found that the first trip had a longer duration than the others, and High Activity workers were faster than Low Activity workers, suggesting that the higher efficiency of High Activity workers may be related to the reinforcement mechanism, which in turn lessens their response threshold to the task. Finally, workers had similar mandible morphometry (length of the first and second tooth, number of teeth), and body mass components (water content, lean dry weight and lipid content) despite their activity category, indicating that these variables did not explain differences in performance or efficiency among workers. The hypometric mass scaling metabolic rate showed that Non-transporters had proportionally lower energy expenditure than other categories. High Activity workers showed remarkable performance, efficiency, and faster responses to foraging stimulus. We suggest that this heightened level of individual proficiency is in line with the threshold model, explaining the operation of the task allocation mechanism within the same worker size class.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140076270","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
‘Beware, I am large and dangerous’ – human listeners can be deceived by dynamic manipulation of the indexical content of agonistic dog growls 当心,我又大又危险"--人类听众可以通过动态操纵狗的咆哮声中的索引内容而上当受骗
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03452-9
Péter Pongrácz, Petra Dobos, Borbála Zsilák, Tamás Faragó, Bence Ferdinandy

Dishonest vocal signals about body size are rarely encountered, however, dogs are capable of modifying indexical information in their growls. This apparent acoustic body-size manipulation could be affected by the level of threat experienced by the dog. We tested whether this natural size manipulation actually affects how listeners assess the size of the dog, thus whether it could be considered as a successful indexical information manipulation. We requested human participants to assess dog growls, originally recorded when dogs encountered various ‘threatening strangers’ (of different sex, stature). The participants heard several sets of growl pairs, where they had to guess, which growl belonged to the ‘larger dog’. In the Control condition, dog growls originated from two different dogs in a pair; in the Test condition, growls of the same dog were presented pair by pair, always recorded in the presence of different threatening humans. Human listeners reliably picked the larger dog from two differently sized animals based on their growls alone. In the Test condition, participants thought that the dog was ‘larger’ when it was threatened by a female experimenter, and when the dog was growling at a larger sized human. We found that while growl length modulation was the main factor behind size-choice decisions in the case of female strangers, formant dispersion difference contributed the most when listeners chose which dog was the larger in the case of male opponents. Our results provide firsthand evidence of dogs’ functionally deceptive vocalizations towards humans, a phenomenon which has not been shown before in any interspecific scenario.

关于身体大小的不诚实声音信号很少遇到,但是,狗能够在咆哮声中修改索引信息。这种明显的体型声学操纵可能会受到狗所经历的威胁程度的影响。我们测试了这种自然的体型操纵是否会影响听者如何评估狗的体型,从而是否可以将其视为一种成功的索引信息操纵。我们要求人类参与者对狗的咆哮声进行评估,这些咆哮声最初是在狗遇到各种 "威胁性陌生人"(性别、身材各不相同)时录制的。参与者会听到几组成对的咆哮声,他们必须猜测哪种咆哮声属于 "较大的狗"。在 "对照组 "条件下,狗的咆哮声来自一对不同的狗;而在 "测试组 "条件下,同一只狗的咆哮声则是一对对地出现,而且总是在不同的威胁人类的情况下录制的。人类听者仅凭咆哮声就能从两只不同体型的动物中选出体型较大的狗。在 "测试 "条件下,当狗受到女性实验者的威胁时,以及当狗向体型较大的人类咆哮时,参与者会认为狗 "体型较大"。我们发现,在雌性陌生人的情况下,咆哮声长度调制是决定狗大小的主要因素,而在雄性对手的情况下,当听者选择哪只狗更大时,声调分散差异的作用最大。我们的研究结果提供了狗对人类进行功能性欺骗发声的第一手证据,这种现象以前从未在任何种间情景中出现过。
{"title":"‘Beware, I am large and dangerous’ – human listeners can be deceived by dynamic manipulation of the indexical content of agonistic dog growls","authors":"Péter Pongrácz, Petra Dobos, Borbála Zsilák, Tamás Faragó, Bence Ferdinandy","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03452-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03452-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Dishonest vocal signals about body size are rarely encountered, however, dogs are capable of modifying indexical information in their growls. This apparent acoustic body-size manipulation could be affected by the level of threat experienced by the dog. We tested whether this natural size manipulation actually affects how listeners assess the size of the dog, thus whether it could be considered as a successful indexical information manipulation. We requested human participants to assess dog growls, originally recorded when dogs encountered various ‘threatening strangers’ (of different sex, stature). The participants heard several sets of growl pairs, where they had to guess, which growl belonged to the ‘larger dog’. In the Control condition, dog growls originated from two different dogs in a pair; in the Test condition, growls of the same dog were presented pair by pair, always recorded in the presence of different threatening humans. Human listeners reliably picked the larger dog from two differently sized animals based on their growls alone. In the Test condition, participants thought that the dog was ‘larger’ when it was threatened by a female experimenter, and when the dog was growling at a larger sized human. We found that while growl length modulation was the main factor behind size-choice decisions in the case of female strangers, formant dispersion difference contributed the most when listeners chose which dog was the larger in the case of male opponents. Our results provide firsthand evidence of dogs’ functionally deceptive vocalizations towards humans, a phenomenon which has not been shown before in any interspecific scenario.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140005402","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Competitive asymmetries, birthing asynchrony and sibling rivalry in a social lizard 社会蜥蜴的竞争不对称、分娩不同步和同胞竞争
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-28 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03442-x
Alix Bouffet-Halle, Erik Wapstra, Geoffrey M. While

Abstract

Family life often involves interactions between individuals who have different fitness goals, leading to conflict. Resolution of this conflict is key for the stabilisation of family life. Here, we used a lizard, Liopholis whitii, that lives in facultative family groups to characterise the nature and extent of sibling conflict and test the role that individual and litter characteristics play in shaping conflict between family members. We found significant variation in conflict between family groups, specifically in relation to siblings. In approximately half of the litters, siblings were aggressive towards one another, while in the other half of litters, there was no aggression observed between siblings. There were no differences between aggressive and non-aggressive litters in the key factors predicted to mediate conflict, including sex, offspring size, or litter size. However, in aggressive litters, the maximum amount of within-litter conflict decreased with an increase in the spread between births of siblings. First-born offspring were significantly more likely to be aggressive towards their siblings compared to second and third born offspring. While one offspring was usually the target of that aggression, we found no evidence that any individual-level factor predicted who received aggression. In aggressive litters, aggressive offspring spent a greater amount of time with their mother compared to non-aggressive offspring. Similar asymmetries in the amount of time offspring spent with their mother between siblings were also observed in non-aggressive litters. Combined, our results suggest that birth order is the main driver of sibling conflict in aggressive litters in this facultatively social lizard species, suggesting that birthing asynchrony may provide females with a mechanism to manage conflict.

