首页 > 最新文献

Applied Animal Ethology最新文献

英文 中文
Washing lambs and confinement as aids to fostering 清洗羔羊和坐月子以帮助培育
Pub Date : 1983-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90146-3
G. Alexander, D. Stevens, L.R. Bradley

The effects on maternal behaviour, of washing lambs and of confining ewes with alien lambs for several days, were examined in groups of Merino ewes that had lambed approximately 1, 3, 9 or 27 days previously. In the 1-, 3- and 9-day groups, 25–43% of ewes refused to allow their own washed lambs to suck and 7–47% permitted a washed alien to suck, though most were able to distinguish their own washed lamb from 2 washed aliens in a multiple choice test. In the 27-day group, all ewes permitted only their own washed lamb to suck. Similar results were obtained in the 4 age groups with the same lambs when only the hind half was washed, indicating that odour from this region is responsible for releasing suckling behaviour.

When ewes were penned together with washed alien lambs for at least 2 days, 73–100% of lambs in the 1-, 3- and 9-day groups either gained weight progressively or were seen to suck freely from the foster mother, while only 33% of lambs in the 27-day group were similary accepted. With unwashed lambs, the acceptance rate was approximately half that of washed lambs. Labelling the alien lamb with wool from the tail region of the foster ewe's own lamb also tended to facilitate fostering, but not to any marked extent. Where the ewe's own lamb was also present in the flock after fostering, foster ewes tended to revert to suckling their own lamb at the expense of the foster lamb, especially in the 9- and 27-day groups.

The use of washing coupled with close confinement appears to be an effective method of fostering lambs on to ewes, especially within the first day or two after birth.

Fifty-five % of ewes in the 1- and 3-day groups failed to accept their own unwashed lamb after being separated from it for only 40–48 h, indicating that the ewe's memory for lamb odour is transient and/or that the odour changes with time.

对母性行为的影响,清洗羔羊和母羊与外来羔羊一起生活数天,在美利奴母羊组中进行了检查,这些母羊大约在1、3、9或27天前产羔。在第1、3和9天的组中,25-43%的母羊拒绝让自己洗过的羔羊吸吮,7-47%的母羊允许洗过的外星人吸吮,尽管大多数母羊能够在多项选择测试中区分出自己洗过的羔羊和2个洗过的外星人。在27天的小组中,所有母羊只允许自己洗过的羊羔吸吮。在4个年龄组中,同样的羔羊只洗后半部分,也得到了类似的结果,表明该区域的气味负责释放哺乳行为。当母羊与洗净的外来羔羊一起圈养至少2天时,第1、3和9天组中73-100%的羔羊体重逐渐增加,或者可以自由地吮吸养母的奶,而27天组中只有33%的羔羊被接受。对于未洗涤的羔羊,接受率大约是洗涤羔羊的一半。用母羊自己的羊的尾巴上的羊毛标记外来羔羊也有利于培育,但没有任何明显的程度。如果母羊自己的小羊在饲养后也出现在羊群中,寄养母羊倾向于以牺牲寄养小羊为代价恢复自己的小羊,特别是在9天和27天的组中。使用清洗加上封闭的禁闭似乎是一种有效的方法来培养羔羊母羊,特别是在出生后的头一两天。在第1天和第3天的组中,55%的母羊在与自己未洗过的羔羊分开40-48小时后就不能接受它,这表明母羊对羔羊气味的记忆是短暂的,或者气味会随着时间的推移而变化。
{"title":"Washing lambs and confinement as aids to fostering","authors":"G. Alexander,&nbsp;D. Stevens,&nbsp;L.R. Bradley","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90146-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90146-3","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The effects on maternal behaviour, of washing lambs and of confining ewes with alien lambs for several days, were examined in groups of Merino ewes that had lambed approximately 1, 3, 9 or 27 days previously. In the 1-, 3- and 9-day groups, 25–43% of ewes refused to allow their own washed lambs to suck and 7–47% permitted a washed alien to suck, though most were able to distinguish their own washed lamb from 2 washed aliens in a multiple choice test. In the 27-day group, all ewes permitted only their own washed lamb to suck. Similar results were obtained in the 4 age groups with the same lambs when only the hind half was washed, indicating that odour from this region is responsible for releasing suckling behaviour.</p><p>When ewes were penned together with washed alien lambs for at least 2 days, 73–100% of lambs in the 1-, 3- and 9-day groups either gained weight progressively or were seen to suck freely from the foster mother, while only 33% of lambs in the 27-day group were similary accepted. With unwashed lambs, the acceptance rate was approximately half that of washed lambs. Labelling the alien lamb with wool from the tail region of the foster ewe's own lamb also tended to facilitate fostering, but not to any marked extent. Where the ewe's own lamb was also present in the flock after fostering, foster ewes tended to revert to suckling their own lamb at the expense of the foster lamb, especially in the 9- and 27-day groups.</p><p>The use of washing coupled with close confinement appears to be an effective method of fostering lambs on to ewes, especially within the first day or two after birth.</p><p>Fifty-five % of ewes in the 1- and 3-day groups failed to accept their own unwashed lamb after being separated from it for only 40–48 h, indicating that the ewe's memory for lamb odour is transient and/or that the odour changes with time.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90146-3","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"137407236","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The enrichment of a bare environment for animals in confined conditions 在受限条件下为动物提供丰富的贫瘠环境
Pub Date : 1983-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90142-6
D.G.M. Wood-Gush , R.G. Beilharz