Significance statement

Conflict is a ubiquitous aspect of family life; it occurs between adults, between parents and offspring as well as between siblings. We show that the extent of conflict between siblings varies considerably within and between families in a lizard that exhibits prolonged associations between parents and offspring. We found no effects of offspring sex or size on within or between litter conflict. Instead, the number of days that passed between the birth of offspring appears to be the main factor that influences how much conflict there is between siblings. Furthermore, birth order was the main factor predicting which offspring was dominant. Combined this suggests that birthing asynchrony, the ability of females to spread out births across multiple days, may play a crucial role in the management of conflict in this system. As the moderation of conflict is crucial for the stabilisation of family life, these results provide important insights into the early evolution of social life.

摘要家庭生活往往涉及具有不同健康目标的个人之间的互动,从而导致冲突。解决这种冲突是稳定家庭生活的关键。在这里,我们利用一种生活在面生家庭群体中的蜥蜴--Liopholis whitii--来描述兄弟姐妹冲突的性质和程度,并检验个体和窝的特征在形成家庭成员间冲突中所起的作用。我们发现,家庭群体之间的冲突差异很大,尤其是兄弟姐妹之间的冲突。在大约一半的幼仔中,兄弟姐妹之间具有攻击性,而在另一半幼仔中,兄弟姐妹之间没有攻击性。攻击性幼仔和非攻击性幼仔之间在性别、后代大小或幼仔大小等预计会调解冲突的关键因素上没有差异。然而,在攻击性窝中,窝内冲突的最大值随着兄弟姐妹出生间隔的增加而减少。与第二胎和第三胎的后代相比,第一胎的后代更有可能对其兄弟姐妹采取攻击性行为。虽然通常有一个后代是攻击的目标,但我们没有发现任何个体水平的因素能预测谁会受到攻击。在具有攻击性的幼仔中,与不具有攻击性的幼仔相比,具有攻击性的幼仔与母亲在一起的时间更长。在非攻击性幼仔中,也观察到同胞之间后代与母亲相处时间的类似不对称性。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,出生顺序是这一表面社会性蜥蜴物种攻击性幼仔中兄弟姐妹冲突的主要驱动因素,这表明分娩不同步可能为雌性提供了一种管理冲突的机制。我们的研究表明,在一种父母与后代之间有长期联系的蜥蜴中,兄弟姐妹之间的冲突程度在家庭内部和家庭之间有很大的不同。我们发现,后代的性别或大小对同窝或同窝之间的冲突没有影响。相反,后代出生间隔天数似乎是影响兄弟姐妹间冲突程度的主要因素。此外,出生顺序也是预测哪个后代占优势的主要因素。综合这些因素,我们可以看出,在这个系统中,分娩不同步,即雌性在多天内分散分娩的能力,可能在冲突管理中起着至关重要的作用。由于冲突的缓和对家庭生活的稳定至关重要,这些结果为社会生活的早期演化提供了重要的启示。
{"title":"Competitive asymmetries, birthing asynchrony and sibling rivalry in a social lizard","authors":"Alix Bouffet-Halle, Erik Wapstra, Geoffrey M. While","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03442-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03442-x","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Family life often involves interactions between individuals who have different fitness goals, leading to conflict. Resolution of this conflict is key for the stabilisation of family life. Here, we used a lizard, <i>Liopholis whitii</i>, that lives in facultative family groups to characterise the nature and extent of sibling conflict and test the role that individual and litter characteristics play in shaping conflict between family members. We found significant variation in conflict between family groups, specifically in relation to siblings. In approximately half of the litters, siblings were aggressive towards one another, while in the other half of litters, there was no aggression observed between siblings. There were no differences between aggressive and non-aggressive litters in the key factors predicted to mediate conflict, including sex, offspring size, or litter size. However, in aggressive litters, the maximum amount of within-litter conflict decreased with an increase in the spread between births of siblings. First-born offspring were significantly more likely to be aggressive towards their siblings compared to second and third born offspring. While one offspring was usually the target of that aggression, we found no evidence that any individual-level factor predicted who received aggression. In aggressive litters, aggressive offspring spent a greater amount of time with their mother compared to non-aggressive offspring. Similar asymmetries in the amount of time offspring spent with their mother between siblings were also observed in non-aggressive litters. Combined, our results suggest that birth order is the main driver of sibling conflict in aggressive litters in this facultatively social lizard species, suggesting that birthing asynchrony may provide females with a mechanism to manage conflict.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>Conflict is a ubiquitous aspect of family life; it occurs between adults, between parents and offspring as well as between siblings. We show that the extent of conflict between siblings varies considerably within and between families in a lizard that exhibits prolonged associations between parents and offspring. We found no effects of offspring sex or size on within or between litter conflict. Instead, the number of days that passed between the birth of offspring appears to be the main factor that influences how much conflict there is between siblings. Furthermore, birth order was the main factor predicting which offspring was dominant. Combined this suggests that birthing asynchrony, the ability of females to spread out births across multiple days, may play a crucial role in the management of conflict in this system. As the moderation of conflict is crucial for the stabilisation of family life, these results provide important insights into the early evolution of social life.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140005225","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Choosing a bigger opponent: the path to victory and greater gains for the small contestant 选择更大的对手:小选手的胜利之路和更大收益
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-28 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03447-6
Luis M. Burciaga, Guillermina Alcaraz