One out of each of 6 balanced paired groups of 8 weaner piglets, held in flat-deck cages for one week, was supplied with a trough containing sterilized earth. The behaviour of the animals in each group was observed by systematic scanning at 2-min intervals for 4 periods of 20 min on each of 3 days, after the piglets had been in the cages for 4 days. In all groups, the piglets with the trough made use of it and the amount of lying inactive was greater in all groups without the trough. In piglets weaned immediately into the experimental cages, the frequency of lying inactive was positively correlated with temperature. It is suggested that enrichment of the bare cage environment by provision of an earth trough resulted in an elaboration of a number of behaviours. This may be interpreted as an indication of reduced boredom.

6对平衡的断奶仔猪组中,每组8头仔猪中,每组1头仔猪在平板笼中饲养一周,饲喂含有灭菌土的槽。在仔猪入笼4天后,采用系统扫描法观察各组动物的行为,每隔2分钟进行4次扫描,每次扫描20分钟,每3天进行一次。在所有组中,有食槽的仔猪都利用了食槽,无食槽的仔猪躺卧不动量均大于无食槽组。在刚断奶进入实验笼的仔猪中,躺卧不动的频率与温度呈正相关。有人认为,通过提供土槽来丰富裸笼环境导致了许多行为的细化。这可能被解释为无聊程度降低的迹象。
{"title":"The enrichment of a bare environment for animals in confined conditions","authors":"D.G.M. Wood-Gush ,&nbsp;R.G. Beilharz","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90142-6","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90142-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>One out of each of 6 balanced paired groups of 8 weaner piglets, held in flat-deck cages for one week, was supplied with a trough containing sterilized earth. The behaviour of the animals in each group was observed by systematic scanning at 2-min intervals for 4 periods of 20 min on each of 3 days, after the piglets had been in the cages for 4 days. In all groups, the piglets with the trough made use of it and the amount of lying inactive was greater in all groups without the trough. In piglets weaned immediately into the experimental cages, the frequency of lying inactive was positively correlated with temperature. It is suggested that enrichment of the bare cage environment by provision of an earth trough resulted in an elaboration of a number of behaviours. This may be interpreted as an indication of reduced boredom.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90142-6","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83917958","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 110
The accuracy of several aids in detecting oestrus in dairy cattle 几种检测奶牛发情的辅助方法的准确性
Pub Date : 1983-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90141-4
W.J. Fulkerson , G.J. Sawyer , I. Crothers

The accuracy and efficiency of oestrus detection using behavioural observations only, or in combination with hormone-treated steers or tail paint, was assessed on a large commercial dairy herd of 120 cows. The oestrus detection rate was 50, 88 and 80%, respectively, with no significant difference in conception rate between methods following artificial insemination. Continual observation for 12 h during the day detected 83% of oestrous cows over the 3-week period.

Progesterone analysis of fore-milk confirmed oestrus in all cows detected except for 5 cows detected with steers and 4 with tail paint. As all except 2 of these animals conceived at that oestrus, the accuracy of the 3 methods was considered to be the same.

Oestrous activity in cows was high at morning milking, then rose again to reach a peak in early afternoon. Mean number of mounts/oestrous cow, observed during the 12 h of continual daily observation, was 14.6 +- 1.8.

The implication of these results on dairy herd management are discussed.

在120头大型商业奶牛群中,仅使用行为观察或结合使用激素处理的阉牛或尾漆来评估发情检测的准确性和效率。人工授精后的发情检出率分别为50%、88%和80%,两种方法受精率差异无统计学意义。白天连续观察12小时,3周内发情奶牛的发情率为83%。前乳黄体酮分析显示,除5头有阉牛和4头有尾漆的奶牛外,所有奶牛均有发情。除了2只动物在发情期怀孕外,其他所有动物都在发情期怀孕,这3种方法的准确性被认为是相同的。奶牛的发情活动在上午挤奶时高,然后再次上升,在下午早些时候达到高峰。在连续每日观察的12小时内,每头发情奶牛的平均坐骑数为14.6 +- 1.8。讨论了这些结果对奶牛群管理的启示。
{"title":"The accuracy of several aids in detecting oestrus in dairy cattle","authors":"W.J. Fulkerson ,&nbsp;G.J. Sawyer ,&nbsp;I. Crothers","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90141-4","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90141-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The accuracy and efficiency of oestrus detection using behavioural observations only, or in combination with hormone-treated steers or tail paint, was assessed on a large commercial dairy herd of 120 cows. The oestrus detection rate was 50, 88 and 80%, respectively, with no significant difference in conception rate between methods following artificial insemination. Continual observation for 12 h during the day detected 83% of oestrous cows over the 3-week period.</p><p>Progesterone analysis of fore-milk confirmed oestrus in all cows detected except for 5 cows detected with steers and 4 with tail paint. As all except 2 of these animals conceived at that oestrus, the accuracy of the 3 methods was considered to be the same.</p><p>Oestrous activity in cows was high at morning milking, then rose again to reach a peak in early afternoon. Mean number of mounts/oestrous cow, observed during the 12 h of continual daily observation, was 14.6 +- 1.8.</p><p>The implication of these results on dairy herd management are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90141-4","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88213703","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 18
The behaviour of dairy cows and their newborn calves in individual or group housing 奶牛及其新生小牛在个体或群体住宅中的行为
Pub Date : 1983-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90140-2
S.A. Edwards

Continuous observations during the first 6 h post partum were made on 38 heifers and 44 cows calving in individual pens, and 11 heifers and 40 cows calving in group housing. Only 22% of group-housed cows isolated themselves at calving. Many interactions involving alien cows and calves were observed in a group-calving situation and resulted in 4 cases of desertion of a newborn calf. Such interactions most commonly involved newborn calves and cows close to parturition. Calves born in group housing moved away from their dam more and directed a high proportion of teat-seeking towards alien cows. 33% of calves cross-suckled during the first 6 h of life.

在产后的前6小时,对38头母牛和44头母牛在单独的猪圈产仔,以及11头母牛和40头母牛在群舍产仔进行了连续观察。只有22%的群居奶牛在产犊时孤立自己。在集体产犊的情况下,观察到许多涉及外来奶牛和小牛的互动,导致4例遗弃新生小牛。这种互动最常见的是新生牛犊和即将分娩的母牛。在群居地出生的小牛更多地离开了它们的水坝,并将很大比例的寻找奶头的行为指向了外来的母牛。33%的小牛在出生后的前6小时进行交叉哺乳。
{"title":"The behaviour of dairy cows and their newborn calves in individual or group housing","authors":"S.A. Edwards","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90140-2","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90140-2","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Continuous observations during the first 6 h post partum were made on 38 heifers and 44 cows calving in individual pens, and 11 heifers and 40 cows calving in group housing. Only 22% of group-housed cows isolated themselves at calving. Many interactions involving alien cows and calves were observed in a group-calving situation and resulted in 4 cases of desertion of a newborn calf. Such interactions most commonly involved newborn calves and cows close to parturition. Calves born in group housing moved away from their dam more and directed a high proportion of teat-seeking towards alien cows. 33% of calves cross-suckled during the first 6 h of life.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90140-2","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73653961","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 63
Environmental Aspects of Housing for Animal Production 动物生产住房的环境因素
Pub Date : 1983-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90149-9
R. Kilgour
{"title":"Environmental Aspects of Housing for Animal Production","authors":"R. Kilgour","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90149-9","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90149-9","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90149-9","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78226212","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Equine behaviour: I. A review of the literature on social and dam—Foal behaviour 马的行为:1 .社会和马驹行为的文献综述
Pub Date : 1983-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90138-4
K. Carson, D. Wood‐Gush
{"title":"Equine behaviour: I. A review of the literature on social and dam—Foal behaviour","authors":"K. Carson, D. Wood‐Gush","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90138-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90138-4","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77126304","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 49
Equine behaviour: I. A review of the literature on social and dam—Foal behaviour 马的行为:1 .社会和马驹行为的文献综述
Pub Date : 1983-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90138-4
Katherine Carson, D.G.M. Wood-Gush