Abstract

Animals often engage in contests over limited resources. The probability of winning a contest is primarily determined by the individual's fighting ability relative to its opponent and the perceived value of the disputed resource. Individuals of the hermit crab Calcinus californiensis frequently fight over gastropod shells. We conducted a free-choice experiment to assess the factors that determine individuals’ choice of opponent, contest initiation, and contest resolution. We placed hermit crabs occupying two shell species into a large container and monitored agonistic interactions between crabs. We assessed the asymmetries in fighting ability based on the differences in body mass between the opponents. The shell species and fit (i.e., shell size relative to crab body size) were used as measures of the objective and subjective resource value, respectively. Motivation influenced contest initiation; the crabs occupying too-tight shells were more likely to initiate a contest than the ones in looser-fitting shells. In most cases, the attackers fought for a shell with a looser fit, even if that meant losing a tighter-fitting shell of the preferred shell species. The fighting success for attackers was positively associated with the number of bouts of rapping and the shell size improvement. However, success was negatively correlated with body size asymmetry; attackers that chose opponents larger than themselves were more likely to evict their opponent than the attackers that chose opponents that were smaller. Experimental designs that allow animals to select their own opponents, rather than assigning specific opponents, can strongly contribute to knowledge of agonistic interactions.

Significance statement

Fighting dynamics are commonly assessed by estimating the influence of the resource-holding potential and resource value in pair-matched opponents. Here, however, we examined the influence of asymmetries in resource-holding potential and the objective and subjective value of gastropod shells on the choice of a contender and contest resolution through a free-choice opponent experiment in hermit crabs. The contest initiation was driven by the motivation to obtain a better gastropod shell species or one with a better size fit—factors which are known to improve individual fitness. Furthermore, fighting success was associated with an individual's persistence in displaying aggressive behaviors and its motivation to obtain a better resource. However, contrary to the predictions of game theoretical models, fighting success was higher when attackers chose opponents larger than themselves. Our results highlight the relevance of assessing fighting under more natural conditions by allowing animals to select their opponents.

摘要 动物经常会为了争夺有限的资源而展开较量。争夺获胜的概率主要取决于个体相对于对手的战斗力以及对争议资源的认知价值。加州寄居蟹(Calcinus californiensis)的个体经常为争夺腹足类动物的壳而打斗。我们进行了一次自由选择实验,以评估决定个体选择对手、开始争夺和解决争夺的因素。我们将占据两种贝壳的寄居蟹放入一个大容器中,并监测寄居蟹之间的争斗互动。我们根据对手身体质量的差异评估了战斗能力的不对称性。壳的种类和适合度(即壳的大小相对于蟹的体型)分别被用来衡量客观和主观资源价值。螃蟹的动机会影响竞争的开始;壳太紧的螃蟹比壳太松的螃蟹更有可能开始竞争。在大多数情况下,攻击者会争夺一个更宽松的壳,即使这意味着要失去一个更紧身的壳。攻击者的战斗成功率与拍击次数和贝壳大小的改善呈正相关。然而,成功与否与体型不对称呈负相关;选择比自己大的对手的攻击者比选择比自己小的对手的攻击者更有可能驱逐对手。让动物自己选择对手,而不是指定特定对手的实验设计,可以极大地促进对激动式相互作用的了解。意义声明通常通过估计成对匹配对手的资源占有潜力和资源价值的影响来评估战斗动态。然而,在这里,我们通过寄居蟹自由选择对手实验,考察了资源占有潜力和腹足类贝壳的主客观价值的不对称性对竞争者选择和竞争解决的影响。开始竞争的动机是为了获得更好的腹足类贝壳种类或更好的大小适合度--众所周知,这些因素可以提高个体的适应性。此外,搏斗的成功与个体持续表现出的攻击行为及其获得更好资源的动机有关。然而,与博弈论模型的预测相反,当攻击者选择比自己大的对手时,战斗成功率更高。我们的研究结果凸显了在更自然的条件下通过让动物选择对手来评估打斗的意义。
{"title":"Choosing a bigger opponent: the path to victory and greater gains for the small contestant","authors":"Luis M. Burciaga, Guillermina Alcaraz","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03447-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03447-6","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Animals often engage in contests over limited resources. The probability of winning a contest is primarily determined by the individual's fighting ability relative to its opponent and the perceived value of the disputed resource. Individuals of the hermit crab <i>Calcinus californiensis</i> frequently fight over gastropod shells. We conducted a free-choice experiment to assess the factors that determine individuals’ choice of opponent, contest initiation, and contest resolution. We placed hermit crabs occupying two shell species into a large container and monitored agonistic interactions between crabs. We assessed the asymmetries in fighting ability based on the differences in body mass between the opponents. The shell species and fit (i.e., shell size relative to crab body size) were used as measures of the objective and subjective resource value, respectively. Motivation influenced contest initiation; the crabs occupying too-tight shells were more likely to initiate a contest than the ones in looser-fitting shells. In most cases, the attackers fought for a shell with a looser fit, even if that meant losing a tighter-fitting shell of the preferred shell species. The fighting success for attackers was positively associated with the number of bouts of rapping and the shell size improvement. However, success was negatively correlated with body size asymmetry; attackers that chose opponents larger than themselves were more likely to evict their opponent than the attackers that chose opponents that were smaller. Experimental designs that allow animals to select their own opponents, rather than assigning specific opponents, can strongly contribute to knowledge of agonistic interactions.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>Fighting dynamics are commonly assessed by estimating the influence of the resource-holding potential and resource value in pair-matched opponents. Here, however, we examined the influence of asymmetries in resource-holding potential and the objective and subjective value of gastropod shells on the choice of a contender and contest resolution through a free-choice opponent experiment in hermit crabs. The contest initiation was driven by the motivation to obtain a better gastropod shell species or one with a better size fit—factors which are known to improve individual fitness. Furthermore, fighting success was associated with an individual's persistence in displaying aggressive behaviors and its motivation to obtain a better resource. However, contrary to the predictions of game theoretical models, fighting success was higher when attackers chose opponents larger than themselves. Our results highlight the relevance of assessing fighting under more natural conditions by allowing animals to select their opponents.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140005796","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Experimental quantification of genetic and ontogenetic effects on fighting behavior in the broad-horned flour beetle 通过实验量化宽角面粉甲虫的遗传和个体发育对打斗行为的影响
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-24 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03451-w
Toshiki Nishitani, Kentarou Matsumura, Erik Postma, Manmohan Dev Sharma, David J Hosken, Takahisa Miyatake