In most cases, the social organisation of each of the seven species of Equidae existing today outside captivity is either territorial or non-territorial. The striking differences found between these two types of organisation in the social grouping and bonds, mating behaviour, leadership and dominance hierarchies of the animals are examined. It is thought that the non-territorial species show a less primitive type of organisation than the territorial animals.

Infant Equidae are precocious animals and are able to follow their dams soon after birth. They stay close by their dams and travel with the herd from an early age and are therefore classified as “followers”, in contrast to the species which have a period of hiding after birth. Dams recognise their foals immediately after birth, whereas it takes 2 or 3 days for a foal to form an attachment to its dam. Being in close proximity to their dams, foals are able to nurse frequently and, unless artificially weaned, a foal will nurse until its dam foals again.

Foals start to graze during their first week and as they grow older they spend more time grazing and less time nursing and resting. It is normal for foals to be corprophagic until one month old, and this provides them with bacteria essential for the digestion of fibre. Play behaviour is solitary in very young foals, but after 4 weeks of age, foals play together, with male foals playing more than females and showing more aggressive, fighting movements in play.

在大多数情况下,今天在圈养环境之外存在的七种Equidae物种中的每一种的社会组织都是领土或非领土的。这两种类型的组织在社会分组和纽带、交配行为、动物的领导和统治等级等方面发现了显著的差异。人们认为,与有领土的动物相比,无领土的物种表现出较不原始的组织类型。幼马科动物是早熟的动物,出生后不久就能跟随它们的水坝。它们从小就待在自己的水坝附近,与鹿群一起旅行,因此被归类为“追随者”,与出生后有一段躲藏期的物种形成鲜明对比。小马驹在出生后就会被母马认出来,而小马驹则需要2到3天的时间才能与母马驹形成依恋关系。由于离母马很近,小马驹可以经常哺乳,除非人工断奶,否则小马驹会一直哺乳到母马驹再次出生。马驹在第一周开始吃草,随着年龄的增长,它们花更多的时间吃草,更少的时间护理和休息。马驹在一个月大之前是正常的食腐动物,这为它们提供了消化纤维所必需的细菌。非常小的马驹是单独玩耍的,但4周后,马驹会一起玩耍,公马驹比母马驹玩得更多,在玩耍中表现出更强的攻击性和打斗动作。
{"title":"Equine behaviour: I. A review of the literature on social and dam—Foal behaviour","authors":"Katherine Carson,&nbsp;D.G.M. Wood-Gush","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90138-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90138-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In most cases, the social organisation of each of the seven species of <em>Equidae</em> existing today outside captivity is either territorial or non-territorial. The striking differences found between these two types of organisation in the social grouping and bonds, mating behaviour, leadership and dominance hierarchies of the animals are examined. It is thought that the non-territorial species show a less primitive type of organisation than the territorial animals.</p><p>Infant <em>Equidae</em> are precocious animals and are able to follow their dams soon after birth. They stay close by their dams and travel with the herd from an early age and are therefore classified as “followers”, in contrast to the species which have a period of hiding after birth. Dams recognise their foals immediately after birth, whereas it takes 2 or 3 days for a foal to form an attachment to its dam. Being in close proximity to their dams, foals are able to nurse frequently and, unless artificially weaned, a foal will nurse until its dam foals again.</p><p>Foals start to graze during their first week and as they grow older they spend more time grazing and less time nursing and resting. It is normal for foals to be corprophagic until one month old, and this provides them with bacteria essential for the digestion of fibre. Play behaviour is solitary in very young foals, but after 4 weeks of age, foals play together, with male foals playing more than females and showing more aggressive, fighting movements in play.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90138-4","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"90128456","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 49
Fearful behavior by caged hens of two genetic stocks 两种基因组合的母鸡的恐惧行为
Pub Date : 1983-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90147-5
J.V. Craig, T.P. Craig , A.D. Dayton