Most animal behaviors show large within- and among-individual variation, and this includes competitive male behaviors. With male fighting for example, aggressiveness often correlates with dominance, and contest duration varies with age. However, few studies have directly quantified how mean aggressiveness and contest duration, the variation among individuals in both traits, and the relationship among them, vary with age. Here we address these gaps and examine the effect of male age and genotype on two key aspects of male fighting behavior - aggressiveness (here measured as latency to fight) and contest duration - and the relationship between them. We do this using isogenic lines of the broad-horned flour beetle Gnatocerus cornutus. We observed fighting behavior of paired males of similar body size and age. Using uni- and multivariate mixed models, we show that although there was a significant difference between younger and older males in contest duration, mean aggressiveness was not affected by male age. However, the variation in aggression and fight duration varied with age, being greater in younger and older males respectively. Additionally, although there was a positive correlation between aggressiveness and contest duration in younger males, this relationship was not found in older males. Finally, the only significant genetic effect was for aggression in younger males. Our study shows that age differentially shapes key components of male fighting behavior as well as the relationship among them, highlighting the dynamic nature and context-dependence of fighting.

大多数动物行为在个体内部和个体之间都有很大差异,雄性动物的竞争行为也不例外。以雄性争斗为例,攻击性通常与优势相关,争斗持续时间随年龄而变化。然而,很少有研究直接量化平均攻击性和争斗持续时间、个体间这两个特征的变化以及它们之间的关系是如何随年龄变化的。在这里,我们针对这些空白,研究了雄性年龄和基因型对雄性格斗行为的两个关键方面--攻击性(这里用格斗潜伏期来衡量)和较量持续时间--的影响,以及它们之间的关系。我们利用宽角面粉甲虫(Gnatocerus cornutus)的同源系来进行研究。我们观察了体型和年龄相似的配对雄性的打斗行为。通过使用单变量和多变量混合模型,我们发现虽然年轻雄性和年长雄性在较量持续时间上存在显著差异,但平均攻击性并不受雄性年龄的影响。然而,攻击性和搏斗持续时间随年龄的变化而变化,年轻雄性和年长雄性的攻击性和搏斗持续时间分别更大。此外,虽然年轻雄性的攻击性与比赛持续时间呈正相关,但在年长雄性中却没有发现这种关系。最后,唯一显著的遗传效应是年轻雄性的攻击性。我们的研究表明,年龄对雄性格斗行为的关键因素以及它们之间的关系有着不同的影响,这突出了格斗的动态性质和环境依赖性。
{"title":"Experimental quantification of genetic and ontogenetic effects on fighting behavior in the broad-horned flour beetle","authors":"Toshiki Nishitani, Kentarou Matsumura, Erik Postma, Manmohan Dev Sharma, David J Hosken, Takahisa Miyatake","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03451-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03451-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Most animal behaviors show large within- and among-individual variation, and this includes competitive male behaviors. With male fighting for example, aggressiveness often correlates with dominance, and contest duration varies with age. However, few studies have directly quantified how mean aggressiveness and contest duration, the variation among individuals in both traits, and the relationship among them, vary with age. Here we address these gaps and examine the effect of male age and genotype on two key aspects of male fighting behavior - aggressiveness (here measured as latency to fight) and contest duration - and the relationship between them. We do this using isogenic lines of the broad-horned flour beetle <i>Gnatocerus cornutus</i>. We observed fighting behavior of paired males of similar body size and age. Using uni- and multivariate mixed models, we show that although there was a significant difference between younger and older males in contest duration, mean aggressiveness was not affected by male age. However, the variation in aggression and fight duration varied with age, being greater in younger and older males respectively. Additionally, although there was a positive correlation between aggressiveness and contest duration in younger males, this relationship was not found in older males. Finally, the only significant genetic effect was for aggression in younger males. Our study shows that age differentially shapes key components of male fighting behavior as well as the relationship among them, highlighting the dynamic nature and context-dependence of fighting.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139954339","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No sign of reproductive cessation in the old workers of a queenless ponerine ant 无王椿树蚁的老工蚁没有生殖停止的迹象
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-23 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03445-8
Kodai Kishino, Keiko Sakiyama, Haruna Fujioka, Yasukazu Okada

Abstract

In many social hymenopterans, workers of different ages engage in different tasks; younger workers remain inside the nest as intranidal workers, while older workers go outside the nest as extranidal workers (i.e., age polyethism). Previous studies have shown that ovarian activity is diminished in old, extranidal workers, but it remains unclear whether workers’ reproductive ability persists for life or whether they exhibit post-reproductive lifespans. In this study, we investigated the age-dependence of worker reproductive ability in a monomorphic ponerine ant Diacamma cf. indicum. In Diacamma ants, all females in a colony have reproductive ability, but effective reproduction is limited to one or a few dominant females, and the remaining females act as sterile helpers. Using long-term laboratory rearing, we investigated whether worker reproductive ability lasts throughout a worker’s lifetime. The ages of workers were accurately tracked, and the reproductive ability of young and old workers was examined by creating several gamergate-less sub-colonies. Results showed that at least one individual in each sub-colony developed ovaries, even in the sub-colonies that solely consisted of very old workers (> 252 days old). Interestingly, in the presence of younger workers, old workers rarely showed ovarian development. Besides age, we found a positive correlation between the amount of fat (i.e., nutritional condition) and ovarian development in old workers. Our data suggest that reproductive activity of old workers is low but maintained throughout their life in Diacamma.