Two White Leghorn stocks (Y1 and Y2) and sire families within each were compared for nervousness scores and for latencies to feed and to return to “normal” activity after being subjected to fear-inducing stimuli, involving either a metronome or the cage being struck by a human observer. Information on feather damage and loss, age at sexual maturity, and part-year egg-mass production was also collected. Strain differences were clearly evident for nervousness score, duration of fearful behavior, and feather loss. The strain having more nervous and fearful hens also suffered greater feather damage and loss. Sire-family differences were erratically present for the same traits.

Correlation coefficients calculated within strains for latency data obtained by 2 observers, working independently, indicated significant repeatability in 5 of 8 comparisons (r values from 0.50 to 0.91). Correlations of latencies within strains obtained by the use of 2 kinds of fear-inducing stimuli also indicated significant repeatability in 4 of 8 comparisons (r values from 0.35 to 0.88). Further intra-strain correlation analyses indicated that fearful behavior, as measured by latencies, tended to be associated with number of hens per cage, but was relatively independent of neighbors' behavior. Greater nervous and fearful behavior of caged groups tended to be significantly associated with greater feather loss and non-significantly associated with earlier sexual maturity and lower egg-mass production of 30 – 40-week-old pullets.

研究人员比较了两只白莱格霍恩猪群(Y1和Y2)和每只猪的6个家庭的紧张程度,以及在受到引起恐惧的刺激后进食和恢复“正常”活动的延迟时间,这些刺激包括节拍器或人类观察者敲击笼子。还收集了羽毛损伤和损失、性成熟年龄和半年产蛋量的信息。紧张程度的差异在紧张程度、恐惧行为的持续时间和羽毛脱落上是明显的。有更多紧张和恐惧母鸡的品种也遭受了更大的羽毛损伤和损失。在相同的性状上,家族间的差异是不规则的。由2名独立工作的观测者获得的潜伏期数据在应变内计算的相关系数表明,8个比较中有5个具有显著的重复性(r值为0.50至0.91)。使用两种诱发恐惧刺激获得的应变内潜伏期的相关性在8个比较中有4个也显示出显著的重复性(r值从0.35到0.88)。进一步的应变相关性分析表明,恐惧行为(通过潜伏期测量)倾向于与每个笼子的母鸡数量有关,但相对独立于邻居的行为。笼中鸡的紧张和恐惧行为与鸡羽损失显著相关,而与30 - 40周龄雏鸡性成熟提前和产蛋量降低无显著相关。
{"title":"Fearful behavior by caged hens of two genetic stocks","authors":"J.V. Craig,&nbsp;T.P. Craig ,&nbsp;A.D. Dayton","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90147-5","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90147-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Two White Leghorn stocks (Y<sub>1</sub> and Y<sub>2</sub>) and sire families within each were compared for nervousness scores and for latencies to feed and to return to “normal” activity after being subjected to fear-inducing stimuli, involving either a metronome or the cage being struck by a human observer. Information on feather damage and loss, age at sexual maturity, and part-year egg-mass production was also collected. Strain differences were clearly evident for nervousness score, duration of fearful behavior, and feather loss. The strain having more nervous and fearful hens also suffered greater feather damage and loss. Sire-family differences were erratically present for the same traits.</p><p>Correlation coefficients calculated within strains for latency data obtained by 2 observers, working independently, indicated significant repeatability in 5 of 8 comparisons (<em>r</em> values from 0.50 to 0.91). Correlations of latencies within strains obtained by the use of 2 kinds of fear-inducing stimuli also indicated significant repeatability in 4 of 8 comparisons (<em>r</em> values from 0.35 to 0.88). Further intra-strain correlation analyses indicated that fearful behavior, as measured by latencies, tended to be associated with number of hens per cage, but was relatively independent of neighbors' behavior. Greater nervous and fearful behavior of caged groups tended to be significantly associated with greater feather loss and non-significantly associated with earlier sexual maturity and lower egg-mass production of 30 – 40-week-old pullets.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90147-5","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89324961","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 90
A note on canine aggression towards veterinarians 关于犬类对兽医的攻击
Pub Date : 1983-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90118-9
Kenneth L. Marcella

Medical records at a small-animal practice in Vermont were surveyed and animals that displayed unprovoked and consistently aggressive behavior towards veterinarians were grouped by breed and ranked. The 6 dog breeds exhibiting the highest frequency of aggression towards veterinarians and hospital staff members were the Schnauzer, Old English Sheepdog, Beagle, German Shepherd, Doberman Pinscher and English Springer Spaniel. These breeds represented a mixture of traditionally “aggressive” and “non-aggressive” breeds.