Significance statement

Females of social animals, such as cetaceans, are known to stop reproducing before the end of their lifespan. It is suggested that the reproductive ability of ant queens does not decline and is maintained throughout their lives; however, it is unclear whether this is also true in ant workers. We maintained ant colonies for more than 500 days and then tested whether the reproductive ability of ant workers is maintained throughout their lifespan. Even in small groups composed of only very old workers (> 252 days old), at least one individual in each group always reproduced actively. Interestingly, the presence of young workers seemed to suppress the reproduction of old workers. In addition, fat content was positively associated with the individual’s reproductive potential.

摘要 在许多社会性膜翅目昆虫中,不同年龄的工蜂从事不同的任务;年轻的工蜂留在巢内,成为巢内工蜂,而年长的工蜂则走出巢外,成为巢外工蜂(即年龄多态性)。以前的研究表明,年老的潮间带外工蚁的卵巢活动会减弱,但工蚁的生殖能力是否终生存在,或者它们是否表现出生殖后寿命,目前仍不清楚。在这项研究中,我们调查了单形绒毛蚁 Diacamma cf. indicum 工蚁生殖能力的年龄依赖性。在Diacamma蚂蚁中,蚁群中的所有雌性都具有繁殖能力,但有效繁殖仅限于一只或几只优势雌性,其余雌性则充当不育帮手。通过长期实验室饲养,我们研究了工蚁的生殖能力是否贯穿工蚁的一生。我们对工蜂的年龄进行了精确的追踪,并通过建立几个无配子门的子群来考察年轻和年老工蜂的繁殖能力。结果表明,每个子群中至少有一个个体发育出卵巢,即使是在仅由非常老的工蚁(252天)组成的子群中也是如此。有趣的是,在有年轻工蜂存在的情况下,老工蜂很少出现卵巢发育。除年龄外,我们还发现老龄工蚁的脂肪量(即营养状况)与卵巢发育呈正相关。我们的数据表明,在 Diacamma 中,老龄工蚁的生殖活动较低,但终其一生都能保持生殖活动。意义声明众所周知,鲸目动物等社会性动物的雌性会在寿命结束前停止生殖。有研究表明,蚁后的繁殖能力不会衰退,而且会终生保持;但目前还不清楚蚂蚁工蚁是否也是如此。我们将蚂蚁群落维持了 500 多天,然后测试了蚂蚁工蚁的生殖能力是否在其整个生命周期中都能保持。即使是由非常年老的工蚁(252 天)组成的小群体,每个群体中至少有一个个体总是在积极繁殖。有趣的是,年轻工蚁的存在似乎抑制了老工蚁的繁殖。此外,脂肪含量与个体的繁殖潜力呈正相关。
{"title":"No sign of reproductive cessation in the old workers of a queenless ponerine ant","authors":"Kodai Kishino, Keiko Sakiyama, Haruna Fujioka, Yasukazu Okada","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03445-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03445-8","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>In many social hymenopterans, workers of different ages engage in different tasks; younger workers remain inside the nest as intranidal workers, while older workers go outside the nest as extranidal workers (i.e., age polyethism). Previous studies have shown that ovarian activity is diminished in old, extranidal workers, but it remains unclear whether workers’ reproductive ability persists for life or whether they exhibit post-reproductive lifespans. In this study, we investigated the age-dependence of worker reproductive ability in a monomorphic ponerine ant <i>Diacamma</i> cf. <i>indicum</i>. In <i>Diacamma</i> ants, all females in a colony have reproductive ability, but effective reproduction is limited to one or a few dominant females, and the remaining females act as sterile helpers. Using long-term laboratory rearing, we investigated whether worker reproductive ability lasts throughout a worker’s lifetime. The ages of workers were accurately tracked, and the reproductive ability of young and old workers was examined by creating several gamergate-less sub-colonies. Results showed that at least one individual in each sub-colony developed ovaries, even in the sub-colonies that solely consisted of very old workers (&gt; 252 days old). Interestingly, in the presence of younger workers, old workers rarely showed ovarian development. Besides age, we found a positive correlation between the amount of fat (i.e., nutritional condition) and ovarian development in old workers. Our data suggest that reproductive activity of old workers is low but maintained throughout their life in <i>Diacamma</i>.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>Females of social animals, such as cetaceans, are known to stop reproducing before the end of their lifespan. It is suggested that the reproductive ability of ant queens does not decline and is maintained throughout their lives; however, it is unclear whether this is also true in ant workers. We maintained ant colonies for more than 500 days and then tested whether the reproductive ability of ant workers is maintained throughout their lifespan. Even in small groups composed of only very old workers (&gt; 252 days old), at least one individual in each group always reproduced actively. Interestingly, the presence of young workers seemed to suppress the reproduction of old workers. In addition, fat content was positively associated with the individual’s reproductive potential.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139954581","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Behavioral repertoire of Lahille’s bottlenose dolphins that interact with artisanal fishers 与个体渔民互动的拉氏瓶鼻海豚的行为曲目
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-23 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03443-w

Abstract

Many human cultures involve positive interactions with wildlife in the past and present. Lahille’s bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops gephyreus), for example, have developed tactics for coastal and estuarine foraging, which sustains a fishing practice known as “cooperative fishing” by traditional fishers in estuaries of southern Brazil. Here, we use aerial footage to describe the behavioral repertoire of the Lahille’s bottlenose dolphins and how it relates to the frequency of net casting by fishers in the Tramandaí Inlet. From nearly 8 h of footage from June 2017 to May 2018, we mainly observed dolphins foraging in the estuary inlet when fishers were present along the shoreline. Dolphins performed at least 27 clearly distinct behaviors and three types of movement patterns. A generalized additive model supported that the fishers interpret a subset of this repertoire (64%) as cues for casting their nets. The behavioral overview of the Lahille’s bottlenose dolphins presented here demonstrates not only a diverse repertoire for this population, but also its clear influence on fishers’ activities. Scientific and traditional perspectives should be integrated to better understand the ecological significance of this “cooperative fishing” for both dolphin populations and fishers that depend on them.

Significance statement

Interactions between different species can be very complex. It is a three-dimensional universe that comprises the behavioral and ecological characteristics of both sides, and then the dynamic they create together. In the case of the “cooperative fishing” between fishers and dolphins in southern Brazil, we still have one dimension to disclose in detail: the dolphins’ behavioral repertoire. Using aerial videos from a drone, we were able to shed light on how those dolphins behave and also how fishers coordinate their activity in response to the dolphins. We also showed that the dolphins’ behavioral repertoire is more diverse than assumed so far. Results suggest that the “cooperative fishing” seems to be based on the human’s perception of an extensive Lahille’s bottlenose dolphin behavioral repertoire during hunting. The knowledge provided here is essential to track the dynamic of this unique interaction in a given space and time.

摘要 在过去和现在,许多人类文化都涉及与野生动物的积极互动。例如,拉氏瓶鼻海豚(Tursiops gephyreus)已经形成了在沿海和河口觅食的策略,这种策略维持了巴西南部河口传统渔民称为 "合作捕鱼 "的捕鱼方式。在这里,我们利用航拍镜头来描述拉氏瓶鼻海豚的行为剧目,以及它与特拉曼达伊海湾渔民撒网频率的关系。从 2017 年 6 月至 2018 年 5 月近 8 小时的录像中,我们主要观察到海豚在河口入海口觅食时,捕鱼者出现在海岸线上。海豚至少有 27 种明显不同的行为和 3 种运动模式。广义相加模型证明,渔民将这些行为中的一部分(64%)解释为撒网的提示。本文介绍的拉氏瓶鼻海豚行为概述不仅展示了这一种群的多种行为,而且还展示了其对渔民活动的明显影响。应将科学和传统观点结合起来,以更好地理解这种 "合作捕鱼 "对海豚种群和依赖它们的渔民的生态意义。 意义声明 不同物种之间的相互作用可能非常复杂。这是一个由双方的行为和生态特征以及它们共同创造的动态组成的三维世界。就巴西南部渔民与海豚之间的 "合作捕鱼 "而言,我们仍有一个维度需要详细披露:海豚的行为剧目。利用无人机拍摄的空中视频,我们得以揭示这些海豚的行为方式,以及渔民如何协调他们的活动以应对海豚。我们还发现,海豚的行为剧目比目前假设的更加多样化。研究结果表明,"合作捕鱼 "似乎是基于人类对拉氏瓶鼻海豚在捕猎过程中广泛行为的感知。这里提供的知识对于在特定空间和时间内追踪这种独特互动的动态至关重要。
{"title":"Behavioral repertoire of Lahille’s bottlenose dolphins that interact with artisanal fishers","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03443-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03443-w","url":null,"abstract":"<span> <h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Many human cultures involve positive interactions with wildlife in the past and present. Lahille’s bottlenose dolphins (<em>Tursiops gephyreus</em>), for example, have developed tactics for coastal and estuarine foraging, which sustains a fishing practice known as “cooperative fishing” by traditional fishers in estuaries of southern Brazil. Here, we use aerial footage to describe the behavioral repertoire of the Lahille’s bottlenose dolphins and how it relates to the frequency of net casting by fishers in the Tramandaí Inlet. From nearly 8 h of footage from June 2017 to May 2018, we mainly observed dolphins foraging in the estuary inlet when fishers were present along the shoreline. Dolphins performed at least 27 clearly distinct behaviors and three types of movement patterns. A generalized additive model supported that the fishers interpret a subset of this repertoire (64%) as cues for casting their nets. The behavioral overview of the Lahille’s bottlenose dolphins presented here demonstrates not only a diverse repertoire for this population, but also its clear influence on fishers’ activities. Scientific and traditional perspectives should be integrated to better understand the ecological significance of this “cooperative fishing” for both dolphin populations and fishers that depend on them.</p> </span> <span> <h3>Significance statement</h3> <p>Interactions between different species can be very complex. It is a three-dimensional universe that comprises the behavioral and ecological characteristics of both sides, and then the dynamic they create together. In the case of the “cooperative fishing” between fishers and dolphins in southern Brazil, we still have one dimension to disclose in detail: the dolphins’ behavioral repertoire. Using aerial videos from a drone, we were able to shed light on how those dolphins behave and also how fishers coordinate their activity in response to the dolphins. We also showed that the dolphins’ behavioral repertoire is more diverse than assumed so far. Results suggest that the “cooperative fishing” seems to be based on the human’s perception of an extensive Lahille’s bottlenose dolphin behavioral repertoire during hunting. The knowledge provided here is essential to track the dynamic of this unique interaction in a given space and time.</p> </span>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139954340","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Diving behavior in semi-aquatic Anolis lizards results in heat loss with sex-specific cooling tolerance 半水栖巨蜥的潜水行为导致热量损失,其耐冷性因性别而异
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-23 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03448-5
Alexandra M. Martin, Christopher K. Boccia, Lindsey Swierk

Abstract

Males and females often differ in use of antipredator behaviors, particularly when antipredator behavior comes at the cost of missed mating opportunities or territory defense. When using thermally suboptimal refugia, ectotherms are especially vulnerable to these costs, as their performance is linked to body temperature. To flee from predators, semi-aquatic Anolis lizards dive underwater for long periods and rebreathe from a bubble of air. We hypothesized that using aquatic refugia would result in body heat loss, that dive duration is influenced by sex, and that oxygen consumption when diving would help explain sex differences. We tested these hypotheses by measuring dive length and body temperatures in A. aquaticus, and by recording oxygen consumption and final oxygen partial pressure during controlled dives in several semi-aquatic Anolis species. Not only was there a significant thermal cost to diving, but A. aquaticus males and females appeared to tolerate different levels of this cost: males re-emerged from water more quickly and at higher body temperatures than did females. Body temperature decreased according to an exponential decay function, dropping up to 6 °C in 5 min. Oxygen consumption rates in semi-aquatic anoles were primarily explained by the expected allometric scaling relationship with mass and, therefore, are unlikely to lead to sex differences in physiological limits to dive times. Instead, shorter male dives may help them maintain physiological performance, mating opportunities or territory defense. Antipredator diving behavior is physiologically costly but undoubtedly beneficial to both sexes, highlighting the need for further study of sex-based antipredator optimization.