研究人员对佛蒙特州一家小动物诊所的医疗记录进行了调查,并对那些对兽医表现出无端攻击行为的动物按品种进行了分组和排名。对兽医和医院工作人员最具攻击性的6种狗分别是雪纳瑞、古英国牧羊犬、比格犬、德国牧羊犬、杜宾犬和英国史宾格犬。这些品种代表了传统上“侵略性”和“非侵略性”品种的混合物。
{"title":"A note on canine aggression towards veterinarians","authors":"Kenneth L. Marcella","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90118-9","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90118-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Medical records at a small-animal practice in Vermont were surveyed and animals that displayed unprovoked and consistently aggressive behavior towards veterinarians were grouped by breed and ranked. The 6 dog breeds exhibiting the highest frequency of aggression towards veterinarians and hospital staff members were the Schnauzer, Old English Sheepdog, Beagle, German Shepherd, Doberman Pinscher and English Springer Spaniel. These breeds represented a mixture of traditionally “aggressive” and “non-aggressive” breeds.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90118-9","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88436530","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Early social isolation of the domestic cat: Responses during mechanical toy testing 家猫的早期社会隔离:机械玩具测试中的反应
Pub Date : 1983-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90115-3
Gary W. Guyot, Henry A. Cross, Thomas L. Bennett

Four male and four female kittens were reared in each of the following rearing conditions: (1) mother and a littermate; (2) alone with their mother; (3) brooder and littermate; (4) alone with a brooder. Each of the 32 kittens were separated briefly from their rearing condition once a week from 2–5 weeks of age. They were placed alone in an open field with a mechanical toy and a brooder replicate. The brooder-reared kittens spent more time with the toy than the mother-reared kittens. In fact, they spent more time with the mechanical toy than they did with the brooder. The brooder-reared kittens entered more squares than the mother-reared infants, but vocalized less than the mother-reared kittens. It was concluded that while all kittens appear to form social attachments, brooder-reared kittens do not seem to form substitute social attachments to a brooder.

分别在以下饲养条件下饲养4只公猫和4只母猫:(1)母猫和一窝仔猫;(二)与母亲单独生活的;(三)育雏和窝伴;(4)单独与育雏机。32只小猫从2-5周龄开始,每周短暂分离一次。他们被单独放置在一个开放的田野里,有一个机械玩具和一个复制的种鸡。母猫饲养的小猫比母猫饲养的小猫玩玩具的时间更长。事实上,它们花在机械玩具上的时间比花在孵蛋器上的时间要多。母猫饲养的幼猫比母猫饲养的幼猫进入更多的方格,但发出的声音比母猫饲养的幼猫少。结论是,虽然所有的小猫似乎都形成了社会依恋,但母猫饲养的小猫似乎并没有形成对母猫的替代社会依恋。
{"title":"Early social isolation of the domestic cat: Responses during mechanical toy testing","authors":"Gary W. Guyot,&nbsp;Henry A. Cross,&nbsp;Thomas L. Bennett","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90115-3","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90115-3","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Four male and four female kittens were reared in each of the following rearing conditions: (1) mother and a littermate; (2) alone with their mother; (3) brooder and littermate; (4) alone with a brooder. Each of the 32 kittens were separated briefly from their rearing condition once a week from 2–5 weeks of age. They were placed alone in an open field with a mechanical toy and a brooder replicate. The brooder-reared kittens spent more time with the toy than the mother-reared kittens. In fact, they spent more time with the mechanical toy than they did with the brooder. The brooder-reared kittens entered more squares than the mother-reared infants, but vocalized less than the mother-reared kittens. It was concluded that while all kittens appear to form social attachments, brooder-reared kittens do not seem to form substitute social attachments to a brooder.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90115-3","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"82628863","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
期刊
Applied Animal Ethology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1