Significance statement

To avoid predators, semi-aquatic Anolis lizards can dive underwater and remain there for an extended time by rebreathing a bubble of air over their heads. In this study, we reveal that diving to escape predators also comes with a cost: submersion in water reduces lizard body temperatures. Reduced body temperature can impair a lizard’s ability to move quickly and defend mates or territories, suggesting that there may be divergent diving behaviors in males and females. Our findings confirm that males do indeed spend less time underwater than females. We measured oxygen consumption during dives, and our data suggest that sex differences in diving behavior are unrelated to oxygen use. This study sheds light on the sex-specific balance of antipredator behaviors and the maintenance of optimal body temperatures, and more broadly contributes insight into adaptive responses to environmental challenges.

摘要雄性和雌性在使用反捕食者行为方面常常存在差异,特别是当反捕食者行为以错失交配机会或领地防御为代价时。外温动物在使用温度不理想的避难所时尤其容易受到这些代价的影响,因为它们的表现与体温有关。为了逃离捕食者,半水栖的Anolis蜥蜴会长期潜入水下,并从气泡中呼吸。我们假设,利用水生避难所会导致体热损失,潜水持续时间受性别影响,潜水时的耗氧量有助于解释性别差异。我们通过测量水栖蚁的潜水时间和体温,以及记录几种半水栖蚁在受控潜水过程中的耗氧量和最终氧分压,对这些假设进行了验证。潜水不仅需要付出巨大的热代价,而且雌雄水龙似乎能承受不同程度的热代价:雄性水龙比雌性水龙出水更快,体温更高。体温按指数衰减函数下降,5 分钟内最高下降 6 °C。半水栖鳗鱼的耗氧率主要是通过预期的与质量的等比例关系来解释的,因此不太可能导致潜水时间生理极限的性别差异。相反,雄性潜水时间较短可能有助于它们维持生理机能、交配机会或领地防御。为了躲避捕食者,半水栖的Anolis蜥蜴可以潜入水下,并通过在头顶上回呼一个气泡来长时间停留在水下。在这项研究中,我们发现潜入水中躲避捕食者也是有代价的:浸入水中会降低蜥蜴的体温。体温降低会影响蜥蜴快速移动和保卫配偶或领地的能力,这表明雌雄蜥蜴的潜水行为可能存在差异。我们的研究结果证实,雄性蜥蜴在水下的时间确实比雌性蜥蜴少。我们测量了潜水时的耗氧量,数据表明潜水行为的性别差异与耗氧量无关。这项研究揭示了不同性别在反捕食行为和维持最佳体温之间的平衡,更广泛地有助于深入了解对环境挑战的适应性反应。
{"title":"Diving behavior in semi-aquatic Anolis lizards results in heat loss with sex-specific cooling tolerance","authors":"Alexandra M. Martin, Christopher K. Boccia, Lindsey Swierk","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03448-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03448-5","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Males and females often differ in use of antipredator behaviors, particularly when antipredator behavior comes at the cost of missed mating opportunities or territory defense. When using thermally suboptimal refugia, ectotherms are especially vulnerable to these costs, as their performance is linked to body temperature. To flee from predators, semi-aquatic <i>Anolis</i> lizards dive underwater for long periods and rebreathe from a bubble of air. We hypothesized that using aquatic refugia would result in body heat loss, that dive duration is influenced by sex, and that oxygen consumption when diving would help explain sex differences. We tested these hypotheses by measuring dive length and body temperatures in <i>A. aquaticus</i>, and by recording oxygen consumption and final oxygen partial pressure during controlled dives in several semi-aquatic <i>Anolis</i> species. Not only was there a significant thermal cost to diving, but <i>A. aquaticus</i> males and females appeared to tolerate different levels of this cost: males re-emerged from water more quickly and at higher body temperatures than did females. Body temperature decreased according to an exponential decay function, dropping up to 6 °C in 5 min. Oxygen consumption rates in semi-aquatic anoles were primarily explained by the expected allometric scaling relationship with mass and, therefore, are unlikely to lead to sex differences in physiological limits to dive times. Instead, shorter male dives may help them maintain physiological performance, mating opportunities or territory defense. Antipredator diving behavior is physiologically costly but undoubtedly beneficial to both sexes, highlighting the need for further study of sex-based antipredator optimization.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>To avoid predators, semi-aquatic <i>Anolis</i> lizards can dive underwater and remain there for an extended time by rebreathing a bubble of air over their heads. In this study, we reveal that diving to escape predators also comes with a cost: submersion in water reduces lizard body temperatures. Reduced body temperature can impair a lizard’s ability to move quickly and defend mates or territories, suggesting that there may be divergent diving behaviors in males and females. Our findings confirm that males do indeed spend less time underwater than females. We measured oxygen consumption during dives, and our data suggest that sex differences in diving behavior are unrelated to oxygen use. This study sheds light on the sex-specific balance of antipredator behaviors and the maintenance of optimal body temperatures, and more broadly contributes insight into adaptive responses to environmental challenges.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139954433","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lead pollution impacts the dynamics and resolution of contests between the native Mexican mojarra and invasive convict cichlid 铅污染影响墨西哥原生鲯鳅与外来入侵的定罪慈鲷之间的竞争动态和解决方法
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-21 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03444-9
Irma Inchaurregui, Hugo F. Olivares-Rubio, Elsah Arce, Luis M. Burciaga, M. Franco

Abstract

Both pollution and invasive species are threats for freshwater environments and their native ichthyofauna. The Mexican mojarra is the only cichlid fish species native to the Amacuzac River of central Mexico. Its habitat has been modified by the presence of invasive cichlid fishes, such as the convict cichlid, and by the presence of heavy metals such as lead. Since pollutants may affect invasive and native species differently, the combined effects of these two threats on native fish are not easily predictable and may be synergistic. We evaluated the effects of Pb exposure on interspecific contest dynamics and resolution between juvenile Mexican mojarras and convict cichlids. We performed ten interspecific contests between native and invasive cichlid at an environmentally relevant water concentration of Pb (38.2 µg L−1) in three phases: Pb absence, Pb exposure, and Pb depuration, as well as ten interspecific control contests without Pb. The Mexican mojarra consistently won the contests, regardless of Pb exposure, showing no differences in contest resolution between the Pb group and control group. The presence of Pb increased the number of aggressions in convict cichlids but reduced them in the Mexican mojarra, and contest duration decreased in the Pb exposure relative to the control group. However, these differences were not maintained during the Pb depuration period. Thus, we found effects of Pb on the agonistic interactions between the native Mexican mojarra and the invasive convict cichlid. Other invasive cichlid fishes and pollutants deserve attention in the context of the conservation of the Amacuzac River and its native ichthyofauna.

Significance statement

Native cichlid Mexican mojarra have been negatively affected by the introduction of the invasive convict cichlid and by the presence of heavy metals such as lead (Pb). Here, we evaluated the effects of Pb exposure on contest dynamics and outcome between Mexican mojarras and convict cichlids. Pb did modify the contest dynamics, augmenting the aggression in the Mexican mojarra and reducing fight duration. Our results show that invasive species may be favored in contaminated environments and that native species are more sensitive to pollution, further increasing their vulnerability to biological invasions.

摘要污染和入侵物种都是淡水环境及其本地鱼类的威胁。墨西哥蓑鲉是墨西哥中部阿马库扎克河唯一的原生慈鲷鱼种。由于入侵慈鲷(如罪犯慈鲷)和重金属(如铅)的存在,它的栖息地发生了改变。由于污染物对入侵物种和本地物种的影响可能不同,这两种威胁对本地鱼类的综合影响不易预测,而且可能会产生协同效应。我们评估了铅暴露对墨西哥鲯鳅幼鱼和厥鲤之间种间竞争动态和解决能力的影响。我们在环境相关的铅水浓度(38.2 µg L-1)下,分三个阶段在本地慈鲷和外来慈鲷之间进行了十次种间竞争:同时还进行了十次不含铅含量的种间对照竞赛。无论暴露于何种铅,墨西哥麋鹿在竞赛中始终获胜,铅暴露组与对照组在竞赛分辨率上没有差异。铅的存在增加了定罪慈鲷的攻击次数,但减少了墨西哥胭脂鱼的攻击次数,与对照组相比,暴露于铅的墨西哥胭脂鱼的竞赛持续时间缩短。然而,这些差异在铅消除期并没有得到维持。因此,我们发现铅对本地墨西哥鲯鳅和外来入侵的定罪慈鲷之间的激动互动有影响。在保护阿马库扎克河及其本地鱼类区系的背景下,其他入侵慈鲷鱼类和污染物值得关注。意义声明墨西哥原生慈鲷受到入侵厥慈鲷和铅(Pb)等重金属的负面影响。在此,我们评估了铅暴露对墨西哥鲯鳅和厥鳅之间的竞争动态和结果的影响。铅确实改变了竞争动态,增强了墨西哥驼鸟的攻击性并缩短了搏斗时间。我们的研究结果表明,入侵物种在受污染的环境中可能会受到青睐,而本地物种对污染更加敏感,从而进一步增加了它们遭受生物入侵的可能性。
{"title":"Lead pollution impacts the dynamics and resolution of contests between the native Mexican mojarra and invasive convict cichlid","authors":"Irma Inchaurregui, Hugo F. Olivares-Rubio, Elsah Arce, Luis M. Burciaga, M. Franco","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03444-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03444-9","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Both pollution and invasive species are threats for freshwater environments and their native ichthyofauna. The Mexican mojarra is the only cichlid fish species native to the Amacuzac River of central Mexico. Its habitat has been modified by the presence of invasive cichlid fishes, such as the convict cichlid, and by the presence of heavy metals such as lead. Since pollutants may affect invasive and native species differently, the combined effects of these two threats on native fish are not easily predictable and may be synergistic. We evaluated the effects of Pb exposure on interspecific contest dynamics and resolution between juvenile Mexican mojarras and convict cichlids. We performed ten interspecific contests between native and invasive cichlid at an environmentally relevant water concentration of Pb (38.2 µg L<sup>−1</sup>) in three phases: Pb absence, Pb exposure, and Pb depuration, as well as ten interspecific control contests without Pb. The Mexican mojarra consistently won the contests, regardless of Pb exposure, showing no differences in contest resolution between the Pb group and control group. The presence of Pb increased the number of aggressions in convict cichlids but reduced them in the Mexican mojarra, and contest duration decreased in the Pb exposure relative to the control group. However, these differences were not maintained during the Pb depuration period. Thus, we found effects of Pb on the agonistic interactions between the native Mexican mojarra and the invasive convict cichlid. Other invasive cichlid fishes and pollutants deserve attention in the context of the conservation of the Amacuzac River and its native ichthyofauna.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>Native cichlid Mexican mojarra have been negatively affected by the introduction of the invasive convict cichlid and by the presence of heavy metals such as lead (Pb). Here, we evaluated the effects of Pb exposure on contest dynamics and outcome between Mexican mojarras and convict cichlids. Pb did modify the contest dynamics, augmenting the aggression in the Mexican mojarra and reducing fight duration. Our results show that invasive species may be favored in contaminated environments and that native species are more sensitive to pollution, further increasing their vulnerability to biological invasions.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139922461","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1